首页 > 最新文献

Current research in behavioral sciences最新文献

英文 中文
How does divided attention hinder different stages of episodic memory retrieval? 注意力分散如何阻碍情景记忆提取的不同阶段?
Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2023-01-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.crbeha.2023.100139
Nursena Ataseven , Nursima Ünver , Eren Günseli

Episodic memory retrieval is crucial for survival and can be impaired by divided attention. However, since memory retrieval consists of different stages, divided attention can impair each stage uniquely, leading to retrieval failures. It is important to acknowledge the multistage characteristics of episodic memory retrieval to understand the cognitive mechanisms that mediate the relationship between memory retrieval and divided attention. Here we attempt to unravel the role of divided attention in gating the access to long-term memories through its unique impact on a non-exhaustive list of six stages of a memory retrieval task: processing the retrieval cue, initiating a retrieval mode, searching for the target memory, reactivating the target LTM in WM, deciding on the accuracy of the retrieved content, and motor preparation to produce a response We describe how each stage might be affected by divided attention. To do so, we review not only studies on memory retrieval but also areas that constitute different stages described above given the lack of extensive research that explores the memory retrieval stages distinctively and the role of attention for each stage. We hope this work will contribute to carefully controlling and manipulating how different stages are affected by attention, which in turn will improve our understanding of the relationship between attention and memory retrieval.

情景记忆提取对生存至关重要,并且会因注意力分散而受损。然而,由于记忆提取由不同的阶段组成,注意力分散会对每个阶段造成独特的损害,从而导致记忆提取失败。认识情景记忆提取的多阶段特征,对理解情景记忆提取与注意力分离关系的认知机制具有重要意义。在这里,我们试图通过其对记忆检索任务的非详尽的六个阶段的独特影响来揭示分散注意在控制对长期记忆的访问中的作用:处理检索线索,启动检索模式,搜索目标记忆,在WM中重新激活目标LTM,决定检索内容的准确性,以及产生反应的运动准备。我们描述了分散注意如何影响每个阶段。为了做到这一点,我们不仅回顾了记忆检索的研究,而且还回顾了构成上述不同阶段的领域,因为缺乏广泛的研究来探索记忆检索的不同阶段以及每个阶段的注意作用。我们希望这项工作将有助于仔细控制和操纵不同阶段如何受到注意的影响,从而提高我们对注意和记忆检索之间关系的理解。
{"title":"How does divided attention hinder different stages of episodic memory retrieval?","authors":"Nursena Ataseven ,&nbsp;Nursima Ünver ,&nbsp;Eren Günseli","doi":"10.1016/j.crbeha.2023.100139","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.crbeha.2023.100139","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Episodic memory retrieval is crucial for survival and can be impaired by divided attention. However, since memory retrieval consists of different stages, divided attention can impair each stage uniquely, leading to retrieval failures. It is important to acknowledge the multistage characteristics of episodic memory retrieval to understand the cognitive mechanisms that mediate the relationship between memory retrieval and divided attention. Here we attempt to unravel the role of divided attention in gating the access to long-term memories through its unique impact on a non-exhaustive list of six stages of a memory retrieval task: processing the retrieval cue, initiating a retrieval mode, searching for the target memory, reactivating the target LTM in WM, deciding on the accuracy of the retrieved content, and motor preparation to produce a response We describe how each stage might be affected by divided attention. To do so, we review not only studies on memory retrieval but also areas that constitute different stages described above given the lack of extensive research that explores the memory retrieval stages distinctively and the role of attention for each stage. We hope this work will contribute to carefully controlling and manipulating how different stages are affected by attention, which in turn will improve our understanding of the relationship between attention and memory retrieval.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":72746,"journal":{"name":"Current research in behavioral sciences","volume":"5 ","pages":"Article 100139"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S2666518223000438/pdfft?md5=3beaef4f161f4e37f311fbec5d9673ef&pid=1-s2.0-S2666518223000438-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"92046609","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Maternal cognitions and cognitive, behavior and emotional development in middle childhood 母亲认知与儿童中期的认知、行为和情感发展
Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2023-01-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.crbeha.2023.100098
Carolina Álvarez, Dénes Szücs

Objectives

Maternal cognitions influence the mother-child relationship, and therefore children's development. However, some relationships between maternal cognitions and children's outcomes have yet to be investigated and most studies have been carried out in Western countries. We analysed how three maternal cognitions (depression, parenting stress, and parental sense of competence) were related to children's behavioural and emotional problems; self-esteem and bullying; math achievement, receptive vocabulary, and executive functions in a Latin American context.

Methods

Participants were 10,867 mother-child dyads: a nationally representative sample of children aged 7 to 12 years old (M age = 10.1 years; SD = 1.3; 49.16% girls). Data was collected in 2017 through home visits by the Early Childhood Longitudinal Survey from Chile. The analyses we performed were descriptive, frequency, correlations, and multivariate regression. Our large, representative sample allowed us to estimate effect sizes precisely and the generalize of our results to a wide population.

Results

Novel findings indicated that maternal depression was negatively related to children's self-esteem; parenting stress was negatively related to self-esteem and positively related to being bullied; parental sense of competence was negatively related to experiencing bullying and positively related to self-esteem, math achievement, receptive vocabulary, and executive functions.

Conclusions

Parenting stress was the single predictor that impacted all outcome variables. Maternal depression was mostly related to mothers’ perception of their children's behavioural and emotional problems and children experiencing bullying. Parental sense of competence predicted a diverse range of children's outcomes. We discuss how results can inform prevention and intervention programs targeting child-mother dyads.

目的探讨母亲认知对母子关系的影响,进而影响儿童的发展。然而,母亲认知与儿童结局之间的一些关系尚未得到调查,大多数研究都在西方国家进行。我们分析了三种母亲认知(抑郁、父母压力和父母能力感)与儿童行为和情绪问题的关系;自尊和欺凌;拉丁美洲语境下的数学成绩、接受性词汇和执行功能。方法研究对象为10,867对母子:7 - 12岁儿童(M年龄= 10.1岁;sd = 1.3;49.16%的女孩)。2017年,智利幼儿纵向调查通过家访收集了数据。我们进行了描述性分析、频率分析、相关性分析和多元回归分析。我们的大型代表性样本使我们能够准确地估计效应大小,并将我们的结果推广到更广泛的人群。结果新的研究结果表明,母亲抑郁与儿童自尊呈负相关;父母压力与自尊呈负相关,与被欺负呈正相关;父母的能力感与遭受欺凌负相关,与自尊、数学成绩、接受性词汇和执行功能正相关。结论父母压力是影响所有结局变量的单一预测因素。母亲抑郁主要与母亲对孩子行为和情绪问题的认知以及孩子遭受欺凌有关。父母的能力感预示着孩子的各种结果。我们讨论了结果如何为针对母子二人组的预防和干预计划提供信息。
{"title":"Maternal cognitions and cognitive, behavior and emotional development in middle childhood","authors":"Carolina Álvarez,&nbsp;Dénes Szücs","doi":"10.1016/j.crbeha.2023.100098","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.crbeha.2023.100098","url":null,"abstract":"<div><h3>Objectives</h3><p>Maternal cognitions influence the mother-child relationship, and therefore children's development. However, some relationships between maternal cognitions and children's outcomes have yet to be investigated and most studies have been carried out in Western countries. We analysed how three maternal cognitions (depression, parenting stress, and parental sense of competence) were related to children's behavioural and emotional problems; self-esteem and bullying; math achievement, receptive vocabulary, and executive functions in a Latin American context.</p></div><div><h3>Methods</h3><p>Participants were 10,867 mother-child dyads: a nationally representative sample of children aged 7 to 12 years old (<em>M</em> age = 10.1 years; <em>SD</em> = 1.3; 49.16% girls). Data was collected in 2017 through home visits by the Early Childhood Longitudinal Survey from Chile. The analyses we performed were descriptive, frequency, correlations, and multivariate regression. Our large, representative sample allowed us to estimate effect sizes precisely and the generalize of our results to a wide population.</p></div><div><h3>Results</h3><p>Novel findings indicated that maternal depression was negatively related to children's self-esteem; parenting stress was negatively related to self-esteem and positively related to being bullied; parental sense of competence was negatively related to experiencing bullying and positively related to self-esteem, math achievement, receptive vocabulary, and executive functions.</p></div><div><h3>Conclusions</h3><p>Parenting stress was the single predictor that impacted all outcome variables. Maternal depression was mostly related to mothers’ perception of their children's behavioural and emotional problems and children experiencing bullying. Parental sense of competence predicted a diverse range of children's outcomes. We discuss how results can inform prevention and intervention programs targeting child-mother dyads.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":72746,"journal":{"name":"Current research in behavioral sciences","volume":"4 ","pages":"Article 100098"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"45747105","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Correlates of Self-Assessed Optimism 自我评估乐观主义的相关性
Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2023-01-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.crbeha.2022.100089
Adrian Furnham , Charlotte Robinson

What are the bright- and dark-side personality trait, ideological belief, and mind-set correlates of self-assessed optimism? This paper reports on four studies, with a total N > 2000. In each, participants rated to what extent they were an optimist on an 8-point scale (high to low). We obtained demographic (age, sex) and ideological (political and religious beliefs) data in each study, as well as self-ratings on four variables (e.g., attractiveness, intelligence) which we aggregated and labelled self-esteem, which had alphas ranging from .70 to .80. We assessed personality, intelligence and other belief systems in different studies. Study 1 showed older, more religious, but less intelligent males with higher self-esteem and Belief in a Just World (BJW) were more optimistic. Study 2 showed older, more religious people, with higher self-esteem were more optimistic. Study 3 showed Open, Extraverted, Agreeable, Emotionally Stable, religious people with higher self-esteem and low on Negative Affectivity and Detachment, but high on Disinhibition, were most optimistic. Study 4 showed older, more religious people with higher self-esteem and lower Dweck fixed personality mindset beliefs were more optimistic. The concept and correlates of dispositional optimism and its measurement are discussed. Limitations and implications are noted.

什么是光明和黑暗的人格特质,意识形态信仰,和心态的自我评估乐观相关?本文报道了四项研究,共N >2000. 在每一项研究中,参与者对自己乐观的程度进行打分,满分为8分(从高到低)。我们在每项研究中都获得了人口统计(年龄、性别)和意识形态(政治和宗教信仰)数据,以及对四个变量(如吸引力、智力)的自我评分,我们将这些变量汇总并标记为自尊,其alpha值从0.70到0.80不等。我们在不同的研究中评估了个性、智力和其他信仰体系。研究1显示,年龄较大、信仰较多、但智商较低、自尊心强、相信世界公正的男性更乐观。研究2显示,年龄越大、信仰越多、自尊心越强的人更乐观。研究3显示,性格开放、外向、随和、情绪稳定、有宗教信仰、自尊程度较高、消极情感和超然程度较低但去抑制程度较高的人最乐观。研究4显示,年龄较大、信仰较多、自尊程度较高、德韦克固定人格信念较低的人更乐观。讨论了性格乐观主义的概念、相关因素及其测量方法。指出了局限性和影响。
{"title":"Correlates of Self-Assessed Optimism","authors":"Adrian Furnham ,&nbsp;Charlotte Robinson","doi":"10.1016/j.crbeha.2022.100089","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.crbeha.2022.100089","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>What are the bright- and dark-side personality trait, ideological belief, and mind-set correlates of self-assessed optimism? This paper reports on four studies, with a total <em>N</em> &gt; 2000. In each, participants rated to what extent they were an optimist on an 8-point scale (high to low). We obtained demographic (age, sex) and ideological (political and religious beliefs) data in each study, as well as self-ratings on four variables (e.g., attractiveness, intelligence) which we aggregated and labelled self-esteem, which had alphas ranging from .70 to .80. We assessed personality, intelligence and other belief systems in different studies. Study 1 showed older, more religious, but less intelligent males with higher self-esteem and Belief in a Just World (BJW) were more optimistic. Study 2 showed older, more religious people, with higher self-esteem were more optimistic. Study 3 showed Open, Extraverted, Agreeable, Emotionally Stable, religious people with higher self-esteem and low on Negative Affectivity and Detachment, but high on Disinhibition, were most optimistic. Study 4 showed older, more religious people with higher self-esteem and lower Dweck fixed personality mindset beliefs were more optimistic. The concept and correlates of dispositional optimism and its measurement are discussed. Limitations and implications are noted.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":72746,"journal":{"name":"Current research in behavioral sciences","volume":"4 ","pages":"Article 100089"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"46479074","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
Loneliness and lack of belonging: The experiences of migrant professional footballers playing in Europe 孤独和缺乏归属感:移民职业足球运动员在欧洲踢球的经历
Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2023-01-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.crbeha.2023.100124
G Farrugia , A Muscat
{"title":"Loneliness and lack of belonging: The experiences of migrant professional footballers playing in Europe","authors":"G Farrugia ,&nbsp;A Muscat","doi":"10.1016/j.crbeha.2023.100124","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.crbeha.2023.100124","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":72746,"journal":{"name":"Current research in behavioral sciences","volume":"5 ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"49727442","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
What determines feelings of belonging and majoring in an academic field? Isolating factors by comparing psychology and philosophy 是什么决定了归属感和专业感?心理学与哲学比较中的隔离因素
Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2023-01-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.crbeha.2023.100097
Heather M. Maranges , Maxine Iannuccilli , Katharina Nieswandt , Ulf Hlobil , Kristen A. Dunfield

Feelings of belonging are integral in people's choice of what career to pursue. Women and men are disproportionately represented across careers, starting with academic training. The present research focuses on two fields that are similar in their history and subject matter but feature inverse gender gaps—psychology (more women than men) and philosophy (more men than women)—to investigate how theorized explanations for academic gender gaps contribute to feelings of belonging. Specifically, we simultaneously model the relative contribution of theoretically relevant individual differences (empathizing, systematizing, and intellectual combativeness) as well as life goals (prioritization of family, money, and status) to feelings of belonging and majoring in psychology or philosophy. We find that men report higher intellectual combativeness than women, and intellectual combativeness predicts feelings of belonging and majoring in philosophy over psychology. Although systematizing and empathizing are predictive of belonging and, in turn, majoring in psychology and philosophy, respectively, when other factors are taken into account, women and men do not differ in empathizing and systematizing. Women, more than men, report prioritizing having a family, wealth, and status in choosing a career, and these directly or indirectly feed into feelings of belonging and majoring in psychology, in contrast to prior theory. Together, these findings suggest that students’ perceptions of their own combativeness and the extent to which they desire money and status play essential roles in women's feeling they belong in psychology and men's feeling they belong in philosophy.

归属感是人们选择职业时不可或缺的一部分。从学术培训开始,女性和男性在各行各业的比例都不成比例。目前的研究集中在两个领域,这两个领域在历史和主题上都很相似,但却以相反的性别差距为特征——心理学(女性多于男性)和哲学(男性多于女性)——来调查对学术性别差距的理论解释是如何影响归属感的。具体来说,我们同时模拟了理论相关的个体差异(共情、系统化和智力竞争)以及生活目标(家庭、金钱和地位的优先次序)对归属感和心理学或哲学专业的相对贡献。我们发现男性比女性表现出更高的智力好斗性,而智力好斗性预示着归属感和主修哲学而非心理学。虽然系统化和共情是归属的预测因素,反过来,心理学和哲学专业分别是,当考虑到其他因素时,女性和男性在共情和系统化方面没有差异。与男性相比,女性在选择职业时更看重家庭、财富和地位。与之前的理论相反,这些因素直接或间接地影响了她们的归属感和主修心理学。综上所述,这些发现表明,学生们对自己好斗性的认知,以及他们对金钱和地位的渴望程度,在女性的心理学归属感和男性的哲学归属感中起着至关重要的作用。
{"title":"What determines feelings of belonging and majoring in an academic field? Isolating factors by comparing psychology and philosophy","authors":"Heather M. Maranges ,&nbsp;Maxine Iannuccilli ,&nbsp;Katharina Nieswandt ,&nbsp;Ulf Hlobil ,&nbsp;Kristen A. Dunfield","doi":"10.1016/j.crbeha.2023.100097","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.crbeha.2023.100097","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Feelings of belonging are integral in people's choice of what career to pursue. Women and men are disproportionately represented across careers, starting with academic training. The present research focuses on two fields that are similar in their history and subject matter but feature inverse gender gaps—psychology (more women than men) and philosophy (more men than women)—to investigate how theorized explanations for academic gender gaps contribute to feelings of belonging. Specifically, we simultaneously model the relative contribution of theoretically relevant individual differences (empathizing, systematizing, and intellectual combativeness) as well as life goals (prioritization of family, money, and status) to feelings of belonging and majoring in psychology or philosophy. We find that men report higher intellectual combativeness than women, and intellectual combativeness predicts feelings of belonging and majoring in philosophy over psychology. Although systematizing and empathizing are predictive of belonging and, in turn, majoring in psychology and philosophy, respectively, when other factors are taken into account, women and men do not differ in empathizing and systematizing. Women, more than men, report prioritizing having a family, wealth, and status in choosing a career, and these directly or indirectly feed into feelings of belonging and majoring in psychology, in contrast to prior theory. Together, these findings suggest that students’ perceptions of their own combativeness and the extent to which they desire money and status play essential roles in women's feeling they belong in psychology and men's feeling they belong in philosophy.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":72746,"journal":{"name":"Current research in behavioral sciences","volume":"4 ","pages":"Article 100097"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41450883","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
A 2-hit model of early life stress and later life restraint stress: Susceptibility or resilience to anxiety and alcohol drinking? 早期生活压力和后期生活克制压力的两击模型:对焦虑和饮酒的易感性或弹性?
Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2023-01-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.crbeha.2023.100118
Rosemary B. Bassey , Olubukola Kalejaiye , Brittain Waller , Ifeoluwa Bamidele , Marjorie C. Gondré-Lewis

Early life stress adversely influences neurodevelopment and has profound long-term effects on brain function and behavior. Here we measure endogenous stress hormone and determine locomotor, anxiogenic, and operant binge drinking behavior of rats exposed to a 2-hit model of maternal deprivation (MD) stress in infancy followed by acute (ARS) or chronic restraint stress (CRS) paradigms during adolescence/adulthood to determine resiliency or susceptibility to the second stress exposure. Adolescents (n=40) exhibited higher baseline corticosterone (CORT) levels than adults (n=40) and with CRS exposure, showed significantly elevated circulating CORT irrespective of MD exposure status. In adults, MD females had lower baseline CORT levels than controls, whereas MD and control male CORT were indistinguishable. For behavior, N=40 was used in total. In the open field, locomotion was significantly decreased upon ARS, with MD having a significant sustained impact on ambulatory measures with CRS. When tested for anxiety-like behavior on an elevated zero maze, ARS interacted with MD to induce protracted and more profound anxiogenic behavior. Upon CRS, MD anxiogenic behavior was reversed to baseline levels whereas controls, especially males, exceeded baseline levels to spend significantly more time in the open arms of the EZM. Finally, in testing for binge drinking behavior, MD rats lever-pressed significantly more for 10% ethanol on an operant apparatus and exhibited a 3-fold greater drinking baseline than controls. Binge drinking behavior was further elevated with ARS, but quickly returned to the MD baseline even prior to reaching CRS. By contrast, controls had much lower baseline but significantly elevated binge drinking behavior by 3–5-fold in response to ARS which remained elevated through CRS, stabilizing at MD baseline levels. Sex differences were most evident in controls where males exhibited accelerated binge drinking behavior at consistently higher rates than females until Day 21 of CRS where their binge drinking behaviors were similar. There was also a main effect of alcohol binge behavior for MD males, and not females. This study illustrates that, depending on the biobehavioral output, males and females differ in reactivity to stressors where MD stress may confer sex-dependent resilience to subsequent stressors, and males are differently impacted than females.

早期生活压力会对神经发育产生不利影响,并对大脑功能和行为产生深远的长期影响。在此,我们测量了内源性应激激素,并测定了婴儿期暴露于母亲剥夺(MD)应激的大鼠的运动、焦虑和操作性酗酒行为,然后在青春期/成年期暴露于急性(ARS)或慢性约束应激(CRS)范式,以确定对第二次应激暴露的弹性或易感性。青少年(n=40)表现出比成人(n=40)更高的基线皮质酮(CORT)水平,而CRS暴露后,无论MD暴露状况如何,循环CORT均显著升高。在成人中,MD女性的基线CORT水平低于对照组,而MD和对照组男性的CORT没有区别。对于行为,总共使用N=40。在开阔场地,ARS时运动明显减少,MD对CRS时的动态测量有显著的持续影响。当在高架零迷宫中测试焦虑样行为时,ARS与MD相互作用诱导持久和更深刻的焦虑行为。在CRS中,MD焦虑行为被逆转到基线水平,而对照组,特别是男性,超过基线水平,在EZM张开的怀抱中花费更多的时间。最后,在狂饮行为的测试中,MD大鼠在操作装置上对10%乙醇的杠杆按压明显更多,并且显示出比对照组高3倍的饮酒基线。狂饮行为随着ARS进一步升高,但甚至在达到CRS之前就迅速恢复到MD基线。相比之下,对照组的基线水平要低得多,但对ARS的反应却显著提高了3 - 5倍,ARS通过CRS保持升高,稳定在MD基线水平。性别差异在对照组中最为明显,在CRS的第21天,男性的酗酒行为加速率一直高于女性,而他们的酗酒行为相似。酒精暴饮行为也对MD男性有主要影响,而对女性没有。该研究表明,根据生物行为输出,男性和女性对压力源的反应性不同,其中MD压力可能赋予对后续压力源的性别依赖的恢复力,并且男性受到的影响不同于女性。
{"title":"A 2-hit model of early life stress and later life restraint stress: Susceptibility or resilience to anxiety and alcohol drinking?","authors":"Rosemary B. Bassey ,&nbsp;Olubukola Kalejaiye ,&nbsp;Brittain Waller ,&nbsp;Ifeoluwa Bamidele ,&nbsp;Marjorie C. Gondré-Lewis","doi":"10.1016/j.crbeha.2023.100118","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.crbeha.2023.100118","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Early life stress adversely influences neurodevelopment and has profound long-term effects on brain function and behavior. Here we measure endogenous stress hormone and determine locomotor, anxiogenic, and operant binge drinking behavior of rats exposed to a 2-hit model of maternal deprivation (MD) stress in infancy followed by acute (ARS) or chronic restraint stress (CRS) paradigms during adolescence/adulthood to determine resiliency or susceptibility to the second stress exposure. Adolescents (<em>n</em>=40) exhibited higher baseline corticosterone (CORT) levels than adults (<em>n</em>=40) and with CRS exposure, showed significantly elevated circulating CORT irrespective of MD exposure status. In adults, MD females had lower baseline CORT levels than controls, whereas MD and control male CORT were indistinguishable. For behavior, <em>N</em>=40 was used in total. In the open field, locomotion was significantly decreased upon ARS, with MD having a significant sustained impact on ambulatory measures with CRS. When tested for anxiety-like behavior on an elevated zero maze, ARS interacted with MD to induce protracted and more profound anxiogenic behavior. Upon CRS, MD anxiogenic behavior was reversed to baseline levels whereas controls, especially males, exceeded baseline levels to spend significantly more time in the open arms of the EZM. Finally, in testing for binge drinking behavior, MD rats lever-pressed significantly more for 10% ethanol on an operant apparatus and exhibited a 3-fold greater drinking baseline than controls. Binge drinking behavior was further elevated with ARS, but quickly returned to the MD baseline even prior to reaching CRS. By contrast, controls had much lower baseline but significantly elevated binge drinking behavior by 3–5-fold in response to ARS which remained elevated through CRS, stabilizing at MD baseline levels. Sex differences were most evident in controls where males exhibited accelerated binge drinking behavior at consistently higher rates than females until Day 21 of CRS where their binge drinking behaviors were similar. There was also a main effect of alcohol binge behavior for MD males, and not females. This study illustrates that, depending on the biobehavioral output, males and females differ in reactivity to stressors where MD stress may confer sex-dependent resilience to subsequent stressors, and males are differently impacted than females.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":72746,"journal":{"name":"Current research in behavioral sciences","volume":"5 ","pages":"Article 100118"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"44415203","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Changing face contours reduces the inversion effect and overall recognition performance 改变人脸轮廓会降低反演效果和整体识别性能
Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2023-01-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.crbeha.2023.100115
Siobhan McCourt, I.P.L. McLaren, Ciro Civile

This paper investigates how manipulating the face contour would systematically influence the face inversion effect (i.e., better recognition performance for upright vs inverted upside-down faces) and overall face recognition. Experiment 1 (n=144) addressed the question of whether manipulating the face contour would affect the inversion effect for scrambled faces which have disrupted configural information. Our results revealed that blurring the face contour significantly reduced the inversion effect by means of impaired performance for upright scrambled faces. Experiment 2a (n=144) and Experiment 2b (n=144) examined how either blurring the contour or replacing it with a new contour would influence the inversion effect for normal faces. These results confirmed a reduction of the inversion effect mainly due to impaired performance for upright faces. A reduction in overall recognition performance was also recorded for normal faces with a manipulated contour. Experiment 3 (n=144) manipulated the contour of New Thatcherized faces which suffer from partial configural information disruption. The results revealed no influence on the inversion effect but a significant reduction in overall recognition performance. Taken together, our results suggest that face contour information can have an impact in influencing both the inversion effect and overall recognition performance.

本文研究了对人脸轮廓的处理如何系统地影响人脸反转效果(即对直立和倒立的人脸有更好的识别性能)和整体人脸识别。实验1 (n=144)解决了对打乱了构型信息的打乱后的人脸进行轮廓处理是否会影响反演效果的问题。我们的研究结果表明,模糊的人脸轮廓显著降低了反演效果的表现受损直立打乱的人脸。实验2a (n=144)和实验2b (n=144)考察了模糊轮廓或用新轮廓替换轮廓对正常人脸的反演效果的影响。这些结果证实了反转效应的降低主要是由于直立面部的性能受损。对于轮廓经过处理的普通人脸,总体识别性能也有所下降。实验3 (n=144)对部分构型信息中断的新撒切尔化面孔进行轮廓处理。结果显示,对反转效果没有影响,但整体识别性能显著降低。综上所述,我们的研究结果表明,人脸轮廓信息对反演效果和整体识别性能都有影响。
{"title":"Changing face contours reduces the inversion effect and overall recognition performance","authors":"Siobhan McCourt,&nbsp;I.P.L. McLaren,&nbsp;Ciro Civile","doi":"10.1016/j.crbeha.2023.100115","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.crbeha.2023.100115","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>This paper investigates how manipulating the face contour would systematically influence the face inversion effect (i.e., better recognition performance for upright vs inverted upside-down faces) and overall face recognition. Experiment 1 (n=144) addressed the question of whether manipulating the face contour would affect the inversion effect for scrambled faces which have disrupted configural information. Our results revealed that blurring the face contour significantly reduced the inversion effect by means of impaired performance for upright scrambled faces. Experiment 2a (n=144) and Experiment 2b (n=144) examined how either blurring the contour or replacing it with a new contour would influence the inversion effect for normal faces. These results confirmed a reduction of the inversion effect mainly due to impaired performance for upright faces. A reduction in overall recognition performance was also recorded for normal faces with a manipulated contour. Experiment 3 (n=144) manipulated the contour of New Thatcherized faces which suffer from partial configural information disruption. The results revealed no influence on the inversion effect but a significant reduction in overall recognition performance. Taken together, our results suggest that face contour information can have an impact in influencing both the inversion effect and overall recognition performance.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":72746,"journal":{"name":"Current research in behavioral sciences","volume":"4 ","pages":"Article 100115"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"42464619","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Protective roles of empathy and compassion against loneliness early in the SARS-CoV-2 pandemic 移情和同情在严重急性呼吸系统综合征冠状病毒2型大流行早期对孤独的保护作用
Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2023-01-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.crbeha.2023.100130
Melissa M. Karnaze , Cinnamon S. Bloss

Importance

The SARS-CoV-2 pandemic has caused concern of an impending “second wave” of loneliness and mental health problems following social disruption that included mandates to physically and socially distance from others. Research is needed to identify factors that can mitigate loneliness, which results from perceptions that one's social connections are less than desirable.

Objective

We examined whether individual differences in empathy and compassion would predict loneliness in a large sample of adults surveyed early in the SARS-CoV-2 pandemic controlling for social desirability and loneliness risk factors.

Design

Using a cross-sequential design, a baseline survey was fielded at seven time points (every two weeks) to seven different cohorts from March 22 to June 15, 2020. For each cohort, a follow-up survey was fielded one month after each baseline survey.

Participants

Participants who comprised the seven cohorts were recruited from two U.S. convenience samples, Amazon Mechanical Turk and Qualtrics Online Panels. The response rate across the follow-up surveys was 59% (total N = 3262).

Main outcomes and measures

We hypothesized that dispositional empathy and compassion at baseline would be associated with lower levels of loneliness at one-month follow-up.

Results

Higher empathy at baseline (Beta = -0.03; 95% CI -0.05 to -0.02; P < .001) was associated with lower loneliness at follow-up, even after controlling for social desirability and other loneliness risk factors. Higher compassion at baseline (Beta = -0.03; 95% CI -0.05 to -0.01; P < .001) was also associated with lower loneliness at follow-up.

Conclusion and relevance

We found that early in the SARS-CoV-2 pandemic, higher levels of empathy and compassion predicted lower levels of loneliness one month later. Future research, including experimental designs, should examine whether increasing empathy, or the tendency to share others’ positive and negative feelings, can confer a protective effect against perceived social isolation during collective trauma characterized by physical and social distancing.

SARS-CoV-2大流行引发了人们对即将到来的“第二波”孤独和心理健康问题的担忧,此前的社会破坏包括要求与他人保持身体和社交距离。需要进行研究以确定可以减轻孤独感的因素,孤独感是由于人们认为自己的社会关系不太理想而产生的。目的研究在SARS-CoV-2大流行早期接受调查的大量成年人样本中,同理心和同情心的个体差异是否能预测孤独感,控制社会期望和孤独感风险因素。采用交叉序列设计,从2020年3月22日至6月15日,在七个时间点(每两周)对七个不同的队列进行基线调查。对于每个队列,在每次基线调查后一个月进行随访调查。组成七个队列的参与者是从两个美国便利样本中招募的,亚马逊土耳其机器人和Qualtrics在线小组。随访调查的应答率为59%(总N = 3262)。我们假设,在一个月的随访中,基线时的性格共情和同情心与较低的孤独感水平有关。结果基线共情水平较高(Beta = -0.03;95% CI为-0.05 ~ -0.02;P < .001)与随访时较低的孤独感相关,即使在控制了社会可取性和其他孤独感风险因素后也是如此。基线时更高的同情心(Beta = -0.03;95% CI为-0.05 ~ -0.01;P < .001)也与随访时较低的孤独感相关。结论和相关性我们发现,在SARS-CoV-2大流行的早期,较高的同理心和同情心预示着一个月后较低的孤独感。未来的研究,包括实验设计,应该检查增加同理心,或分享他人积极和消极感受的倾向,是否能在以身体和社会距离为特征的集体创伤中,对感知到的社会孤立产生保护作用。
{"title":"Protective roles of empathy and compassion against loneliness early in the SARS-CoV-2 pandemic","authors":"Melissa M. Karnaze ,&nbsp;Cinnamon S. Bloss","doi":"10.1016/j.crbeha.2023.100130","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.crbeha.2023.100130","url":null,"abstract":"<div><h3>Importance</h3><p>The SARS-CoV-2 pandemic has caused concern of an impending “second wave” of loneliness and mental health problems following social disruption that included mandates to physically and socially distance from others. Research is needed to identify factors that can mitigate loneliness, which results from perceptions that one's social connections are less than desirable.</p></div><div><h3>Objective</h3><p>We examined whether individual differences in empathy and compassion would predict loneliness in a large sample of adults surveyed early in the SARS-CoV-2 pandemic controlling for social desirability and loneliness risk factors.</p></div><div><h3>Design</h3><p>Using a cross-sequential design, a baseline survey was fielded at seven time points (every two weeks) to seven different cohorts from March 22 to June 15, 2020. For each cohort, a follow-up survey was fielded one month after each baseline survey.</p></div><div><h3>Participants</h3><p>Participants who comprised the seven cohorts were recruited from two U.S. convenience samples, Amazon Mechanical Turk and Qualtrics Online Panels. The response rate across the follow-up surveys was 59% (total <em>N</em> = 3262).</p></div><div><h3>Main outcomes and measures</h3><p>We hypothesized that dispositional empathy and compassion at baseline would be associated with lower levels of loneliness at one-month follow-up.</p></div><div><h3>Results</h3><p>Higher empathy at baseline (<em>Beta</em> = -0.03; 95% CI -0.05 to -0.02; <em>P</em> &lt; .001) was associated with lower loneliness at follow-up, even after controlling for social desirability and other loneliness risk factors. Higher compassion at baseline (<em>Beta</em> = -0.03; 95% CI -0.05 to -0.01; <em>P</em> &lt; .001) was also associated with lower loneliness at follow-up.</p></div><div><h3>Conclusion and relevance</h3><p>We found that early in the SARS-CoV-2 pandemic, higher levels of empathy and compassion predicted lower levels of loneliness one month later. Future research, including experimental designs, should examine whether increasing empathy, or the tendency to share others’ positive and negative feelings, can confer a protective effect against perceived social isolation during collective trauma characterized by physical and social distancing.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":72746,"journal":{"name":"Current research in behavioral sciences","volume":"5 ","pages":"Article 100130"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"42118784","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The relation between self-reported healthy living and attentional engagement in everyday life 自我报告的健康生活与日常生活中注意力投入的关系
Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2023-01-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.crbeha.2022.100086
Tyler B. Kruger , Jeremy Marty-Dugas , Brandon C.W. Ralph , Mike J. Dixon , Daniel Smilek

We examined how three different components of self-reported healthy living—physical activity, sleepiness, and dietary habits—relate to self-reported inattention and deep, effortless concentration (i.e., “flow”) in everyday life via two online surveys (N = 171 and N = 172). Our results indicated that sedentariness was associated with less frequent self-reported experiences of flow while sleepiness was associated with inattention in general (i.e., greater self-reported attention-related cognitive errors, attention lapses, and mind-wandering) as well as less frequent self-reported experiences of flow. Additionally, unhealthy dietary habits were associated with more lapses of attention and attention-related cognitive errors and less frequent self-reported experiences of flow. Most importantly, the measures of healthy living collectively accounted for a significant and substantial portion of the variance in each attention measure after controlling for age (up to 29% in Sample One and 34% in Sample Two). These results indicate a strong relation between healthy living and attentional engagement and raise the tantalizing possibility that an increase in healthy living may substantially improve attentional engagement in everyday life.

通过两项在线调查(N = 171和N = 172),我们研究了自我报告的健康生活的三个不同组成部分——体育活动、嗜睡和饮食习惯——与自我报告的日常生活中的注意力不集中和深度、轻松集中(即“心流”)之间的关系。我们的研究结果表明,久坐与自我报告的心流体验频率较低有关,而嗜睡则与注意力不集中(即,更大的自我报告的与注意力相关的认知错误、注意力缺失和走神)以及自我报告的心流体验频率较低有关。此外,不健康的饮食习惯与更多的注意力缺失和与注意力相关的认知错误以及更少的自我报告的心流体验有关。最重要的是,在控制了年龄之后,健康生活的措施在每个注意力测量中占了显著和实质性的部分(样本一中高达29%,样本二中高达34%)。这些结果表明,健康的生活方式和注意力投入之间有很强的关系,并提出了一种诱人的可能性,即健康生活方式的增加可能会大大提高日常生活中的注意力投入。
{"title":"The relation between self-reported healthy living and attentional engagement in everyday life","authors":"Tyler B. Kruger ,&nbsp;Jeremy Marty-Dugas ,&nbsp;Brandon C.W. Ralph ,&nbsp;Mike J. Dixon ,&nbsp;Daniel Smilek","doi":"10.1016/j.crbeha.2022.100086","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.crbeha.2022.100086","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>We examined how three different components of self-reported healthy living—physical activity, sleepiness, and dietary habits—relate to self-reported inattention and deep, effortless concentration (i.e., “flow”) in everyday life via two online surveys (N = 171 and N = 172). Our results indicated that sedentariness was associated with less frequent self-reported experiences of flow while sleepiness was associated with inattention in general (i.e., greater self-reported attention-related cognitive errors, attention lapses, and mind-wandering) as well as less frequent self-reported experiences of flow. Additionally, unhealthy dietary habits were associated with more lapses of attention and attention-related cognitive errors and less frequent self-reported experiences of flow. Most importantly, the measures of healthy living collectively accounted for a significant and substantial portion of the variance in each attention measure after controlling for age (up to 29% in Sample One and 34% in Sample Two). These results indicate a strong relation between healthy living and attentional engagement and raise the tantalizing possibility that an increase in healthy living may substantially improve attentional engagement in everyday life.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":72746,"journal":{"name":"Current research in behavioral sciences","volume":"4 ","pages":"Article 100086"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"45274505","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
Short communication: The development of loneliness across the transition from primary to secondary school 短沟通:孤独感在小学到中学的发展
Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2023-01-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.crbeha.2023.100123
Flore Geukens , Susanne Buecker , Wim Van den Noortgate , Patricia Bijttebier , Guy Bosmans , Karla Van Leeuwen , Luc Goossens
{"title":"Short communication: The development of loneliness across the transition from primary to secondary school","authors":"Flore Geukens ,&nbsp;Susanne Buecker ,&nbsp;Wim Van den Noortgate ,&nbsp;Patricia Bijttebier ,&nbsp;Guy Bosmans ,&nbsp;Karla Van Leeuwen ,&nbsp;Luc Goossens","doi":"10.1016/j.crbeha.2023.100123","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.crbeha.2023.100123","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":72746,"journal":{"name":"Current research in behavioral sciences","volume":"5 ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"49754048","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Current research in behavioral sciences
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1