首页 > 最新文献

Current research in behavioral sciences最新文献

英文 中文
An animal model of human gambling behavior 人类赌博行为的动物模型
Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2023-01-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.crbeha.2023.100101
Thomas R. Zentall

Human gambling almost always results in a loss. Thus, it is generally assumed that humans gamble for enjoyment and the enticement of winning. Although animals are purported to engage in optimal foraging behavior and should be sensitive to the probability of reinforcement, similar suboptimal behavior can be found in pigeons and other animals. They show a preference for an alternative that is associated with a signal for a low probability of a large reward (e.g., 20% probability of 10 pellets – a mean of 2 pellets) over an alternative that is associated with a signal for a high probability of a smaller reward (100% probability of 3 pellets). This effect may result from the strong conditioned reinforcement associated with a stimulus that is always followed by a reinforcer, but surprisingly, little conditioned inhibition associated with the signal for the absence of a reinforcer. A similar mechanism appears to be responsible for human gambling (gamblers tend to overvalue wins and undervalue losses). We have also found that for pigeons (and perhaps humans as well), the probability of the conditioned reinforcer is relatively unimportant, it is primarily the value of the reinforcer when it does occur (e.g., 10 pellets vs. 3 pellets) that is important. Interestingly, pigeons show several other parallels to human gambling behavior. For example, hungrier pigeons show a greater tendency to choose suboptimally. Also, pigeons that have had enrichment in the form of social experience in a larger cage show a reduced tendency to choose suboptimally. This animal model may provide a useful analog to human gambling behavior, one that is free from the influence of human culture, language, social reinforcement, and other experiential biases that may encourage human gambling.

人类赌博几乎总是以输为结果。因此,一般认为人类赌博是为了享受和获胜的诱惑。虽然动物被认为是最优的觅食行为,并且应该对强化的可能性敏感,但在鸽子和其他动物中也可以发现类似的次优行为。他们更倾向于选择一个低概率获得大奖励的选项(例如,20%的概率获得10个球,平均2个球),而不是选择一个高概率获得小奖励的选项(100%的概率获得3个球)。这种效应可能是由于与刺激相关的强条件强化总是伴随着强化物,但令人惊讶的是,与缺乏强化物的信号相关的条件抑制很少。人类赌博似乎也有类似的机制(赌徒往往高估胜利,低估损失)。我们还发现,对鸽子(也许对人类也是如此)来说,条件强化物的概率相对不重要,重要的是强化物在发生时的价值(例如,10粒vs. 3粒)。有趣的是,鸽子与人类的赌博行为还有其他一些相似之处。例如,饥饿的鸽子更倾向于做出次优选择。此外,在更大的笼子里以社会经验的形式获得丰富的鸽子,表现出较低的次优选择倾向。这个动物模型可能提供了一个有用的模拟人类赌博行为的模型,它不受人类文化、语言、社会强化和其他可能鼓励人类赌博的经验偏见的影响。
{"title":"An animal model of human gambling behavior","authors":"Thomas R. Zentall","doi":"10.1016/j.crbeha.2023.100101","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.crbeha.2023.100101","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Human gambling almost always results in a loss. Thus, it is generally assumed that humans gamble for enjoyment and the enticement of winning. Although animals are purported to engage in optimal foraging behavior and should be sensitive to the probability of reinforcement, similar suboptimal behavior can be found in pigeons and other animals. They show a preference for an alternative that is associated with a signal for a low probability of a large reward (e.g., 20% probability of 10 pellets – a mean of 2 pellets) over an alternative that is associated with a signal for a high probability of a smaller reward (100% probability of 3 pellets). This effect may result from the strong conditioned reinforcement associated with a stimulus that is always followed by a reinforcer, but surprisingly, little conditioned inhibition associated with the signal for the absence of a reinforcer. A similar mechanism appears to be responsible for human gambling (gamblers tend to overvalue wins and undervalue losses). We have also found that for pigeons (and perhaps humans as well), the <em>probability</em> of the conditioned reinforcer is relatively unimportant, it is primarily the <em>value</em> of the reinforcer when it does occur (e.g., 10 pellets vs. 3 pellets) that is important. Interestingly, pigeons show several other parallels to human gambling behavior. For example, hungrier pigeons show a greater tendency to choose suboptimally. Also, pigeons that have had enrichment in the form of social experience in a larger cage show a reduced tendency to choose suboptimally. This animal model may provide a useful analog to human gambling behavior, one that is free from the influence of human culture, language, social reinforcement, and other experiential biases that may encourage human gambling.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":72746,"journal":{"name":"Current research in behavioral sciences","volume":"4 ","pages":"Article 100101"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"47985452","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Preschoolers’ screen time and reduced opportunities for quality interaction: Associations with language development and parent-child closeness 学龄前儿童的屏幕时间和减少的高质量互动机会:与语言发展和亲子亲密的关系
Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2023-01-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.crbeha.2023.100140
Megan Gath , Brigid McNeill , Gail Gillon

Children today spend an unprecedented amount of their time watching or interacting with screens. In this research, we examined the use of screen time (both television and electronic media use) in relation to preschoolers’ language development and the parent-child relationship. Participants were 84 parents and their preschoolers. Children ranged in age from 3 to 5 years old. Parents provided survey data and children were assessed on language ability and comprehension. Results show that preschool children who spent more time on screens, both television viewing and electronic media use, scored lower on language production, language comprehension, and parent-child closeness. Further, we found support for the displacement hypothesis, with significant indirect effects of screen time on language and parent-child closeness through reductions in shared reading and quality parent-child interactions.

今天的孩子花在看屏幕或与屏幕互动上的时间是前所未有的。在这项研究中,我们调查了屏幕时间的使用(包括电视和电子媒体的使用)与学龄前儿童语言发展和亲子关系的关系。参与者是84位家长和他们的学龄前儿童。孩子们的年龄从3岁到5岁不等。家长提供调查数据,并评估儿童的语言能力和理解能力。结果表明,学龄前儿童在屏幕上花费更多的时间,无论是看电视还是使用电子媒体,在语言产生、语言理解和亲子亲密度方面得分较低。此外,我们还发现了对位移假说的支持,屏幕时间通过减少共同阅读和亲子互动的质量,对语言和亲子亲密度产生了显著的间接影响。
{"title":"Preschoolers’ screen time and reduced opportunities for quality interaction: Associations with language development and parent-child closeness","authors":"Megan Gath ,&nbsp;Brigid McNeill ,&nbsp;Gail Gillon","doi":"10.1016/j.crbeha.2023.100140","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.crbeha.2023.100140","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Children today spend an unprecedented amount of their time watching or interacting with screens. In this research, we examined the use of screen time (both television and electronic media use) in relation to preschoolers’ language development and the parent-child relationship. Participants were 84 parents and their preschoolers. Children ranged in age from 3 to 5 years old. Parents provided survey data and children were assessed on language ability and comprehension. Results show that preschool children who spent more time on screens, both television viewing and electronic media use, scored lower on language production, language comprehension, and parent-child closeness. Further, we found support for the displacement hypothesis, with significant indirect effects of screen time on language and parent-child closeness through reductions in shared reading and quality parent-child interactions.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":72746,"journal":{"name":"Current research in behavioral sciences","volume":"5 ","pages":"Article 100140"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S266651822300044X/pdfft?md5=262a3c3594263fd060f9ac2e79f62927&pid=1-s2.0-S266651822300044X-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138327670","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Popular names are given less frequently to babies in individualistic countries: Further validation of unique names as an indicator of individualism 在个人主义国家,给婴儿起流行名字的频率较低:将独特名字作为个人主义指标的进一步验证
Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2023-01-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.crbeha.2022.100094
Yuji Ogihara

A prior study showed that popular names are given less frequently to babies in individualistic countries in European, Anglo-American (North American), and Oceanian cultures. However, the samples of the previous study were limited, and it did not examine other cultures, leaving it unclear whether the relationship is still found even when other cultures are included. It is important to confirm validity of indices cross-culturally. Thus, the present study included two unexamined cultures in the analyses: East Asian culture and Latin American culture. Following the previous study, I calculated the rates of popular baby names in Japan and Puerto Rico, and examined how the addition of these two regions affected the results. Analyses showed that the negative relationships between the rates of popular names and individualism scores were still salient. Therefore, this study further confirmed the validity of unique names as an indicator of individualism in more diverse cultural contexts.

先前的一项研究表明,在欧洲、英美(北美)和大洋洲文化的个人主义国家,给婴儿起流行名字的频率较低。然而,之前的研究样本有限,而且没有考察其他文化,因此,即使包括其他文化,这种关系是否仍然存在尚不清楚。跨文化确认指标的有效性是非常重要的。因此,本研究在分析中包括了两种未经检验的文化:东亚文化和拉丁美洲文化。根据之前的研究,我计算了日本和波多黎各受欢迎的婴儿名字的比率,并检查了这两个地区的加入对结果的影响。分析表明,流行名字的比率与个人主义得分之间的负相关关系仍然很明显。因此,本研究进一步证实了独特名字在更多样化的文化背景下作为个人主义指标的有效性。
{"title":"Popular names are given less frequently to babies in individualistic countries: Further validation of unique names as an indicator of individualism","authors":"Yuji Ogihara","doi":"10.1016/j.crbeha.2022.100094","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.crbeha.2022.100094","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>A prior study showed that popular names are given less frequently to babies in individualistic countries in European, Anglo-American (North American), and Oceanian cultures. However, the samples of the previous study were limited, and it did not examine other cultures, leaving it unclear whether the relationship is still found even when other cultures are included. It is important to confirm validity of indices cross-culturally. Thus, the present study included two unexamined cultures in the analyses: East Asian culture and Latin American culture. Following the previous study, I calculated the rates of popular baby names in Japan and Puerto Rico, and examined how the addition of these two regions affected the results. Analyses showed that the negative relationships between the rates of popular names and individualism scores were still salient. Therefore, this study further confirmed the validity of unique names as an indicator of individualism in more diverse cultural contexts.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":72746,"journal":{"name":"Current research in behavioral sciences","volume":"4 ","pages":"Article 100094"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"42874094","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
Loneliness and preferences for palatable foods: The role of coping 孤独和对美味食物的偏好:应对的作用
Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2023-01-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.crbeha.2023.100119
Fuschia M. Sirois, Marios Biskas
{"title":"Loneliness and preferences for palatable foods: The role of coping","authors":"Fuschia M. Sirois,&nbsp;Marios Biskas","doi":"10.1016/j.crbeha.2023.100119","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.crbeha.2023.100119","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":72746,"journal":{"name":"Current research in behavioral sciences","volume":"5 ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"49753891","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 3
Effects of individuating information on implicit person perception are largely consistent across individual differences and two types of target groups 个体化信息对内隐人知觉的影响在个体差异和两种类型的目标群体中基本一致
Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2023-01-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.crbeha.2022.100090
Rachel S. Rubinstein , Madelyn Marshall , Lee Jussim , Nathan Honeycutt

Previous research has investigated characteristics of individuating information, stereotypes, and evaluative circumstances that moderate reliance on social category information and individuating information in implicit person perception. However, possibly no research has examined characteristics of perceivers that may be involved in these processes. In four studies (N = 1545), the present research tested the effects of six individual differences on application of race and gender stereotypes in implicit perceptions of individuals and the potential moderating effects of diagnosticity of individuating information. We found that individuating information affected implicit person similarly regardless of the diagnosticity of the individuating information, the target group, and—largely—individual differences. Although these findings involved several null results, these results are nonetheless informative because they provide evidence that individuating information is a promising means of bias reduction given its consistent effects.

先前的研究调查了个体信息、刻板印象和评价环境的特征,这些特征在内隐的人感知中适度依赖社会类别信息和个体信息。然而,可能没有任何研究考察可能参与这些过程的感知者的特征。在四项研究中(N=1545),本研究测试了六个个体差异对个体内隐认知中种族和性别刻板印象应用的影响,以及个性化信息诊断性的潜在调节作用。我们发现,无论个体信息的诊断性、目标群体以及很大程度上的个体差异如何,个体信息对内隐人的影响都是相似的。尽管这些发现涉及几个无效的结果,但这些结果仍然是有信息的,因为它们提供了证据,证明个性化信息是一种很有前途的减少偏见的方法,因为它具有一致的效果。
{"title":"Effects of individuating information on implicit person perception are largely consistent across individual differences and two types of target groups","authors":"Rachel S. Rubinstein ,&nbsp;Madelyn Marshall ,&nbsp;Lee Jussim ,&nbsp;Nathan Honeycutt","doi":"10.1016/j.crbeha.2022.100090","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.crbeha.2022.100090","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Previous research has investigated characteristics of individuating information, stereotypes, and evaluative circumstances that moderate reliance on social category information and individuating information in implicit person perception. However, possibly no research has examined characteristics of <em>perceivers</em> that may be involved in these processes. In four studies (<em>N</em> = 1545), the present research tested the effects of six individual differences on application of race and gender stereotypes in implicit perceptions of individuals and the potential moderating effects of diagnosticity of individuating information. We found that individuating information affected implicit person similarly regardless of the diagnosticity of the individuating information, the target group, and—largely—individual differences. Although these findings involved several null results, these results are nonetheless informative because they provide evidence that individuating information is a promising means of bias reduction given its consistent effects.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":72746,"journal":{"name":"Current research in behavioral sciences","volume":"4 ","pages":"Article 100090"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"49815248","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Do concreteness fading and guidance fading aid learning from perceptually rich visualizations? Changes in style lead to more cognitive load and interfere with learning 具体性消退和指导性消退是否有助于从感知丰富的可视化中学习?风格的改变会导致更多的认知负荷并干扰学习
Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2023-01-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.crbeha.2023.100112
Alexander Skulmowski

Perceptually rich visualizations can overburden learners with too many details. As some learning tasks depend on realism and details, techniques that facilitate the cognitive processing of such visualizations are sought after. Concreteness fading has been proposed as an instructional method in which learners are first presented with a detailed visualization that is later replaced with a schematic version. A second technique that is thought to help learners is guidance fading, in which tasks get successively more difficult. In the first experiment (n = 125), fading the concreteness of an anatomical visualization, however, had a negative effect on learning, while using guidance fading during testing (realized by increasing the difficulty of the tests over time) had no effect. The second experiment (n = 107) was conducted to assess whether guidance fading implemented by first showing a complex model with color cues and later removing these hints can foster learning. The study revealed that this form of guidance fading had no effect on learning. The results have implications for the design of instructional visualizations and animations as they outline how changing the style of a visualization can interfere with building mental models. Based on the findings, educators should carefully consider whether they need to show visualizations that differ in their visual style in quick succession during a lecture or in an animation.

视觉上丰富的可视化可能会让学习者负担过重,因为细节太多。由于一些学习任务依赖于真实感和细节,因此寻求有助于这种可视化的认知处理的技术。具体性消退已被提出作为一种教学方法,在这种方法中,首先向学习者呈现详细的可视化,然后用示意图版本取代。第二种被认为有助于学习者的技术是引导消退,即任务变得越来越困难。然而,在第一个实验(n=125)中,减弱解剖可视化的具体性对学习产生了负面影响,而在测试过程中使用指导减弱(通过随着时间的推移增加测试难度来实现)则没有影响。进行第二个实验(n=107),以评估通过首先显示具有颜色提示的复杂模型并随后去除这些提示来实现的引导衰落是否可以促进学习。研究表明,这种形式的指导消退对学习没有影响。研究结果对教学可视化和动画的设计具有启示意义,因为它们概述了改变可视化风格如何干扰构建心理模型。基于这些发现,教育工作者应该仔细考虑他们是否需要在讲座或动画中快速连续地展示不同视觉风格的可视化效果。
{"title":"Do concreteness fading and guidance fading aid learning from perceptually rich visualizations? Changes in style lead to more cognitive load and interfere with learning","authors":"Alexander Skulmowski","doi":"10.1016/j.crbeha.2023.100112","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.crbeha.2023.100112","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Perceptually rich visualizations can overburden learners with too many details. As some learning tasks depend on realism and details, techniques that facilitate the cognitive processing of such visualizations are sought after. Concreteness fading has been proposed as an instructional method in which learners are first presented with a detailed visualization that is later replaced with a schematic version. A second technique that is thought to help learners is guidance fading, in which tasks get successively more difficult. In the first experiment (<em>n</em> = 125), fading the concreteness of an anatomical visualization, however, had a negative effect on learning, while using guidance fading during testing (realized by increasing the difficulty of the tests over time) had no effect. The second experiment (<em>n</em> = 107) was conducted to assess whether guidance fading implemented by first showing a complex model with color cues and later removing these hints can foster learning. The study revealed that this form of guidance fading had no effect on learning. The results have implications for the design of instructional visualizations and animations as they outline how changing the style of a visualization can interfere with building mental models. Based on the findings, educators should carefully consider whether they need to show visualizations that differ in their visual style in quick succession during a lecture or in an animation.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":72746,"journal":{"name":"Current research in behavioral sciences","volume":"4 ","pages":"Article 100112"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"49815249","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Loneliness in (inter)action: Behavioral correlates of loneliness in friendship interactions 行动中的孤独感:友谊互动中孤独感的行为相关性
Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2023-01-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.crbeha.2023.100135
Marcus Mund , Jingqi Yang , Pamela Qualter

Previous studies have found that loneliness of one person can be judged quite accurately by a close friend or partner. Yet, it is unclear whether there are specific behavioral cues the other-ratings are based on. In the present study, 54 female friendship dyads were videotaped during a guided conversation and behavioral cuesl were coded using the SPAFF coding system. The results indicated that loneliness was negatively associated with one's own and the friend's overall friendship satisfaction and their satisfaction with the interaction. However, with the exception of an inconsistent mediation effect found for sadness, none of the coded behavioral cues were found to mediate the association between loneliness and interaction quality. Nevertheless, the results of the present study may help to understand why it is so difficult to identify people at risk for experiencing loneliness and draws attention to other processes through which loneliness may become visible to others.

之前的研究发现,一个人的孤独程度可以通过亲密的朋友或伴侣来准确判断。然而,目前尚不清楚其他评级是否基于特定的行为线索。在本研究中,研究人员对54名女性朋友在谈话过程中进行了录像,并使用SPAFF编码系统对行为线索进行了编码。结果表明,孤独感与自己和朋友的整体友谊满意度以及他们对互动的满意度呈负相关。然而,除了在悲伤中发现不一致的中介效应外,没有发现任何编码行为线索可以中介孤独和互动质量之间的关联。然而,目前的研究结果可能有助于理解为什么很难识别有孤独风险的人,并引起人们对孤独可能对他人可见的其他过程的关注。
{"title":"Loneliness in (inter)action: Behavioral correlates of loneliness in friendship interactions","authors":"Marcus Mund ,&nbsp;Jingqi Yang ,&nbsp;Pamela Qualter","doi":"10.1016/j.crbeha.2023.100135","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.crbeha.2023.100135","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Previous studies have found that loneliness of one person can be judged quite accurately by a close friend or partner. Yet, it is unclear whether there are specific behavioral cues the other-ratings are based on. In the present study, 54 female friendship dyads were videotaped during a guided conversation and behavioral cuesl were coded using the SPAFF coding system. The results indicated that loneliness was negatively associated with one's own and the friend's overall friendship satisfaction and their satisfaction with the interaction. However, with the exception of an inconsistent mediation effect found for sadness, none of the coded behavioral cues were found to mediate the association between loneliness and interaction quality. Nevertheless, the results of the present study may help to understand why it is so difficult to identify people at risk for experiencing loneliness and draws attention to other processes through which loneliness may become visible to others.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":72746,"journal":{"name":"Current research in behavioral sciences","volume":"5 ","pages":"Article 100135"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"43908955","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Topographical map for quantities – Indeed? Commentary on Harvey et al 2013, 2017 工程量地形图——是吗?Harvey等人评论20132017
Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2023-01-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.crbeha.2023.100103
Tali Leibovich-Raveh

In an fMRI passive viewing task made in a 7T scanner, Harvey et al (2013) asked adult participants to passively view dots on a screen and reported a topographical map of quantities in the human intraparietal sulcus (IPS), suggesting that quantities are directly perceived, and their cortical organization may be fundamental to human abilities in mathematics. This paper, which was published in Science, got lots of attention and was cited 469 times since. In a later Neuroimage paper (2017), the authors analyzed the response to the visual features of the stimuli and suggested that the neural responses do not reflect the processing of visual features. I raise several reasons to restrict these conclusions. (1) the study design emphasized quantity over continuous magnitudes such as density and area. This may be problematic since neural activity can be influenced by attending to a specific dimension. (2) Poor control over continuous magnitudes. (3) The conclusions should be restricted to small quantities. (4) Task context influences neural activity, and (5) When the analysis is restricted to a specific cortical area, the conclusion should be restricted as well.

Harvey等人(2013)在7T扫描仪上进行的fMRI被动观看任务中,要求成年参与者被动观看屏幕上的点,并报告了人类顶叶内沟(IPS)数量的地形图,表明数量是直接感知的,它们的皮层组织可能是人类数学能力的基础。这篇发表在《科学》杂志上的论文引起了广泛关注,被引用了469次。在随后的Neuroimage论文(2017)中,作者分析了对刺激视觉特征的反应,并提出神经反应并不反映视觉特征的处理。我提出了几个理由来限制这些结论。(1)研究设计强调数量多于密度、面积等连续量级。这可能是有问题的,因为神经活动可以通过关注特定的维度来影响。(2)对连续幅度控制不力。(3)结论应限于小数量。(4)任务情境影响神经活动;(5)当分析仅限于特定的皮层区域时,结论也应受到限制。
{"title":"Topographical map for quantities – Indeed? Commentary on Harvey et al 2013, 2017","authors":"Tali Leibovich-Raveh","doi":"10.1016/j.crbeha.2023.100103","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.crbeha.2023.100103","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>In an fMRI passive viewing task made in a 7T scanner, Harvey et al (2013) asked adult participants to passively view dots on a screen and reported a topographical map of quantities in the human intraparietal sulcus (IPS), suggesting that quantities are directly perceived, and their cortical organization may be fundamental to human abilities in mathematics. This paper, which was published in Science, got lots of attention and was cited 469 times since. In a later Neuroimage paper (2017), the authors analyzed the response to the visual features of the stimuli and suggested that the neural responses do not reflect the processing of visual features. I raise several reasons to restrict these conclusions. (1) the study design emphasized quantity over continuous magnitudes such as density and area. This may be problematic since neural activity can be influenced by attending to a specific dimension. (2) Poor control over continuous magnitudes. (3) The conclusions should be restricted to small quantities. (4) Task context influences neural activity, and (5) When the analysis is restricted to a specific cortical area, the conclusion should be restricted as well.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":72746,"journal":{"name":"Current research in behavioral sciences","volume":"4 ","pages":"Article 100103"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41851286","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Exploring blog narratives of parental loneliness: A thematic network analysis 探索父母孤独的博客叙事:一个主题网络分析
Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2023-01-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.crbeha.2023.100137
R. Nowland , G. Thomson , L. Cross , K. Whittaker , P. Gregory , J.M. Charles , C. Day

UK-based national surveys and international longitudinal studies have shown that around a third of parents experience chronic or persistent loneliness. There is limited research about the experience of loneliness in parenthood, however blogs authored by parents, sharing their personal experiences about loneliness offer a potential rich data source. The purpose of this study was to identify and analyse blog narratives written by parents who had experienced loneliness to increase understanding of their experiences. One hundred and ninety-six relevant blog posts were identified, of which 157 had contact details to request permission to use the blog post in the study. Twenty-two parent bloggers gave their permission. Thematic network analysis was used to open code the blogs and 4 organising themes and subthemes emerged, which centred on a global theme of disconnection. Disconnection underpinned themes relating to a sense that being a parent was overwhelming, changes in identity linked to becoming a parent, difficulties in sharing feelings of loneliness with others, and a need for social connection. Findings point to parents being unprepared for the transition to parenthood, with implications for perinatal education and support, including further opportunities for parents to connect to reduce social isolation.

英国的全国性调查和国际纵向研究表明,大约三分之一的父母会经历长期或持续的孤独。关于为人父母的孤独体验的研究有限,然而,父母撰写的博客分享了他们关于孤独的个人经历,提供了一个潜在的丰富数据来源。本研究的目的是识别和分析经历过孤独的父母写的博客故事,以增加对他们经历的理解。共发现了196篇相关博客文章,其中157篇有联系方式,要求在研究中使用该博客文章。22位家长博主表示同意。主题网络分析用于博客的开放代码,出现了4个组织主题和子主题,这些主题和子标题围绕着一个全球性的断开主题。断开联系支撑了一些主题,这些主题涉及为人父母的感觉是压倒性的,与为人父母相关的身份变化,与他人分享孤独感的困难,以及社会联系的需要。研究结果表明,父母对为人父母的过渡没有做好准备,这对围产期教育和支持产生了影响,包括父母有更多的机会建立联系,以减少社会孤立。
{"title":"Exploring blog narratives of parental loneliness: A thematic network analysis","authors":"R. Nowland ,&nbsp;G. Thomson ,&nbsp;L. Cross ,&nbsp;K. Whittaker ,&nbsp;P. Gregory ,&nbsp;J.M. Charles ,&nbsp;C. Day","doi":"10.1016/j.crbeha.2023.100137","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.crbeha.2023.100137","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>UK-based national surveys and international longitudinal studies have shown that around a third of parents experience chronic or persistent loneliness. There is limited research about the experience of loneliness in parenthood, however blogs authored by parents, sharing their personal experiences about loneliness offer a potential rich data source. The purpose of this study was to identify and analyse blog narratives written by parents who had experienced loneliness to increase understanding of their experiences. One hundred and ninety-six relevant blog posts were identified, of which 157 had contact details to request permission to use the blog post in the study. Twenty-two parent bloggers gave their permission. Thematic network analysis was used to open code the blogs and 4 organising themes and subthemes emerged, which centred on a global theme of disconnection. Disconnection underpinned themes relating to a sense that being a parent was overwhelming, changes in identity linked to becoming a parent, difficulties in sharing feelings of loneliness with others, and a need for social connection. Findings point to parents being unprepared for the transition to parenthood, with implications for perinatal education and support, including further opportunities for parents to connect to reduce social isolation.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":72746,"journal":{"name":"Current research in behavioral sciences","volume":"5 ","pages":"Article 100137"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"49756021","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Sensory processing sensitivity and its relation to sensation seeking 感觉加工敏感性及其与感觉寻求的关系
Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2023-01-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.crbeha.2023.100100
Bianca P. Acevedo , Elaine N. Aron , Arthur Aron , Tracy Cooper , Robert Marhenke

Sensory processing sensitivity (SPS) is a trait associated with enhanced responsivity to environmental stimuli and the tendency to pause before acting. However, qualitative data suggest that some who are high in SPS may also be high in sensation seeking (SS), a trait characterized by the seeking of novel and intense experiences. Thus, this online study examined SPS and SS among 214 individuals (mean age, 30), using the Highly Sensitive Person Scale (the standard SPS measure), a standard measure of sensation seeking (ZKA-PQ); and the newly developed Sensation-Seeking Scale for Highly Sensitive Persons (SSS/HSP). We also measured risk-taking, impulsivity, and neuroticism, as control variables. Results showed that SPS (controlling or not controlling for neuroticism) was moderately negatively correlated with the ZKA-PQ, risk-taking and impulsivity; but positively associated with negative urgency—the tendency to act impulsively due to negative affect. Also, the validity of the SSS/HSP was supported, as it showed a strong positive correlation with standard measures of SS and risk-taking, a moderate association with impulsivity, and (as it was designed to do) a near zero correlation with SPS. In conclusion, although some individuals with SPS may express high SS, the two traits are largely separate; and the SSS/HSP is a reliable measure of SS, having items better-suited for the highly sensitive.

感觉加工敏感性(SPS)是一种与对环境刺激的增强反应和行动前暂停倾向相关的特征。然而,定性数据表明,一些SPS高的人可能也有高的感觉寻求(SS),这是一种以寻求新奇和强烈体验为特征的特征。因此,这项在线研究使用高度敏感者量表(标准SPS测量)和感觉寻求的标准测量(ZKA-PQ)对214名个体(平均年龄30岁)的SPS和SS进行了检查;以及新开发的《高敏感人群寻求感觉量表》(SSS/HSP)。我们还测量了冒险、冲动和神经质作为控制变量。结果表明,SPS(控制或不控制神经质)与ZKA-PQ、冒险精神和冲动性呈中度负相关;但正相关的是消极的紧迫性——由于消极影响而冲动行事的倾向。此外,SSS/HSP的有效性也得到了支持,因为它显示出与SS和冒险的标准测量有很强的正相关,与冲动性有适度的关联,并且(正如其设计的那样)与SPS的相关性接近于零。综上所述,虽然某些SPS个体可能表现出较高的SS,但这两个性状在很大程度上是分开的;而SSS/HSP是一种可靠的SS测量方法,其中的项目更适合高敏感人群。
{"title":"Sensory processing sensitivity and its relation to sensation seeking","authors":"Bianca P. Acevedo ,&nbsp;Elaine N. Aron ,&nbsp;Arthur Aron ,&nbsp;Tracy Cooper ,&nbsp;Robert Marhenke","doi":"10.1016/j.crbeha.2023.100100","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.crbeha.2023.100100","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Sensory processing sensitivity (SPS) is a trait associated with enhanced responsivity to environmental stimuli and the tendency to pause before acting. However, qualitative data suggest that some who are high in SPS may also be high in sensation seeking (SS), a trait characterized by the seeking of novel and intense experiences. Thus, this online study examined SPS and SS among 214 individuals (mean age, 30), using the Highly Sensitive Person Scale (the standard SPS measure), a standard measure of sensation seeking (ZKA-PQ); and the newly developed Sensation-Seeking Scale for Highly Sensitive Persons (SSS/HSP). We also measured risk-taking, impulsivity, and neuroticism, as control variables. Results showed that SPS (controlling or not controlling for neuroticism) was moderately negatively correlated with the ZKA-PQ, risk-taking and impulsivity; but positively associated with negative urgency—the tendency to act impulsively due to negative affect. Also, the validity of the SSS/HSP was supported, as it showed a strong positive correlation with standard measures of SS and risk-taking, a moderate association with impulsivity, and (as it was designed to do) a near zero correlation with SPS. In conclusion, although some individuals with SPS may express high SS, the two traits are largely separate; and the SSS/HSP is a reliable measure of SS, having items better-suited for the highly sensitive.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":72746,"journal":{"name":"Current research in behavioral sciences","volume":"4 ","pages":"Article 100100"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"42687755","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 6
期刊
Current research in behavioral sciences
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1