首页 > 最新文献

Affective science最新文献

英文 中文
A Critique of Automated Approaches to Code Facial Expressions: What Do Researchers Need to Know? 对面部表情自动编码方法的批评:研究人员需要知道什么?
Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2023-07-10 DOI: 10.1007/s42761-023-00195-0
Marie P. Cross, Amanda M. Acevedo, John F. Hunter

Facial expression recognition software is becoming more commonly used by affective scientists to measure facial expressions. Although the use of this software has exciting implications, there are persistent and concerning issues regarding the validity and reliability of these programs. In this paper, we highlight three of these issues: biases of the programs against certain skin colors and genders; the common inability of these programs to capture facial expressions made in non-idealized conditions (e.g., “in the wild”); and programs being forced to adopt the underlying assumptions of the specific theory of emotion on which each software is based. We then discuss three directions for the future of affective science in the area of automated facial coding. First, researchers need to be cognizant of exactly how and on which data sets the machine learning algorithms underlying these programs are being trained. In addition, there are several ethical considerations, such as privacy and data storage, surrounding the use of facial expression recognition programs. Finally, researchers should consider collecting additional emotion data, such as body language, and combine these data with facial expression data in order to achieve a more comprehensive picture of complex human emotions. Facial expression recognition programs are an excellent method of collecting facial expression data, but affective scientists should ensure that they recognize the limitations and ethical implications of these programs.

情感科学家越来越普遍地使用面部表情识别软件来测量面部表情。尽管该软件的使用具有令人兴奋的意义,但在这些程序的有效性和可靠性方面仍存在持续和令人担忧的问题。在本文中,我们强调了其中的三个问题:节目对某些肤色和性别的偏见;这些程序通常无法捕捉在非理想化条件下(例如“在野外”)的面部表情;程序被迫采用每个软件所基于的特定情感理论的基本假设。然后,我们讨论了情感科学在自动面部编码领域的三个未来方向。首先,研究人员需要确切地了解这些程序背后的机器学习算法是如何以及在哪些数据集上进行训练的。此外,面部表情识别程序的使用还有一些伦理考虑,如隐私和数据存储。最后,研究人员应该考虑收集额外的情绪数据,如肢体语言,并将这些数据与面部表情数据相结合,以便更全面地了解复杂的人类情绪。面部表情识别程序是收集面部表情数据的一种很好的方法,但情感科学家应该确保他们认识到这些程序的局限性和道德含义。
{"title":"A Critique of Automated Approaches to Code Facial Expressions: What Do Researchers Need to Know?","authors":"Marie P. Cross,&nbsp;Amanda M. Acevedo,&nbsp;John F. Hunter","doi":"10.1007/s42761-023-00195-0","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s42761-023-00195-0","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Facial expression recognition software is becoming more commonly used by affective scientists to measure facial expressions. Although the use of this software has exciting implications, there are persistent and concerning issues regarding the validity and reliability of these programs. In this paper, we highlight three of these issues: biases of the programs against certain skin colors and genders; the common inability of these programs to capture facial expressions made in non-idealized conditions (e.g., “in the wild”); and programs being forced to adopt the underlying assumptions of the specific theory of emotion on which each software is based. We then discuss three directions for the future of affective science in the area of automated facial coding. First, researchers need to be cognizant of exactly how and on which data sets the machine learning algorithms underlying these programs are being trained. In addition, there are several ethical considerations, such as privacy and data storage, surrounding the use of facial expression recognition programs. Finally, researchers should consider collecting additional emotion data, such as body language, and combine these data with facial expression data in order to achieve a more comprehensive picture of complex human emotions. Facial expression recognition programs are an excellent method of collecting facial expression data, but affective scientists should ensure that they recognize the limitations and ethical implications of these programs.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":72119,"journal":{"name":"Affective science","volume":"4 3","pages":"500 - 505"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-07-10","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://link.springer.com/content/pdf/10.1007/s42761-023-00195-0.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41155752","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Framing Subjective Emotion Reports as Dynamic Affective Decisions 将主观情绪报告视为动态情感决策。
Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2023-07-10 DOI: 10.1007/s42761-023-00197-y
Yi Yang Teoh, William A. Cunningham, Cendri A. Hutcherson

Self-reports remain affective science’s only direct measure of subjective affective experiences. Yet, little research has sought to understand the psychological process that transforms subjective experience into self-reports. Here, we propose that by framing these self-reports as dynamic affective decisions, affective scientists may leverage the computational tools of decision-making research, sequential sampling models specifically, to better disentangle affective experience from the noisy decision processes that constitute self-report. We further outline how such an approach could help affective scientists better probe the specific mechanisms that underlie important moderators of affective experience (e.g., contextual differences, individual differences, and emotion regulation) and discuss how adopting this decision-making framework could generate insight into affective processes more broadly and facilitate reciprocal collaborations between affective and decision scientists towards a more comprehensive and integrative psychological science.

自我报告仍然是情感科学对主观情感体验的唯一直接衡量标准。然而,很少有研究试图了解将主观体验转化为自我报告的心理过程。在这里,我们提出,通过将这些自我报告定义为动态情感决策,情感科学家可以利用决策研究的计算工具,特别是顺序抽样模型,更好地将情感体验与构成自我报告的嘈杂决策过程区分开来。我们进一步概述了这种方法如何帮助情感科学家更好地探索情感体验的重要调节因素(例如,情境差异、个体差异和情绪调节)的具体机制,并讨论了采用这种决策框架如何更广泛地洞察情感过程,并促进互动情感科学家和决策科学家之间的合作,致力于更全面、更综合的心理科学。
{"title":"Framing Subjective Emotion Reports as Dynamic Affective Decisions","authors":"Yi Yang Teoh,&nbsp;William A. Cunningham,&nbsp;Cendri A. Hutcherson","doi":"10.1007/s42761-023-00197-y","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s42761-023-00197-y","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Self-reports remain affective science’s only direct measure of subjective affective experiences. Yet, little research has sought to understand the psychological process that transforms subjective experience into self-reports. Here, we propose that by framing these self-reports as dynamic affective decisions, affective scientists may leverage the computational tools of decision-making research, sequential sampling models specifically, to better disentangle affective experience from the noisy decision processes that constitute self-report. We further outline how such an approach could help affective scientists better probe the specific mechanisms that underlie important moderators of affective experience (e.g., contextual differences, individual differences, and emotion regulation) and discuss how adopting this decision-making framework could generate insight into affective processes more broadly and facilitate reciprocal collaborations between affective and decision scientists towards a more comprehensive and integrative psychological science.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":72119,"journal":{"name":"Affective science","volume":"4 3","pages":"522 - 528"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-07-10","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://link.springer.com/content/pdf/10.1007/s42761-023-00197-y.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41158961","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
Are Self-transcendent Emotions One Big Family? An Empirical Taxonomy of Positive Self-transcendent Emotion Labels 自我超越情感是一个大家庭吗?积极的自我超越情绪标签的经验分类法
IF 2.1 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2023-06-19 DOI: 10.1007/s42761-023-00194-1
Angela Gaia F. Abatista, Florian Cova

In the past decade, there has been a growing amount of research on so-called self-transcendent emotions, mainly in the domain of positive emotions. However, most candidate self-transcendent emotions (e.g., Awe, Gratitude, Being Moved, Wonder) have been studied in isolation, leaving the commonalities and the differences of their phenomenology unknown. In the present paper, we sought to identify the phenomenological nature of main families of self-transcendent emotions. We drew on two large datasets (N1 = 3,113; N2 = 1,443) in which participants had to recall an emotional episode or to watch emotional videos and had to report their emotions through a list of 40 emotion labels. Participants were also presented with a large list of items probing their cognitive appraisals, bodily feelings, and action tendencies. Using a principal component analysis, we identified three main dimensions of positive emotions: hedonic, social, and epistemic states. Candidate self-transcendent emotions were distributed across two dimensions, suggesting that at least two main different families of self-transcendent emotions should be distinguished. Our results also allowed us to identify self-reported cognitive appraisals, bodily feelings, and action tendencies characteristic of each family.

在过去十年中,关于所谓自我超越情绪的研究日益增多,主要集中在积极情绪领域。然而,大多数候选的自我超越情绪(如敬畏、感激、感动、惊叹)都是孤立研究的,它们在现象学上的共性和差异不得而知。在本文中,我们试图确定自我超越情绪主要家族的现象学性质。我们利用了两个大型数据集(N1 = 3,113; N2 = 1,443),在这些数据集中,参与者必须回忆一个情绪事件或观看情绪视频,并通过一个包含 40 个情绪标签的列表来报告他们的情绪。此外,我们还向受试者提供了一大串项目,以探究他们的认知评价、身体感受和行动倾向。通过主成分分析,我们确定了积极情绪的三个主要维度:享乐状态、社会状态和认识状态。候选的自我超越情绪分布在两个维度上,这表明至少应该区分两大不同的自我超越情绪家族。我们的研究结果还让我们确定了自我报告的认知评价、身体感受和行动倾向,这些都是每个家族的特征。
{"title":"Are Self-transcendent Emotions One Big Family? An Empirical Taxonomy of Positive Self-transcendent Emotion Labels","authors":"Angela Gaia F. Abatista,&nbsp;Florian Cova","doi":"10.1007/s42761-023-00194-1","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s42761-023-00194-1","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>In the past decade, there has been a growing amount of research on so-called self-transcendent emotions, mainly in the domain of positive emotions. However, most candidate self-transcendent emotions (e.g., Awe, Gratitude, Being Moved, Wonder) have been studied in isolation, leaving the commonalities and the differences of their phenomenology unknown. In the present paper, we sought to identify the phenomenological nature of main families of self-transcendent emotions. We drew on two large datasets (<i>N</i><sub>1</sub> = 3,113; <i>N</i><sub>2</sub> = 1,443) in which participants had to recall an emotional episode or to watch emotional videos and had to report their emotions through a list of 40 emotion labels. Participants were also presented with a large list of items probing their cognitive appraisals, bodily feelings, and action tendencies. Using a principal component analysis, we identified three main dimensions of positive emotions: hedonic, social, and epistemic states. Candidate self-transcendent emotions were distributed across two dimensions, suggesting that at least two main different families of self-transcendent emotions should be distinguished. Our results also allowed us to identify self-reported cognitive appraisals, bodily feelings, and action tendencies characteristic of each family.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":72119,"journal":{"name":"Affective science","volume":"4 4","pages":"731 - 743"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2023-06-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://link.springer.com/content/pdf/10.1007/s42761-023-00194-1.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"117222400","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The Role of Hedonic Goal Pursuit in Self-Control and Self-Regulation: Is Pleasure the Problem or Part of the Solution? 享乐目标追求在自我控制和自我调节中的作用:快乐是问题还是解决方案的一部分?
Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2023-06-12 DOI: 10.1007/s42761-023-00193-2
Daniela Becker, Katharina Bernecker

This paper examines the role of hedonic goal pursuit in self-control and self-regulation. We argue that not all pursuit of immediate pleasure is problematic and that successful hedonic goal pursuit can be beneficial for long-term goal pursuit and for achieving positive self-regulatory outcomes, such as health and well-being. The following two key questions for future research are discussed: How can people’s positive affective experiences during hedonic goal pursuit be enhanced, and how exactly do those affective experiences contribute to self-regulatory outcomes? We also call for an intercultural perspective linking hedonic goal pursuit to self-regulatory outcomes at different levels. We suggest that understanding the cognitive, motivational, and affective mechanisms at play can help individuals reap the benefits of successful hedonic goal pursuit. Considering those potential benefits, hedonic goal pursuit should be studied more systematically. To achieve this, we argue for a stronger integration of affective science and self-control research.

本文考察了享乐目标追求在自我控制和自我调节中的作用。我们认为,并非所有追求即时快乐的行为都是有问题的,成功的享乐目标追求有利于长期目标追求和实现积极的自我调节结果,如健康和幸福。下面讨论了未来研究的两个关键问题:如何增强人们在追求享乐目标过程中的积极情感体验,以及这些情感体验究竟如何促进自我调节结果?我们还呼吁从跨文化的角度将享乐目标追求与不同层面的自我调节结果联系起来。我们认为,了解游戏中的认知、动机和情感机制可以帮助个人从成功的享乐目标追求中获益。考虑到这些潜在的好处,享乐目标追求应该被更系统地研究。为了实现这一点,我们主张将情感科学和自我控制研究更紧密地结合起来。
{"title":"The Role of Hedonic Goal Pursuit in Self-Control and Self-Regulation: Is Pleasure the Problem or Part of the Solution?","authors":"Daniela Becker,&nbsp;Katharina Bernecker","doi":"10.1007/s42761-023-00193-2","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s42761-023-00193-2","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>This paper examines the role of hedonic goal pursuit in self-control and self-regulation. We argue that not all pursuit of immediate pleasure is problematic and that successful hedonic goal pursuit can be beneficial for long-term goal pursuit and for achieving positive self-regulatory outcomes, such as health and well-being. The following two key questions for future research are discussed: How can people’s positive affective experiences during hedonic goal pursuit be enhanced, and how exactly do those affective experiences contribute to self-regulatory outcomes? We also call for an intercultural perspective linking hedonic goal pursuit to self-regulatory outcomes at different levels. We suggest that understanding the cognitive, motivational, and affective mechanisms at play can help individuals reap the benefits of successful hedonic goal pursuit. Considering those potential benefits, hedonic goal pursuit should be studied more systematically. To achieve this, we argue for a stronger integration of affective science and self-control research.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":72119,"journal":{"name":"Affective science","volume":"4 3","pages":"470 - 474"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-06-12","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://link.springer.com/content/pdf/10.1007/s42761-023-00193-2.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41174868","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 3
Category Flexibility in Emotion Learning 情感学习中的类别灵活性
IF 2.1 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2023-06-09 DOI: 10.1007/s42761-023-00192-3
Rista C. Plate, Kristina Woodard, Seth D. Pollak

Learners flexibly update category boundaries to adjust to the range of experiences they encounter. However, little is known about whether the degree of flexibility is consistent across domains. We examined whether categorization of social input, specifically emotions, is afforded more flexibility as compared to other biological input. To address this question, children (6–12 years; 32 female, 37 male; 7 Hispanic or Latino, 62 not Hispanic or Latino; 8 Black or African American, 14 multiracial, 46 White, 1 selected “other”) categorized faces morphed from calm to upset and animals morphed from a horse to a cow across task phases that differed in the distribution of stimuli presented. Learners flexibly adjusted both emotion and animal category boundaries according to distributional information, yet children showed more flexibility when updating their category boundaries for emotions. These results provide support for the idea that children—who must adjust to the vast and varied emotional signals of their social partners—respond to social signals dynamically in order to make predictions about the internal states and future behaviors of others.

学习者会灵活地更新类别界限,以适应他们所遇到的各种经验。然而,人们对不同领域的灵活性是否一致却知之甚少。我们研究了与其他生物输入相比,社会输入(特别是情绪)的分类是否具有更大的灵活性。为了解决这个问题,儿童(6-12 岁;32 名女性,37 名男性;7 名西班牙裔或拉丁裔,62 名非西班牙裔或拉丁裔;8 名黑人或非裔美国人,14 名多种族,46 名白人,1 名选择了 "其他")在不同的任务阶段对从平静到不安的人脸和从马到牛的动物进行了分类,这些任务阶段的刺激分布各不相同。学习者根据刺激物的分布信息灵活地调整了情绪和动物的类别界限,然而儿童在更新情绪类别界限时表现出了更大的灵活性。这些结果为以下观点提供了支持,即儿童必须适应其社会伙伴的大量不同的情绪信号,动态地对社会信号做出反应,以便对他人的内部状态和未来行为做出预测。
{"title":"Category Flexibility in Emotion Learning","authors":"Rista C. Plate,&nbsp;Kristina Woodard,&nbsp;Seth D. Pollak","doi":"10.1007/s42761-023-00192-3","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s42761-023-00192-3","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Learners flexibly update category boundaries to adjust to the range of experiences they encounter. However, little is known about whether the degree of flexibility is consistent across domains. We examined whether categorization of social input, specifically emotions, is afforded more flexibility as compared to other biological input. To address this question, children (6–12 years; 32 female, 37 male; 7 Hispanic or Latino, 62 not Hispanic or Latino; 8 Black or African American, 14 multiracial, 46 White, 1 selected “other”) categorized faces morphed from calm to upset and animals morphed from a horse to a cow across task phases that differed in the distribution of stimuli presented. Learners flexibly adjusted both emotion and animal category boundaries according to distributional information, yet children showed more flexibility when updating their category boundaries for emotions. These results provide support for the idea that children—who must adjust to the vast and varied emotional signals of their social partners—respond to social signals dynamically in order to make predictions about the internal states and future behaviors of others.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":72119,"journal":{"name":"Affective science","volume":"4 4","pages":"722 - 730"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2023-06-09","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"129749252","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Examining the Role of Emotion Differentiation on Emotion and Cardiovascular Physiological Activity During Acute Stress 研究情绪分化对急性应激期间情绪和心血管生理活动的作用。
Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2023-05-27 DOI: 10.1007/s42761-023-00189-y
Adrienne S. Bonar, Jennifer K. MacCormack, Mallory J. Feldman, Kristen A. Lindquist

Emotion differentiation (ED) — the tendency to experience one’s emotions with specificity — is a well-established predictor of adaptive responses to daily life stress. Yet, there is little research testing the role of ED in self-reported and physiological responses to an acute stressor. In the current study, we investigate the effects of negative emotion differentiation (NED) and positive emotion differentiation (PED) on participants’ self-reported emotions and cardiac-mediated sympathetic nervous system reactivity (i.e., pre-ejection period) in response to a stressful task. Healthy young adults enrolled in a two-session study. At an initial session, participants completed a modified experience sampling procedure (i.e., the Day Reconstruction Method). At session 2, 195 completed the Trier Social Stress Test while cardiac impedance was acquired throughout. Linear regressions demonstrated that higher NED, but not PED, was associated with experiencing less intense self-reported negative, high arousal emotions (e.g., irritated, panicky) during the stressor (β =  − .15, p < .05) although people with higher NED also exhibited greater sympathetic reactivity (β = .16, p < .05). In exploratory analyses, we tested whether the effect of NED on self-reported stress was mediated by the tendency to make internally focus (or self-focused) attributions about performance on the task but did not find a significant indirect effect (p = .085). These results both complement prior work and provide a more complex picture of the role of NED in adaptive responses to stressful life events, suggesting that people with higher NED may experience their emotions as more manageable regardless of their level of physiological arousal.

情绪分化(ED)是一种特定体验情绪的倾向,是对日常生活压力的适应性反应的一个公认的预测指标。然而,很少有研究测试ED在对急性应激源的自我报告和生理反应中的作用。在目前的研究中,我们研究了消极情绪分化(NED)和积极情绪分化(PED)对参与者自我报告的情绪和心脏介导的交感神经系统对压力任务的反应(即射血前期)的影响。健康的年轻人参加了一项为期两次的研究。在最初的一次会议上,参与者完成了一个修改后的经验抽样程序(即日重建法)。在第2阶段,195人完成了Trier社会压力测试,同时全程采集了心脏阻抗。线性回归表明,在压力源(β =  - .15,p β = .16,p p = .085)。这些结果既补充了先前的工作,又提供了一幅更复杂的NED在对压力生活事件的适应性反应中的作用图,表明无论生理唤醒水平如何,NED较高的人可能会觉得自己的情绪更容易控制。补充信息:在线版本包含补充材料,请访问10.1007/s42761-023-00189-y。
{"title":"Examining the Role of Emotion Differentiation on Emotion and Cardiovascular Physiological Activity During Acute Stress","authors":"Adrienne S. Bonar,&nbsp;Jennifer K. MacCormack,&nbsp;Mallory J. Feldman,&nbsp;Kristen A. Lindquist","doi":"10.1007/s42761-023-00189-y","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s42761-023-00189-y","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Emotion differentiation (ED) — the tendency to experience one’s emotions with specificity — is a well-established predictor of adaptive responses to daily life stress. Yet, there is little research testing the role of ED in self-reported and physiological responses to an acute stressor. In the current study, we investigate the effects of negative emotion differentiation (NED) and positive emotion differentiation (PED) on participants’ self-reported emotions and cardiac-mediated sympathetic nervous system reactivity (i.e., pre-ejection period) in response to a stressful task. Healthy young adults enrolled in a two-session study. At an initial session, participants completed a modified experience sampling procedure (i.e., the Day Reconstruction Method). At session 2, 195 completed the Trier Social Stress Test while cardiac impedance was acquired throughout. Linear regressions demonstrated that higher NED, but not PED, was associated with experiencing less intense self-reported negative, high arousal emotions (e.g., irritated, panicky) during the stressor (<i>β</i> =  − .15, <i>p</i> &lt; .05) although people with higher NED also exhibited greater sympathetic reactivity (<i>β</i> = .16, <i>p</i> &lt; .05). In exploratory analyses, we tested whether the effect of NED on self-reported stress was mediated by the tendency to make internally focus (or self-focused) attributions about performance on the task but did not find a significant indirect effect (<i>p</i> = .085). These results both complement prior work and provide a more complex picture of the role of NED in adaptive responses to stressful life events, suggesting that people with higher NED may experience their emotions as more manageable regardless of their level of physiological arousal.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":72119,"journal":{"name":"Affective science","volume":"4 2","pages":"317 - 331"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-05-27","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://link.springer.com/content/pdf/10.1007/s42761-023-00189-y.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9619528","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Emotion Regulation Strategies and Beliefs About Emotions Predict Psychosocial Outcomes in Response to Multiple Stressors 情绪调节策略和关于情绪的信念预测多重压力下的心理社会结果
Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2023-05-08 DOI: 10.1007/s42761-023-00187-0
Yael H. Waizman, Anna E. Sedykin, João F. Guassi Moreira, Natalie M. Saragosa-Harris, Jennifer A. Silvers, Tara S. Peris

Emotion regulation (ER) strategies and beliefs about emotions (implicit theories of emotions; ITE) may shape psychosocial outcomes during turbulent times, including the transition to adulthood and college while encountering stressors. The normative stressors associated with these transitions were compounded by the COVID-19 pandemic, providing a novel opportunity to examine how emerging adults (EAs) cope with sustained stressors. Stress exposures can heighten existing individual differences and serve as “turning points” that predict psychosocial trajectories. This pre-registered study (https://osf.io/k8mes) of 101 EAs (18–19 years old) examined whether ITE (believing emotions can change or not; incremental vs. entity beliefs) and ER strategy usage (cognitive reappraisal and expressive suppression usage) predicted changes in anxiety symptomatology and feelings of loneliness across five longitudinal assessments (across a 6-month period) before and during the initial months of the COVID-19 pandemic. On average, EAs’ anxiety decreased after the pandemic outbreak but returned to baseline over time, while loneliness remained relatively unchanged across time. ITE explained variance in anxiety across time over and above reappraisal use. Conversely, reappraisal use explained variance in loneliness over and above ITE. For both anxiety and loneliness, suppression use resulted in maladaptive psychosocial outcomes across time. Thus, interventions that target ER strategies and ITE may ameliorate risk and promote resilience in EAs who experience increased instability.

情绪调节(ER)策略和对情绪的信念(情绪的内隐理论;ITE)可能会在动荡时期影响社会心理结果,包括在遇到压力源时向成年和大学过渡。新冠肺炎大流行加剧了与这些转变相关的规范性压力源,为研究新兴成年人(EA)如何应对持续压力源提供了一个新的机会。压力暴露会加剧现有的个体差异,并成为预测心理社会轨迹的“转折点”。这项预先注册的研究(https://osf.io/k8mes)在新冠肺炎爆发前和最初几个月期间,101名EA(18-19岁)在五项纵向评估(6个月期间)中检查了ITE(相信情绪是否会改变;增量信念与实体信念)和ER策略使用(认知重新评估和表达抑制使用)是否预测了焦虑症状和孤独感的变化流行病平均而言,EAs的焦虑在疫情爆发后有所下降,但随着时间的推移又回到了基线,而孤独感在一段时间内保持相对不变。ITE解释了焦虑随时间的变化,而不是重新评估使用。相反,重新评估使用解释了ITE以上孤独感的差异。对于焦虑和孤独,抑制的使用会随着时间的推移导致适应不良的心理社会结果。因此,针对ER策略和ITE的干预措施可以改善经历不稳定性增加的EA的风险并提高其恢复力。
{"title":"Emotion Regulation Strategies and Beliefs About Emotions Predict Psychosocial Outcomes in Response to Multiple Stressors","authors":"Yael H. Waizman,&nbsp;Anna E. Sedykin,&nbsp;João F. Guassi Moreira,&nbsp;Natalie M. Saragosa-Harris,&nbsp;Jennifer A. Silvers,&nbsp;Tara S. Peris","doi":"10.1007/s42761-023-00187-0","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s42761-023-00187-0","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Emotion regulation (ER) strategies and beliefs about emotions (implicit theories of emotions; ITE) may shape psychosocial outcomes during turbulent times, including the transition to adulthood and college while encountering stressors. The normative stressors associated with these transitions were compounded by the COVID-19 pandemic, providing a novel opportunity to examine how emerging adults (EAs) cope with sustained stressors. Stress exposures can heighten existing individual differences and serve as “turning points” that predict psychosocial trajectories. This pre-registered study (https://osf.io/k8mes) of 101 EAs (18–19 years old) examined whether ITE (believing emotions can change or not; incremental vs. entity beliefs) and ER strategy usage (cognitive reappraisal and expressive suppression usage) predicted changes in anxiety symptomatology and feelings of loneliness across five longitudinal assessments (across a 6-month period) before and during the initial months of the COVID-19 pandemic. On average, EAs’ anxiety decreased after the pandemic outbreak but returned to baseline over time, while loneliness remained relatively unchanged across time. ITE explained variance in anxiety across time over and above reappraisal use. Conversely, reappraisal use explained variance in loneliness over and above ITE. For both anxiety and loneliness, suppression use resulted in maladaptive psychosocial outcomes across time. Thus, interventions that target ER strategies and ITE may ameliorate risk and promote resilience in EAs who experience increased instability.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":72119,"journal":{"name":"Affective science","volume":"4 2","pages":"275 - 290"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-05-08","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://link.springer.com/content/pdf/10.1007/s42761-023-00187-0.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9613519","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Emotion Norms Are Unique 情感规范是独一无二的。
Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2023-05-04 DOI: 10.1007/s42761-023-00188-z
Allon Vishkin, Maya Tamir

Emotion norms shape the pursuit, regulation, and experience of emotions, yet much about their nature remains unknown. Like other types of social norms, emotion norms reflect intersubjective consensus, vary in both content and strength, and benefit the well-being of people who adhere to them. However, we propose that emotion norms may also be a unique type of social norm. First, whereas social norms typically target behaviors, emotion norms can target both expressive behavior and subjective states. Second, whereas it may be possible to identify universally held social norms, norms for emotions may lack any universality. Finally, whereas social norms are typically stronger in more collectivist cultures, emotion norms appear to be stronger in more individualist cultures. For each of the potentially distinct features of emotion norms suggested above, we highlight new directions for future research.

情感规范塑造了情感的追求、调节和体验,但其本质仍有很多未知之处。与其他类型的社会规范一样,情感规范反映了主体间的共识,在内容和强度上都有所不同,并有利于遵守这些规范的人的福祉。然而,我们认为情绪规范也可能是一种独特的社会规范。首先,虽然社会规范通常针对行为,但情绪规范可以针对表达行为和主观状态。其次,尽管可以确定普遍持有的社会规范,但情绪规范可能缺乏任何普遍性。最后,尽管社会规范在更集体主义的文化中通常更强大,但情感规范在更个人主义的文化里似乎更强大。对于上述情绪规范的每一个潜在的不同特征,我们强调了未来研究的新方向。
{"title":"Emotion Norms Are Unique","authors":"Allon Vishkin,&nbsp;Maya Tamir","doi":"10.1007/s42761-023-00188-z","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s42761-023-00188-z","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Emotion norms shape the pursuit, regulation, and experience of emotions, yet much about their nature remains unknown. Like other types of social norms, emotion norms reflect intersubjective consensus, vary in both content and strength, and benefit the well-being of people who adhere to them. However, we propose that emotion norms may also be a unique type of social norm. First, whereas social norms typically target behaviors, emotion norms can target both expressive behavior and subjective states. Second, whereas it may be possible to identify universally held social norms, norms for emotions may lack any universality. Finally, whereas social norms are typically stronger in more collectivist cultures, emotion norms appear to be stronger in more individualist cultures. For each of the potentially distinct features of emotion norms suggested above, we highlight new directions for future research.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":72119,"journal":{"name":"Affective science","volume":"4 3","pages":"453 - 457"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-05-04","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://link.springer.com/content/pdf/10.1007/s42761-023-00188-z.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41162329","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Differences Between East Asians and Westerners in the Mental Representations and Visual Information Extraction Involved in the Decoding of Pain Facial Expression Intensity 东亚人与西方人在面部表情强度解码中的心理表征和视觉信息提取的差异
Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2023-05-02 DOI: 10.1007/s42761-023-00186-1
Camille Saumure, Marie-Pier Plouffe-Demers, Daniel Fiset, Stéphanie Cormier, Ye Zhang, Dan Sun, Manni Feng, Feifan Luo, Miriam Kunz, Caroline Blais

Effectively communicating pain is crucial for human beings. Facial expressions are one of the most specific forms of behavior associated with pain, but the way culture shapes expectations about the intensity with which pain is typically facially conveyed, and the visual strategies deployed to decode pain intensity in facial expressions, is poorly understood. The present study used a data-driven approach to compare two cultures, namely East Asians and Westerners, with respect to their mental representations of pain facial expressions (experiment 1, N=60; experiment 2, N=74) and their visual information utilization during the discrimination of facial expressions of pain of different intensities (experiment 3; N=60). Results reveal that compared to Westerners, East Asians expect more intense pain expressions (experiments 1 and 2), need more signal, and do not rely as much as Westerners on core facial features of pain expressions to discriminate between pain intensities (experiment 3). Together, those findings suggest that cultural norms regarding socially accepted pain behaviors shape the expectations about pain facial expressions and decoding visual strategies. Furthermore, they highlight the complexity of emotional facial expressions and the importance of studying pain communication in multicultural settings.

有效沟通疼痛对人类来说至关重要。面部表情是与疼痛相关的最具体的行为形式之一,但文化塑造人们对疼痛通常通过面部传达的强度的期望的方式,以及用于解码面部表情中疼痛强度的视觉策略,人们对此知之甚少。本研究使用数据驱动的方法比较了两种文化,即东亚人和西方人对疼痛面部表情的心理表征(实验1,N=60;实验2,N=74)以及他们在区分不同强度疼痛面部表情时的视觉信息利用率(实验3;N=60)。结果表明,与西方人相比,东亚人期望更强烈的疼痛表情(实验1和2),需要更多的信号,并且不像西方人那样依赖疼痛表情的核心面部特征来区分疼痛强度(实验3)。总之,这些发现表明,关于社会接受的疼痛行为的文化规范塑造了对疼痛面部表情和解码视觉策略的期望。此外,他们强调了情感面部表情的复杂性,以及在多元文化环境中研究疼痛沟通的重要性。
{"title":"Differences Between East Asians and Westerners in the Mental Representations and Visual Information Extraction Involved in the Decoding of Pain Facial Expression Intensity","authors":"Camille Saumure,&nbsp;Marie-Pier Plouffe-Demers,&nbsp;Daniel Fiset,&nbsp;Stéphanie Cormier,&nbsp;Ye Zhang,&nbsp;Dan Sun,&nbsp;Manni Feng,&nbsp;Feifan Luo,&nbsp;Miriam Kunz,&nbsp;Caroline Blais","doi":"10.1007/s42761-023-00186-1","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s42761-023-00186-1","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Effectively communicating pain is crucial for human beings. Facial expressions are one of the most specific forms of behavior associated with pain, but the way culture shapes expectations about the intensity with which pain is typically facially conveyed, and the visual strategies deployed to decode pain intensity in facial expressions, is poorly understood. The present study used a data-driven approach to compare two cultures, namely East Asians and Westerners, with respect to their mental representations of pain facial expressions (experiment 1, <i>N</i>=60; experiment 2, <i>N</i>=74) and their visual information utilization during the discrimination of facial expressions of pain of different intensities (experiment 3; <i>N</i>=60). Results reveal that compared to Westerners, East Asians expect more intense pain expressions (experiments 1 and 2), need more signal, and do not rely as much as Westerners on core facial features of pain expressions to discriminate between pain intensities (experiment 3). Together, those findings suggest that cultural norms regarding socially accepted pain behaviors shape the expectations about pain facial expressions and decoding visual strategies. Furthermore, they highlight the complexity of emotional facial expressions and the importance of studying pain communication in multicultural settings.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":72119,"journal":{"name":"Affective science","volume":"4 2","pages":"332 - 349"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-05-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://link.springer.com/content/pdf/10.1007/s42761-023-00186-1.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9610390","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
How to Improve Others’ Emotions: Reappraise and be Responsive 如何改善他人情绪:重新评价并做出回应
Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2023-04-15 DOI: 10.1007/s42761-023-00183-4
Olivia Jurkiewicz, C. Blair McGarrigle, Christopher Oveis

People often try to improve others’ emotions. However, it is unclear which interpersonal emotion regulation strategies are most effective and why. In 121 candid dyadic conversations between undergraduate students via video conferencing, target participants recounted a stressful event to regulator participants. Three strategies used by regulators during these conversations to change targets’ emotions were obtained from the regulator after the conversation: extrinsic reappraisal, extrinsic suppression, and extrinsic acceptance. Perceived regulator responsiveness was obtained from targets to examine the social consequences of extrinsic emotion regulation and its mediating role in successful extrinsic emotion regulation. We found that regulators’ extrinsic reappraisal use was associated with improved target emotions measured across two distinct classes of outcomes: targets’ emotions during the conversation and targets’ perception that the regulator improved their emotions. Regulators’ extrinsic suppression and acceptance, in contrast, were not related with improved target emotions or perceptions of improvement. Instead, all extrinsic regulatory strategies were associated with improved targets’ emotions when mediated by targets’ perceptions of regulator responsiveness. Finally, observer-ratings of regulators’ extrinsic reappraisal and suppression use were found to be consistent with regulators’ self-ratings and follow the same pattern of results on the outcome measures. These findings provide insight into why the social regulation of emotions can succeed or fail and hold implications for interventions aimed at guiding people toward more successfully improving others’ emotions.

人们经常试图改善他人的情绪。然而,目前尚不清楚哪些人际情绪调节策略最有效,以及为什么。在本科生之间通过视频会议进行的121次坦诚的二人对话中,目标参与者向监管参与者讲述了一个紧张的事件。在这些对话中,调节者用来改变目标情绪的三种策略是在对话后从调节者那里获得的:外在重新评估、外在抑制和外在接受。从目标中获得感知调节反应性,以检验外在情绪调节的社会后果及其在成功的外在情绪调节中的中介作用。我们发现,在两类不同的结果中,监管者的外在重新评估使用与目标情绪的改善有关:目标在对话中的情绪和目标认为监管者改善了他们的情绪。相反,监管者的外在抑制和接受与目标情绪的改善或对改善的感知无关。相反,当目标对调节反应的感知介导时,所有外在调节策略都与目标情绪的改善有关。最后,观察者对监管机构外部重新评估和抑制使用的评级与监管机构的自我评级一致,并在结果测量上遵循相同的结果模式。这些发现深入了解了为什么情绪的社会调节会成功或失败,并对旨在引导人们更成功地改善他人情绪的干预措施具有启示意义。
{"title":"How to Improve Others’ Emotions: Reappraise and be Responsive","authors":"Olivia Jurkiewicz,&nbsp;C. Blair McGarrigle,&nbsp;Christopher Oveis","doi":"10.1007/s42761-023-00183-4","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s42761-023-00183-4","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>People often try to improve others’ emotions. However, it is unclear which interpersonal emotion regulation strategies are most effective and why. In 121 candid dyadic conversations between undergraduate students via video conferencing, target participants recounted a stressful event to regulator participants. Three strategies used by regulators during these conversations to change targets’ emotions were obtained from the regulator after the conversation: extrinsic reappraisal, extrinsic suppression, and extrinsic acceptance. Perceived regulator responsiveness was obtained from targets to examine the social consequences of extrinsic emotion regulation and its mediating role in successful extrinsic emotion regulation. We found that regulators’ extrinsic reappraisal use was associated with improved target emotions measured across two distinct classes of outcomes: targets’ emotions during the conversation and targets’ perception that the regulator improved their emotions. Regulators’ extrinsic suppression and acceptance, in contrast, were not related with improved target emotions or perceptions of improvement. Instead, all extrinsic regulatory strategies were associated with improved targets’ emotions when mediated by targets’ perceptions of regulator responsiveness. Finally, observer-ratings of regulators’ extrinsic reappraisal and suppression use were found to be consistent with regulators’ self-ratings and follow the same pattern of results on the outcome measures. These findings provide insight into why the social regulation of emotions can succeed or fail and hold implications for interventions aimed at guiding people toward more successfully improving others’ emotions.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":72119,"journal":{"name":"Affective science","volume":"4 2","pages":"233 - 247"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-04-15","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://link.springer.com/content/pdf/10.1007/s42761-023-00183-4.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9607722","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
期刊
Affective science
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1