首页 > 最新文献

American Journal of Primatology最新文献

英文 中文
Update of the geographic range of Humboldt's squirrel monkeys (Saimiri cassiquiarensis cassiquiarensis Lesson, 1840) using a model-based approach 利用基于模型的方法更新洪堡松鼠猴(Saimiri cassiquiarensis cassiquiarensis Lesson,1840 年)的地理分布范围。
IF 2.4 3区 生物学 Q1 ZOOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-04-01 DOI: 10.1002/ajp.23625
Ítalo Mourthé, Fernanda P. Paim, Michelle P. Mercês, João Valsecchi, Rafael M. Rabelo

Saimiri cassiquiarensis cassiquiarensis (Cebidae) is a primate subspecies with a wide distribution in the Amazonian region of Brazil, Colombia, and Venezuela. However, the boundaries of its geographic range remain poorly defined. This study presents new occurrence localities for this subspecies and updates its distribution using a compiled data set of 140 occurrence records based on literature, specimens vouchered in scientific collections, and new field data to produce model-based range maps. After cleaning our data set, we updated the subspecies' extent of occurrence, which was used in model calibration. We then modeled the subspecies' range using a maximum entropy algorithm (MaxEnt). The final model was adjusted using a fixed threshold, and we revised this polygon based on known geographic barriers and parapatric congeneric ranges. Our findings indicate that this subspecies is strongly associated with lowland areas, with consistently high daily temperatures. We propose modifications to all range boundaries and estimate that 3% of the area of occupancy (AOO, as defined by IUCN) has already been lost due to deforestation, resulting in a current range of 224,469 km2. We also found that 54% of their AOO is currently covered by protected areas (PAs). Based on these results, we consider that this subspecies is currently properly classified as Least Concern, because it occupies an extensive range, which is relatively well covered by PAs, and is currently experiencing low rates of deforestation.

Saimiri cassiquiarensis cassiquiarensis(宿兔科)是一个灵长类亚种,广泛分布于巴西、哥伦比亚和委内瑞拉的亚马逊地区。然而,其地理分布范围的边界仍未得到很好的界定。本研究介绍了该亚种新的出现地点,并利用根据文献、科学收藏的标本和新的野外数据编制的 140 个出现记录数据集更新了该亚种的分布,从而绘制了基于模型的分布图。清理数据集后,我们更新了亚种的出现范围,并将其用于模型校准。然后,我们使用最大熵算法(MaxEnt)对亚种的分布范围进行建模。我们使用固定阈值对最终模型进行了调整,并根据已知的地理障碍和同域同种分布范围对这一多边形进行了修订。我们的研究结果表明,该亚种与低洼地区密切相关,这些地区的日温度持续较高。我们建议修改所有的分布区边界,并估计由于森林砍伐,3%的栖息地(AOO,由世界自然保护联盟定义)已经消失,因此目前的分布区为 224 469 平方公里。我们还发现,它们 54% 的栖息地目前被保护区覆盖。基于这些结果,我们认为该亚种目前应被列为 "最不关注 "物种,因为它的分布范围很广,保护区的覆盖率相对较高,而且目前的森林砍伐率较低。
{"title":"Update of the geographic range of Humboldt's squirrel monkeys (Saimiri cassiquiarensis cassiquiarensis Lesson, 1840) using a model-based approach","authors":"Ítalo Mourthé,&nbsp;Fernanda P. Paim,&nbsp;Michelle P. Mercês,&nbsp;João Valsecchi,&nbsp;Rafael M. Rabelo","doi":"10.1002/ajp.23625","DOIUrl":"10.1002/ajp.23625","url":null,"abstract":"<p><i>Saimiri cassiquiarensis cassiquiarensis</i> (Cebidae) is a primate subspecies with a wide distribution in the Amazonian region of Brazil, Colombia, and Venezuela. However, the boundaries of its geographic range remain poorly defined. This study presents new occurrence localities for this subspecies and updates its distribution using a compiled data set of 140 occurrence records based on literature, specimens vouchered in scientific collections, and new field data to produce model-based range maps. After cleaning our data set, we updated the subspecies' extent of occurrence, which was used in model calibration. We then modeled the subspecies' range using a maximum entropy algorithm (MaxEnt). The final model was adjusted using a fixed threshold, and we revised this polygon based on known geographic barriers and parapatric congeneric ranges. Our findings indicate that this subspecies is strongly associated with lowland areas, with consistently high daily temperatures. We propose modifications to all range boundaries and estimate that 3% of the area of occupancy (AOO, as defined by IUCN) has already been lost due to deforestation, resulting in a current range of 224,469 km<sup>2</sup>. We also found that 54% of their AOO is currently covered by protected areas (PAs). Based on these results, we consider that this subspecies is currently properly classified as Least Concern, because it occupies an extensive range, which is relatively well covered by PAs, and is currently experiencing low rates of deforestation.</p>","PeriodicalId":7662,"journal":{"name":"American Journal of Primatology","volume":"86 7","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.4,"publicationDate":"2024-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140334383","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The establishment of a collaborative surveillance program with indigenous hunters to characterize primate health in Southern Guyana 与土著狩猎者建立合作监测计划,以了解圭亚那南部灵长类动物的健康状况。
IF 2.4 3区 生物学 Q1 ZOOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-04-01 DOI: 10.1002/ajp.23622
Marissa S. Milstein, Christopher A. Shaffer, Phillip Suse, Elisha Marawanaru, Romel Shoni, Steven Suse, Bemner Issacs, Peter A. Larsen, Dominic A. Travis, Karen A. Terio, Tiffany M. Wolf

The consumption of primates is integral to the traditional subsistence strategies of many Indigenous communities throughout Amazonia. Understanding the overall health of primates harvested for food in the region is critical to Indigenous food security and thus, these communities are highly invested in long-term primate population health. Here, we describe the establishment of a surveillance comanagement program among the Waiwai, an Indigenous community in the Konashen Amerindian Protected Area (KAPA). To assess primate health in the KAPA, hunters performed field necropsies on primates harvested for food and tissues collected from these individuals were analyzed using histopathology. From 2015 to 2019, hunters conducted 127 necropsies across seven species of primates. Of this sample, 82 primates (between 2015 and 2017) were submitted for histopathological screening. Our histopathology data revealed that KAPA primates had little evidence of underlying disease. Of the tissue abnormalities observed, the majority were either due to diet (e.g., hepatocellular pigment), degenerative changes resulting from aging (e.g., interstitial nephritis, myocyte lipofusion), or nonspecific responses to antigenic stimulation (renal and splenic lymphoid hyperplasia). In our sample, 7.32% of individuals had abnormalities that were consistent with a viral etiology, including myocarditis and hepatitis. Internal parasites were observed in 53.66% of individuals and is consistent with what would be expected from a free-ranging primate population. This study represents the importance of baseline data for long-term monitoring of primate populations hunted for food. More broadly, this research begins to close a critical gap in zoonotic disease risk related to primate harvesting in Amazonia, while also demonstrating the benefits of partnering with Indigenous hunters and leveraging hunting practices in disease surveillance and primate population health assessment.

食用灵长类动物是整个亚马逊地区许多土著社区传统生存战略中不可或缺的一部分。了解该地区作为食物捕获的灵长类动物的总体健康状况对原住民的粮食安全至关重要,因此,这些社区对灵长类动物的长期健康状况投入了大量精力。在此,我们介绍了在科纳申美洲印第安人保护区(KAPA)的一个土著社区怀瓦伊(Waiwai)中建立监测共同管理计划的情况。为了评估科纳申美洲印第安人保护区灵长类动物的健康状况,狩猎者对采集为食的灵长类动物进行了野外尸体解剖,并对从这些个体身上采集的组织进行了组织病理学分析。从 2015 年到 2019 年,猎人对七种灵长类动物进行了 127 次尸体解剖。其中,82 只灵长类动物(2015 年至 2017 年)被提交进行组织病理学筛查。我们的组织病理学数据显示,KAPA灵长类动物几乎没有潜在疾病的证据。在观察到的组织异常中,大多数是由于饮食(如肝细胞色素)、衰老引起的退行性变化(如间质性肾炎、肌细胞脂质融合)或对抗原刺激的非特异性反应(肾脏和脾脏淋巴细胞增生)。在我们的样本中,7.32%的患者出现了与病毒病因一致的异常,包括心肌炎和肝炎。在 53.66% 的个体中观察到体内寄生虫,这与自由活动的灵长类群体的预期相符。这项研究表明,基线数据对于长期监测以捕食为生的灵长类种群非常重要。更广泛地说,这项研究开始填补亚马逊地区与灵长类动物捕猎有关的人畜共患疾病风险方面的一个重要空白,同时也证明了与土著猎人合作并利用狩猎实践进行疾病监测和灵长类动物种群健康评估的益处。
{"title":"The establishment of a collaborative surveillance program with indigenous hunters to characterize primate health in Southern Guyana","authors":"Marissa S. Milstein,&nbsp;Christopher A. Shaffer,&nbsp;Phillip Suse,&nbsp;Elisha Marawanaru,&nbsp;Romel Shoni,&nbsp;Steven Suse,&nbsp;Bemner Issacs,&nbsp;Peter A. Larsen,&nbsp;Dominic A. Travis,&nbsp;Karen A. Terio,&nbsp;Tiffany M. Wolf","doi":"10.1002/ajp.23622","DOIUrl":"10.1002/ajp.23622","url":null,"abstract":"<p>The consumption of primates is integral to the traditional subsistence strategies of many Indigenous communities throughout Amazonia. Understanding the overall health of primates harvested for food in the region is critical to Indigenous food security and thus, these communities are highly invested in long-term primate population health. Here, we describe the establishment of a surveillance comanagement program among the Waiwai, an Indigenous community in the Konashen Amerindian Protected Area (KAPA). To assess primate health in the KAPA, hunters performed field necropsies on primates harvested for food and tissues collected from these individuals were analyzed using histopathology. From 2015 to 2019, hunters conducted 127 necropsies across seven species of primates. Of this sample, 82 primates (between 2015 and 2017) were submitted for histopathological screening. Our histopathology data revealed that KAPA primates had little evidence of underlying disease. Of the tissue abnormalities observed, the majority were either due to diet (e.g., hepatocellular pigment), degenerative changes resulting from aging (e.g., interstitial nephritis, myocyte lipofusion), or nonspecific responses to antigenic stimulation (renal and splenic lymphoid hyperplasia). In our sample, 7.32% of individuals had abnormalities that were consistent with a viral etiology, including myocarditis and hepatitis. Internal parasites were observed in 53.66% of individuals and is consistent with what would be expected from a free-ranging primate population. This study represents the importance of baseline data for long-term monitoring of primate populations hunted for food. More broadly, this research begins to close a critical gap in zoonotic disease risk related to primate harvesting in Amazonia, while also demonstrating the benefits of partnering with Indigenous hunters and leveraging hunting practices in disease surveillance and primate population health assessment.</p>","PeriodicalId":7662,"journal":{"name":"American Journal of Primatology","volume":"86 6","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.4,"publicationDate":"2024-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/ajp.23622","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140334382","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Influence of food physical properties and environmental context on manipulative behaviors highlighted by new methodological approaches in zoo-housed bonobos (Pan paniscus) 动物园饲养倭黑猩猩(Pan paniscus)的新方法强调了食物物理特性和环境背景对操作行为的影响。
IF 2.4 3区 生物学 Q1 ZOOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-03-28 DOI: 10.1002/ajp.23624
Caroline Gérard, Ameline Bardo, Jean Pascal Guéry, Emmanuelle Pouydebat, Victor Narat, Bruno Simmen

Research on manipulative abilities in nonhuman primates, in the context of hominid evolution, has mostly focused on manual/pedal postures considered as static behaviors. While these behavioral repertoires highlighted the range of manipulative abilities in many species, manipulation is a dynamic process that mostly involves successive types of grips before reaching its goal. The present study aims to investigate the use of manual/pedal postures in zoo-housed bonobos in diverse dynamic food processing by using an innovative approach: the optimal matching analysis that compares sequences (i.e., succession of grasping postures) with each other. To characterize the manipulative techniques spontaneously employed by bonobos, we performed this sequential analysis of manual/pedal postures during 766 complete feeding sequences of 17 individuals. We analyzed the effectiveness with a score defined by a partial proxy of food intake (i.e., the number of mouthfuls) linked to a handling score measuring both the diversity and changes of manual postures during each sequence. We identified four techniques, used differently depending on the physical substrate on which the individual performed food manipulation and the food physical properties. Our results showed that manipulative techniques were more complex (i.e., higher handling score) for large foods and on substrates with lower stability. But the effectiveness score was not significantly lower for these items since manipulative complexity seemed to be compensated by a greater number of mouthfuls. It appeared that the techniques employed involved a trade-off between manipulative complexity and the amount of food ingested. This study allowed us to test and validate innovative analysis methods that are applicable to diverse ethological studies involving sequential events. Our results bring new data for a better understanding of the evolution of manual abilities in primates in association with different ecological contexts and both terrestrial and arboreal substrates and suggest that social and individual influences need to be explored further.

在类人猿进化的背景下,对非人灵长类动物操纵能力的研究主要集中在被视为静态行为的手动/脚蹬姿势上。虽然这些行为剧目凸显了许多物种的操纵能力,但操纵是一个动态过程,在达到目的之前大多需要连续的抓握。本研究旨在通过一种创新方法:最佳匹配分析,比较序列(即连续的抓握姿势),研究动物园饲养的倭黑猩猩在各种动态食物加工过程中使用手动/脚踏姿势的情况。为了描述倭黑猩猩自发使用的操作技巧,我们对 17 只倭黑猩猩在 766 个完整的进食序列中的手动/脚蹬姿势进行了这种序列分析。我们用食物摄入量的部分替代值(即进食数量)定义的得分来分析其有效性,该得分与每个序列中手动姿势的多样性和变化的操作得分相关联。我们确定了四种操作技巧,根据个人操作食物的物理基质和食物的物理特性,使用的技巧也有所不同。结果表明,对于大块食物和稳定性较差的基质,操作技巧更为复杂(即操作得分更高)。但是,这些食物的有效性得分并没有明显降低,因为操作的复杂性似乎可以通过更多的进食次数得到补偿。看来,所采用的技巧涉及到操作复杂性和摄入食物数量之间的权衡。这项研究使我们能够测试和验证创新的分析方法,这些方法适用于涉及连续事件的各种伦理学研究。我们的研究结果为更好地理解灵长类与不同生态环境、陆地和树栖基质相关的徒手能力的进化提供了新的数据,并表明需要进一步探索社会和个体的影响因素。
{"title":"Influence of food physical properties and environmental context on manipulative behaviors highlighted by new methodological approaches in zoo-housed bonobos (Pan paniscus)","authors":"Caroline Gérard,&nbsp;Ameline Bardo,&nbsp;Jean Pascal Guéry,&nbsp;Emmanuelle Pouydebat,&nbsp;Victor Narat,&nbsp;Bruno Simmen","doi":"10.1002/ajp.23624","DOIUrl":"10.1002/ajp.23624","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Research on manipulative abilities in nonhuman primates, in the context of hominid evolution, has mostly focused on manual/pedal postures considered as static behaviors. While these behavioral repertoires highlighted the range of manipulative abilities in many species, manipulation is a dynamic process that mostly involves successive types of grips before reaching its goal. The present study aims to investigate the use of manual/pedal postures in zoo-housed bonobos in diverse dynamic food processing by using an innovative approach: the optimal matching analysis that compares sequences (<i>i.e</i>., succession of grasping postures) with each other. To characterize the manipulative techniques spontaneously employed by bonobos, we performed this sequential analysis of manual/pedal postures during 766 complete feeding sequences of 17 individuals. We analyzed the effectiveness with a score defined by a partial proxy of food intake (i.e., the number of mouthfuls) linked to a handling score measuring both the diversity and changes of manual postures during each sequence. We identified four techniques, used differently depending on the physical substrate on which the individual performed food manipulation and the food physical properties. Our results showed that manipulative techniques were more complex (<i>i.e</i>., higher handling score) for large foods and on substrates with lower stability. But the effectiveness score was not significantly lower for these items since manipulative complexity seemed to be compensated by a greater number of mouthfuls. It appeared that the techniques employed involved a trade-off between manipulative complexity and the amount of food ingested. This study allowed us to test and validate innovative analysis methods that are applicable to diverse ethological studies involving sequential events. Our results bring new data for a better understanding of the evolution of manual abilities in primates in association with different ecological contexts and both terrestrial and arboreal substrates and suggest that social and individual influences need to be explored further.</p>","PeriodicalId":7662,"journal":{"name":"American Journal of Primatology","volume":"86 6","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.4,"publicationDate":"2024-03-28","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/ajp.23624","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140304421","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Where the small things are: Modelling edge effects on mouse lemur population density and distribution in northwestern Madagascar. 小东西在哪里?模拟马达加斯加西北部鼠狐猴种群密度和分布的边缘效应。
IF 2.4 3区 生物学 Q1 ZOOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-03-25 DOI: 10.1002/ajp.23621
Fernando Mercado Malabet, Malcolm Ramsay, Coral Chell, Bertrand Andriatsitohaina, Ute Radespiel, Shawn Lehman

Edge effects result from the penetration to varying depths and intensities, of abiotic and biotic conditions from the surrounding non-forest matrix into the forest interior. Although 70% of the world's forests are within 1 km of a forest edge, making edge effects a dominant feature of most forest habitats, there are few empirical data on inter-site differences in edge responses in primates. We used spatially explicit capture-recapture (SECR) models to determine spatial patterns of density for two species of mouse lemurs (Microcebus murinus and Microcebus ravelobensis) in two forest landscapes in northwestern Madagascar. The goal of our study was to determine if mouse lemurs displayed spatially variable responses to edge effects. We trapped animals using Sherman live traps in the Mariarano Classified Forest (MCF) and in the Ambanjabe Forest Fragment Site (AFFS) site within Ankarafantsika National Park. We trapped 126 M. murinus and 79 M. ravelobensis at MCF and 78 M. murinus and 308 M. ravelobensis at AFFS. For M. murinus, our top model predicted a positive edge response, where density increased towards edge habitats. In M. ravelobensis, our top model predicted a negative edge response, where density was lower near the forest edges and increased towards the forest interior. At regional and landscape-specific scales, SECR models estimated different density patterns between M. murinus and M. ravelobensis as a result of variation in edge distance. The spatial variability of our results using SECR models indicate the importance of studying the population ecology of primates at varying scales that are appropriate to the processes of interest. Our results lend further support to the theory that some lemurs exhibit a form of ecological flexibility in their responses to forest loss, forest fragmentation, and associated edge effects.

边缘效应是由于周围非森林基质的非生物和生物条件以不同的深度和强度渗透到森林内部而产生的。虽然世界上 70% 的森林都在森林边缘 1 公里范围内,这使得边缘效应成为大多数森林栖息地的主要特征,但有关灵长类动物边缘反应的地点间差异的经验数据却很少。我们使用空间显式捕获-再捕获(SECR)模型来确定马达加斯加西北部两个森林景观中两种鼠狐猴(Microcebus murinus 和 Microcebus ravelobensis)的密度空间模式。我们的研究目标是确定鼠狐猴是否对边缘效应表现出不同的空间反应。我们使用谢尔曼活体诱捕器在马里亚拉诺分类森林(MCF)和安卡拉凡茨卡国家公园内的安班贾贝森林片区(AFFS)进行了诱捕。我们在MCF诱捕了126头M. murinus和79头M. ravelobensis,在AFFS诱捕了78头M. murinus和308头M. ravelobensis。对于M. murinus,我们的顶级模型预测其边缘反应为正,即密度向边缘栖息地增加。对于 M. ravelobensis,我们的顶级模型预测其边缘反应为负,即森林边缘附近密度较低,而森林内部密度增加。在区域和特定景观尺度上,SECR模型估计了M. murinus和M. ravelobensis之间不同的密度模式,这是边缘距离变化的结果。使用 SECR 模型得出的结果在空间上的差异性表明,在不同尺度上研究灵长类种群生态学的重要性,这些尺度应与感兴趣的过程相适应。我们的研究结果进一步支持了这样一种理论,即某些狐猴在对森林消失、森林破碎化及相关边缘效应的反应中表现出一种生态灵活性。
{"title":"Where the small things are: Modelling edge effects on mouse lemur population density and distribution in northwestern Madagascar.","authors":"Fernando Mercado Malabet, Malcolm Ramsay, Coral Chell, Bertrand Andriatsitohaina, Ute Radespiel, Shawn Lehman","doi":"10.1002/ajp.23621","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1002/ajp.23621","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Edge effects result from the penetration to varying depths and intensities, of abiotic and biotic conditions from the surrounding non-forest matrix into the forest interior. Although 70% of the world's forests are within 1 km of a forest edge, making edge effects a dominant feature of most forest habitats, there are few empirical data on inter-site differences in edge responses in primates. We used spatially explicit capture-recapture (SECR) models to determine spatial patterns of density for two species of mouse lemurs (Microcebus murinus and Microcebus ravelobensis) in two forest landscapes in northwestern Madagascar. The goal of our study was to determine if mouse lemurs displayed spatially variable responses to edge effects. We trapped animals using Sherman live traps in the Mariarano Classified Forest (MCF) and in the Ambanjabe Forest Fragment Site (AFFS) site within Ankarafantsika National Park. We trapped 126 M. murinus and 79 M. ravelobensis at MCF and 78 M. murinus and 308 M. ravelobensis at AFFS. For M. murinus, our top model predicted a positive edge response, where density increased towards edge habitats. In M. ravelobensis, our top model predicted a negative edge response, where density was lower near the forest edges and increased towards the forest interior. At regional and landscape-specific scales, SECR models estimated different density patterns between M. murinus and M. ravelobensis as a result of variation in edge distance. The spatial variability of our results using SECR models indicate the importance of studying the population ecology of primates at varying scales that are appropriate to the processes of interest. Our results lend further support to the theory that some lemurs exhibit a form of ecological flexibility in their responses to forest loss, forest fragmentation, and associated edge effects.</p>","PeriodicalId":7662,"journal":{"name":"American Journal of Primatology","volume":" ","pages":"e23621"},"PeriodicalIF":2.4,"publicationDate":"2024-03-25","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140288042","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Capuchin monkeys' (Sapajus [Cebus] apella) categorization of photos of unknown male conspecifics suggests attention to fWHR and a dominance bias 卷尾猴(Sapajus [Cebus] apella)对未知雄性同种动物照片的分类表明,它们会关注fWHR和优势偏好。
IF 2.4 3区 生物学 Q1 ZOOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-03-25 DOI: 10.1002/ajp.23623
Ashley M. Meacham, Meghan J. Sosnowski, Heather M. Kleider-Offutt, Sarah F. Brosnan

The ability to quickly perceive others' rank minimizes costs by helping individuals behave appropriately when interacting with strangers. Indeed, humans and at least some other species can quickly determine strangers' rank or dominance based only on physical features without observing others' interactions or behavior. Nonhuman primates can determine strangers' ranks by observing their interactions, and some evidence suggests that at least some cues to dominance, such as facial width-to-height ratio (fWHR), are also present in other primates. However, it is unknown whether they can determine strangers' rank simply by looking at their faces, rather than observing their interactions. If so, this would suggest selective pressure across the primates on both cues to dominance and the ability to detect those cues accurately. To address this, we examined the ability of male and female tufted capuchin monkeys (Sapajus [Cebus] apella) to categorize images of the faces of unknown conspecifics (Sapajus from different colonies) and humans (computer-generated and real) as dominant or nondominant based only on still images. Capuchins' categorization of unknown conspecific faces was consistent with fWHR, a cue to dominance, although there was a strong tendency to categorize strangers as dominant, particularly for males. This was true despite the continued correct categorization of known individuals. In addition, capuchins did not categorize human strangers in accordance with external pre-ratings of dominance by independent human raters, despite the availability of the same cue, fWHR. We consider these results in the context of capuchin socio-ecology and what they mean for the evolution of rapid decision-making in social contexts.

快速感知他人等级的能力可以帮助个体在与陌生人交往时行为得体,从而将成本降到最低。事实上,人类和至少其他一些物种可以仅根据身体特征迅速判断陌生人的等级或支配地位,而无需观察他人的互动或行为。非人类灵长类动物可以通过观察陌生人的互动来判断他们的等级,而且一些证据表明,其他灵长类动物至少也存在一些支配地位的线索,如面部宽高比(fWHR)。然而,它们是否能仅仅通过观察陌生人的脸而不是通过观察他们的互动来确定他们的等级还不得而知。如果是这样,这将表明灵长类动物在优势线索和准确检测这些线索的能力方面都存在选择性压力。为了解决这个问题,我们研究了雄性和雌性簇毛卷尾猴(Sapajus [Cebus] apella)仅根据静态图像将未知同类(来自不同群落的卷尾猴)和人类(计算机生成的和真实的)的脸部图像归类为占优势或不占优势的能力。卷尾猴对未知同类脸部的分类与作为优势线索的 fWHR 一致,但有一种强烈的倾向是将陌生人归类为优势者,尤其是雄性卷尾猴。尽管卷尾猴对已知个体的分类仍然正确,但这一趋势依然存在。此外,尽管有同样的线索 fWHR,卷尾猴并不根据独立人类评定者对支配地位的外部预评定来对陌生人进行分类。我们将从卷尾猴社会生态学的角度来考虑这些结果,以及它们对社会环境中快速决策进化的意义。
{"title":"Capuchin monkeys' (Sapajus [Cebus] apella) categorization of photos of unknown male conspecifics suggests attention to fWHR and a dominance bias","authors":"Ashley M. Meacham,&nbsp;Meghan J. Sosnowski,&nbsp;Heather M. Kleider-Offutt,&nbsp;Sarah F. Brosnan","doi":"10.1002/ajp.23623","DOIUrl":"10.1002/ajp.23623","url":null,"abstract":"<p>The ability to quickly perceive others' rank minimizes costs by helping individuals behave appropriately when interacting with strangers. Indeed, humans and at least some other species can quickly determine strangers' rank or dominance based only on physical features without observing others' interactions or behavior. Nonhuman primates can determine strangers' ranks by observing their interactions, and some evidence suggests that at least some cues to dominance, such as facial width-to-height ratio (fWHR), are also present in other primates. However, it is unknown whether they can determine strangers' rank simply by looking at their faces, rather than observing their interactions. If so, this would suggest selective pressure across the primates on both cues to dominance and the ability to detect those cues accurately. To address this, we examined the ability of male and female tufted capuchin monkeys (<i>Sapajus [Cebus] apella</i>) to categorize images of the faces of unknown conspecifics (<i>Sapajus</i> from different colonies) and humans (computer-generated and real) as dominant or nondominant based only on still images. Capuchins' categorization of unknown conspecific faces was consistent with fWHR, a cue to dominance, although there was a strong tendency to categorize strangers as dominant, particularly for males. This was true despite the continued correct categorization of known individuals. In addition, capuchins did not categorize human strangers in accordance with external pre-ratings of dominance by independent human raters, despite the availability of the same cue, fWHR. We consider these results in the context of capuchin socio-ecology and what they mean for the evolution of rapid decision-making in social contexts.</p>","PeriodicalId":7662,"journal":{"name":"American Journal of Primatology","volume":"86 6","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.4,"publicationDate":"2024-03-25","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140288041","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Interactions with humans reduce the success of foraging for anthropogenic food by capuchin monkeys (Sapajus libidinosus) in Brasília National Park, Brazil 与人类的互动降低了巴西巴西利亚国家公园卷尾猴(Sapajus libidinosus)觅食的成功率
IF 2.4 3区 生物学 Q1 ZOOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-03-20 DOI: 10.1002/ajp.23620
Murilo Reis Camargo, Túlio Costa Lousa, Ricardo Vasquez Mota, Francisco D. C. Mendes

The progressive growth of urban environments has increasingly forced populations of nonhuman primates to coexist with humans in many cities, which has resulted in problems such as behavioral alterations, conflicts with humans, and threats to the health of the monkeys, due to their consumption of anthropogenic foodstuffs. These anthropogenic foods, which are rich in calories, are the principal driver of the proximity between humans and primates, even though the acquisition of these foods tends to be risky for the monkeys and involve a variety of challenges derived from specific features of the urban environment. The present study evaluated the success/risk relationship of foraging for anthropogenic food by tufted capuchins (Sapajus libidinosus) in Brasília National Park. The data were analyzed using a binary logistic regression, with the backward-stepwise Wald method, to investigate the factors related to the foraging success of the capuchins, considering variables such as their sex and age, the type of approach and its context, and interactions with humans. The capuchins were influenced by the anthropogenic context, which affected their foraging strategies and diet. Interactions with humans reduced the success of foraging for anthropogenic foods. Conflicts between humans and the capuchins were common, especially in the context of access to food. The capuchins thus preferred to access feeding resources directly, probably due to the reduced human interference, which resulted in greater foraging success for unattended food brought by park visitors and the raiding of trash cans. Based on the observed behavior patterns, a number of measures can be proposed to mitigate these conflicts. These recommendations include not bringing food into areas frequented by the capuchins, not reacting to approaching animals, and removing all trash generated during a visit. A cleaning team dedicated to the maintenance of the visitation area free of anthropogenic waste is also be recommended.

随着城市环境的逐步发展,在许多城市中,非人灵长类动物越来越多地被迫与人类共存,这导致了一些问题,如行为改变、与人类的冲突,以及由于猴子食用人类食物而对其健康造成的威胁。这些人为食物含有丰富的热量,是人类与灵长类动物接近的主要驱动力,尽管对猴子来说,获取这些食物往往是危险的,并涉及城市环境特殊性所带来的各种挑战。本研究评估了巴西利亚国家公园丛毛卷尾猴(Sapajus libidinosus)觅食的成功/风险关系。数据分析采用二元逻辑回归法,并考虑了卷尾猴的性别和年龄、觅食方式和环境以及与人类的互动等变量,研究了与卷尾猴觅食成功与否有关的因素。卷尾猴受到人为环境的影响,这影响了它们的觅食策略和饮食。与人类的互动降低了卷尾猴觅食人类食物的成功率。人类与卷尾猴之间的冲突很常见,尤其是在获取食物方面。因此,卷尾猴更愿意直接获取食物资源,这可能是由于人类的干扰减少了,导致卷尾猴对公园游客带来的无人看管的食物的觅食成功率更高,以及对垃圾桶的掠夺。根据观察到的行为模式,可以提出一些措施来缓解这些冲突。这些建议包括不要将食物带入卷尾猴经常出没的区域,不要对靠近的动物做出反应,以及清除游览期间产生的所有垃圾。此外,还建议成立一个清洁小组,专门负责维护探访区内的人为垃圾。
{"title":"Interactions with humans reduce the success of foraging for anthropogenic food by capuchin monkeys (Sapajus libidinosus) in Brasília National Park, Brazil","authors":"Murilo Reis Camargo,&nbsp;Túlio Costa Lousa,&nbsp;Ricardo Vasquez Mota,&nbsp;Francisco D. C. Mendes","doi":"10.1002/ajp.23620","DOIUrl":"10.1002/ajp.23620","url":null,"abstract":"<p>The progressive growth of urban environments has increasingly forced populations of nonhuman primates to coexist with humans in many cities, which has resulted in problems such as behavioral alterations, conflicts with humans, and threats to the health of the monkeys, due to their consumption of anthropogenic foodstuffs. These anthropogenic foods, which are rich in calories, are the principal driver of the proximity between humans and primates, even though the acquisition of these foods tends to be risky for the monkeys and involve a variety of challenges derived from specific features of the urban environment. The present study evaluated the success/risk relationship of foraging for anthropogenic food by tufted capuchins (<i>Sapajus libidinosus</i>) in Brasília National Park. The data were analyzed using a binary logistic regression, with the backward-stepwise Wald method, to investigate the factors related to the foraging success of the capuchins, considering variables such as their sex and age, the type of approach and its context, and interactions with humans. The capuchins were influenced by the anthropogenic context, which affected their foraging strategies and diet. Interactions with humans reduced the success of foraging for anthropogenic foods. Conflicts between humans and the capuchins were common, especially in the context of access to food. The capuchins thus preferred to access feeding resources directly, probably due to the reduced human interference, which resulted in greater foraging success for unattended food brought by park visitors and the raiding of trash cans. Based on the observed behavior patterns, a number of measures can be proposed to mitigate these conflicts. These recommendations include not bringing food into areas frequented by the capuchins, not reacting to approaching animals, and removing all trash generated during a visit. A cleaning team dedicated to the maintenance of the visitation area free of anthropogenic waste is also be recommended.</p>","PeriodicalId":7662,"journal":{"name":"American Journal of Primatology","volume":"86 6","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.4,"publicationDate":"2024-03-20","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140172208","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Population assessment of the Endangered Kashmir Gray Langur (Semnopithecus ajax, Pocock 1928) using the double-observer method 使用双观察者法评估濒危克什米尔灰鹿(Semnopithecus ajax, Pocock 1928)的种群数量。
IF 2.4 3区 生物学 Q1 ZOOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-03-14 DOI: 10.1002/ajp.23618
Shahid Hameed, Tawqir Bashir, Mohammad N. Ali, Munib Khanyari, Ajith Kumar

Primates are among the most threatened taxa globally, therefore, there is a need to estimate and monitor their populations. Kashmir Gray Langur Semnopithecus ajax is an endangered species for which there is no population estimate. We used double-observer method to estimate its population size in the Kashmir region of North-Western Himalaya. We walked 1284 km across 31 survey blocks spanning all three divisions of Kashmir viz., North, Central, and South Kashmir, covering an area of 411 km2. We counted a minimum of 1367 individual langurs from 27 groups. The detection probability for observer 1 (0.719) and observer 2 (0.656) resulted in a population estimate of 1496 (95% confidence interval [CI] 1367–1899) across 30 groups (with a mean group size of 51), giving a density estimate of 3.64 (3.33–4.62) langurs/km². We found double-observer surveys to be suitable for the population estimation of langurs, and we make recommendations on how to effectively conduct primate surveys, especially in mountainous ecosystems. Our records extend the species distribution range beyond stated by the International Union for Conservation of Nature. Our findings also highlight that the Kashmir Himalaya is a stronghold of the species, where conservation efforts should focus.

灵长类动物是全球最受威胁的类群之一,因此有必要对其种群数量进行估计和监测。克什米尔灰叶猴(Semnopithecus ajax)是一种濒危物种,目前尚无种群数量估计。我们采用双重观察法估算了喜马拉雅山西北部克什米尔地区的种群数量。我们在克什米尔的北、中、南三个分区的 31 个调查区行走了 1284 公里,覆盖面积达 411 平方公里。我们对来自 27 个群体的至少 1367 只叶猴个体进行了计数。根据观察者 1(0.719)和观察者 2(0.656)的探测概率,30 个群体(平均群体大小为 51)的叶猴数量估计为 1496 只(95% 置信区间 [CI] 1367-1899 ),密度估计为 3.64(3.33-4.62)只/平方公里。我们发现双观察者调查适用于叶猴的种群估计,并就如何有效开展灵长类动物调查(尤其是在山区生态系统中)提出了建议。我们的记录扩大了该物种的分布范围,超出了国际自然保护联盟的规定。我们的研究结果还强调,克什米尔喜马拉雅山是该物种的大本营,应重点保护。
{"title":"Population assessment of the Endangered Kashmir Gray Langur (Semnopithecus ajax, Pocock 1928) using the double-observer method","authors":"Shahid Hameed,&nbsp;Tawqir Bashir,&nbsp;Mohammad N. Ali,&nbsp;Munib Khanyari,&nbsp;Ajith Kumar","doi":"10.1002/ajp.23618","DOIUrl":"10.1002/ajp.23618","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Primates are among the most threatened taxa globally, therefore, there is a need to estimate and monitor their populations. Kashmir Gray Langur <i>Semnopithecus ajax</i> is an endangered species for which there is no population estimate. We used double-observer method to estimate its population size in the Kashmir region of North-Western Himalaya. We walked 1284 km across 31 survey blocks spanning all three divisions of Kashmir <i>viz</i>., North, Central, and South Kashmir, covering an area of 411 km<sup>2</sup>. We counted a minimum of 1367 individual langurs from 27 groups. The detection probability for observer 1 (0.719) and observer 2 (0.656) resulted in a population estimate of 1496 (95% confidence interval [CI] 1367–1899) across 30 groups (with a mean group size of 51), giving a density estimate of 3.64 (3.33–4.62) langurs/km². We found double-observer surveys to be suitable for the population estimation of langurs, and we make recommendations on how to effectively conduct primate surveys, especially in mountainous ecosystems. Our records extend the species distribution range beyond stated by the International Union for Conservation of Nature. Our findings also highlight that the Kashmir Himalaya is a stronghold of the species, where conservation efforts should focus.</p>","PeriodicalId":7662,"journal":{"name":"American Journal of Primatology","volume":"86 6","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.4,"publicationDate":"2024-03-14","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140118477","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Neutrophil to lymphocyte ratio in captive olive baboons (Papio anubis): The effects of age, sex, rearing, stress, and pregnancy 圈养橄榄狒狒(Papio anubis)的中性粒细胞与淋巴细胞比率:年龄、性别、饲养、压力和怀孕的影响。
IF 2.4 3区 生物学 Q1 ZOOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-03-14 DOI: 10.1002/ajp.23619
Sarah J. Neal, Angela M. Achorn, Steven J. Schapiro, William D. Hopkins, Joe H. Simmons

In apes and humans, neutrophil to lymphocyte ratio (NLR) can be used as a predictive indicator of a variety of clinical conditions, longevity, and physiological stress. In chimpanzees specifically, NLR systematically varies with age, rearing, sex, and premature death, indicating that NLR may be a useful diagnostic tool in assessing primate health. To date, just one very recent study has investigated NLR in old world monkeys and found lower NLR in males and nursery-reared individuals, as well as a negative relationship between NLR and disease outcomes. Given that baboons are increasingly used as research models, we aimed to characterize NLR in baboons by providing descriptive data and examinations of baboon NLR heritability, and of the relationships between NLR, age, rearing, and sex in 387 olive baboons (Papio anubis) between 6 months and 19 years of age. We found that (1) mother-reared baboons had higher NLRs than nursery-reared baboons; (2) females had higher NLRs than males; and (3) there was a quadratic relationship between NLR and age, such that middle-aged individuals had the highest NLR values. We also examined NLR as a function of transport to a new facility using a subset of the data. Baboons exhibited significantly higher transport NLRs compared to routine exam NLRs. More specifically, adult baboons had higher transport NLRs than routine NLRs, whereas juveniles showed no such difference, suggesting that younger animals may experience transport stress differently than older animals. We also found that transport NLR was heritable, whereas routine NLR was not, possibly suggesting that stress responses (as indicated in NLR) have a strong genetic component. Consistent with research in humans and chimpanzees, these findings suggest that NLR varies with important biological and life history variables and that NLR may be a useful health biomarker in baboons.

在猿类和人类中,中性粒细胞与淋巴细胞的比率(NLR)可用作各种临床症状、寿命和生理压力的预测指标。特别是在黑猩猩中,NLR随年龄、饲养、性别和早死而系统地变化,这表明NLR可能是评估灵长类健康状况的有用诊断工具。迄今为止,仅有一项最新研究调查了旧世界猴的无淋巴细胞比率,发现雄性猴和保育猴的无淋巴细胞比率较低,而且无淋巴细胞比率与疾病结果呈负相关。鉴于狒狒越来越多地被用作研究模型,我们的目的是通过提供描述性数据和研究狒狒无淋巴细胞计数的遗传率,以及 387 只 6 个月至 19 岁的橄榄狒狒(Papio anubis)的无淋巴细胞计数、年龄、饲养和性别之间的关系,来描述狒狒无淋巴细胞计数的特征。我们发现:(1) 母亲饲养的狒狒比保育狒狒的无淋巴细胞比率高;(2) 雌性狒狒的无淋巴细胞比率比雄性高;(3) 无淋巴细胞比率与年龄之间存在二次关系,因此中年个体的无淋巴细胞比率值最高。我们还利用部分数据研究了 NLR 与运输到新设施之间的函数关系。与常规检查的 NLR 相比,狒狒的运输 NLR 明显更高。更具体地说,成年狒狒的运输 NLR 值高于常规 NLR 值,而幼年狒狒则没有这种差异,这表明幼年狒狒对运输压力的体验可能与老年狒狒不同。我们还发现,运输 NLR 具有遗传性,而常规 NLR 则不具有遗传性,这可能表明应激反应(如 NLR 所示)具有很强的遗传性。与人类和黑猩猩的研究结果一致,这些研究结果表明,NLR随重要的生物和生活史变量而变化,NLR可能是狒狒的一种有用的健康生物标志物。
{"title":"Neutrophil to lymphocyte ratio in captive olive baboons (Papio anubis): The effects of age, sex, rearing, stress, and pregnancy","authors":"Sarah J. Neal,&nbsp;Angela M. Achorn,&nbsp;Steven J. Schapiro,&nbsp;William D. Hopkins,&nbsp;Joe H. Simmons","doi":"10.1002/ajp.23619","DOIUrl":"10.1002/ajp.23619","url":null,"abstract":"<p>In apes and humans, neutrophil to lymphocyte ratio (NLR) can be used as a predictive indicator of a variety of clinical conditions, longevity, and physiological stress. In chimpanzees specifically, NLR systematically varies with age, rearing, sex, and premature death, indicating that NLR may be a useful diagnostic tool in assessing primate health. To date, just one very recent study has investigated NLR in old world monkeys and found lower NLR in males and nursery-reared individuals, as well as a negative relationship between NLR and disease outcomes. Given that baboons are increasingly used as research models, we aimed to characterize NLR in baboons by providing descriptive data and examinations of baboon NLR heritability, and of the relationships between NLR, age, rearing, and sex in 387 olive baboons (<i>Papio anubis</i>) between 6 months and 19 years of age. We found that (1) mother-reared baboons had higher NLRs than nursery-reared baboons; (2) females had higher NLRs than males; and (3) there was a quadratic relationship between NLR and age, such that middle-aged individuals had the highest NLR values. We also examined NLR as a function of transport to a new facility using a subset of the data. Baboons exhibited significantly higher transport NLRs compared to routine exam NLRs. More specifically, adult baboons had higher transport NLRs than routine NLRs, whereas juveniles showed no such difference, suggesting that younger animals may experience transport stress differently than older animals. We also found that transport NLR was heritable, whereas routine NLR was not, possibly suggesting that stress responses (as indicated in NLR) have a strong genetic component. Consistent with research in humans and chimpanzees, these findings suggest that NLR varies with important biological and life history variables and that NLR may be a useful health biomarker in baboons.</p>","PeriodicalId":7662,"journal":{"name":"American Journal of Primatology","volume":"86 6","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.4,"publicationDate":"2024-03-14","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140118476","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Humans (Homo sapiens) but not baboons (Papio papio) demonstrate crossmodal pitch-luminance correspondence 人类(Homo sapiens)而非狒狒(Papio papio)表现出跨模态的音高-亮度对应关系。
IF 2.4 3区 生物学 Q1 ZOOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-03-12 DOI: 10.1002/ajp.23613
Konstantina Margiotoudi, Joel Fagot, Adrien Meguerditchian, Isabelle Dautriche

Humans spontaneously and consistently map information coming from different sensory modalities. Surprisingly, the phylogenetic origin of such cross-modal correspondences has been under-investigated. A notable exception is the study of Ludwig et al. (Visuoauditory mappings between high luminance and high pitch are shared by chimpanzees [Pan troglodytes] and humans. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108(51), 20661–20665) which reports that both humans and chimpanzees spontaneously map high-pitched sounds with bright objects and low-pitched sounds with dark objects. Our pre-registered study aimed to directly replicate this research on both humans and baboons (Papio papio), an old world monkey which is more phylogenetically distant from humans than chimpanzees. Following Ludwig et al. participants were presented with a visual classification task where they had to sort black and white square (low and high luminance), while background sounds (low or high-pitched tones) were playing. Whereas we replicated the finding that humans' performance on the visual task was affected by congruency between sound and luminance of the target, we did not find any of those effects on baboons' performance. These results question the presence of a shared cross-modal pitch-luminance mapping in other nonhuman primates.

人类自发地、持续地映射来自不同感官模式的信息。令人惊讶的是,对这种跨模态对应关系的系统发育起源研究不足。路德维希等人的研究是一个显著的例外(黑猩猩(Pan troglodytes)和人类共享高亮度和高音调之间的视觉听觉映射。美国国家科学院院刊》,108(51), 20661-20665)报告称,人类和黑猩猩都会自发地将高音调的声音与明亮的物体进行映射,将低音调的声音与黑暗的物体进行映射。我们的预注册研究旨在直接在人类和狒狒(Papio papio)身上复制这项研究,狒狒是一种旧世界的猴子,在系统发育上与人类的距离比黑猩猩更远。根据路德维格等人的研究,我们给参与者布置了一项视觉分类任务,要求他们在播放背景声音(低音调或高音调)的同时对黑白方块(低亮度和高亮度)进行分类。我们重复了人类在视觉任务中的表现会受到声音和目标亮度一致性影响的发现,但我们没有发现狒狒的表现会受到这些影响。这些结果对其他非人灵长类动物是否存在共同的跨模态音调-亮度映射提出了质疑。
{"title":"Humans (Homo sapiens) but not baboons (Papio papio) demonstrate crossmodal pitch-luminance correspondence","authors":"Konstantina Margiotoudi,&nbsp;Joel Fagot,&nbsp;Adrien Meguerditchian,&nbsp;Isabelle Dautriche","doi":"10.1002/ajp.23613","DOIUrl":"10.1002/ajp.23613","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Humans spontaneously and consistently map information coming from different sensory modalities. Surprisingly, the phylogenetic origin of such cross-modal correspondences has been under-investigated. A notable exception is the study of Ludwig et al. (Visuoauditory mappings between high luminance and high pitch are shared by chimpanzees [Pan troglodytes] and humans. <i>Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences</i>, <i>108</i>(51), 20661–20665) which reports that both humans and chimpanzees spontaneously map high-pitched sounds with bright objects and low-pitched sounds with dark objects. Our pre-registered study aimed to directly replicate this research on both humans and baboons (<i>Papio papio</i>), an old world monkey which is more phylogenetically distant from humans than chimpanzees. Following Ludwig et al. participants were presented with a visual classification task where they had to sort black and white square (low and high luminance), while background sounds (low or high-pitched tones) were playing. Whereas we replicated the finding that humans' performance on the visual task was affected by congruency between sound and luminance of the target, we did not find any of those effects on baboons' performance. These results question the presence of a shared cross-modal pitch-luminance mapping in other nonhuman primates.</p>","PeriodicalId":7662,"journal":{"name":"American Journal of Primatology","volume":"86 5","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.4,"publicationDate":"2024-03-12","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/ajp.23613","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140108839","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Consistency in Verreaux's sifaka home range and core area size despite seasonal variation in resource availability as assessed by Enhanced Vegetation Index (EVI) 根据增强植被指数(EVI)评估,尽管资源可用性存在季节性变化,但马尾藻的巢区和核心区大小保持一致。
IF 2.4 3区 生物学 Q1 ZOOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-03-11 DOI: 10.1002/ajp.23617
Anne C. Axel, Brynn M. Harshbarger, Rebecca J. Lewis, Stacey R. Tecot

Primates are adept at dealing with fluctuating availability of resources and display a range of responses to minimize the effects of food scarcity. An important component of primate conservation is to understand how primates adapt their foraging and ranging patterns in response to fluctuating food resources. Animals optimize resource acquisition within the home range through the selection of resource-bearing patches and choose between contrasting foraging strategies (resource-maximizing vs. area-minimizing). Our study aimed to characterize the foraging strategy of a folivorous primate, Verreaux's sifaka (Propithecus verreauxi), by evaluating whether group home range size varied between peak and lean leaf seasons within a seasonally dry tropical forest in Madagascar. We hypothesized that Verreaux's sifaka used the resource maximization strategy to select high-value resource patches so that during periods of resource depression, the home range area did not significantly change in size. We characterized resource availability (i.e., primary productivity) by season at Kirindy Mitea National Park using remotely-sensed Enhanced Vegetation Index data. We calculated group home ranges using 10 years of focal animal sampling data collected on eight groups using both 95% and 50% kernel density estimation. We used area accumulation curves to ensure each group had an adequate number of locations to reach seasonal home range asymptotes. Neither 95% home ranges nor 50% core areas differed across peak and lean leaf resource seasons, supporting the hypothesis that Verreaux's sifaka use a resource maximization strategy. With a better understanding of animal space use strategies, managers can model anticipated changes under environmental and/or anthropogenic resource depression scenarios. These findings demonstrate the value of long-term data for characterizing and understanding foraging and ranging patterns. We also illustrate the benefits of using satellite data for characterizing food resources for folivorous primates.

灵长类动物善于应对资源供应的波动,并表现出一系列反应,以尽量减少食物匮乏的影响。灵长类动物保护的一个重要组成部分是了解灵长类动物如何调整其觅食和活动模式,以应对食物资源的波动。灵长类动物通过选择富含资源的斑块来优化在家园范围内的资源获取,并在不同的觅食策略(资源最大化与面积最小化)之间做出选择。我们的研究旨在通过评估马达加斯加季节性干旱热带雨林中叶片生长旺季和枯叶期的群体觅食范围大小是否会发生变化,来描述食叶灵长类动物--维里欧氏西番卡(Propithecus verreauxi)的觅食策略。我们假定,维氏红叶石楠利用资源最大化策略来选择高价值的资源斑块,因此在资源萧条期,其家域面积不会发生显著变化。我们利用遥感增强植被指数数据,按季节描述了基林迪米泰亚国家公园的资源可用性(即初级生产力)。我们使用 95% 和 50% 核密度估计法,利用 10 年来对 8 个群落收集的重点动物取样数据计算了群落的家园范围。我们使用面积累积曲线来确保每个群体都有足够数量的地点来达到季节性家园范围渐近线。95%的家园范围和50%的核心区域在叶片资源旺季和淡季都没有差异,这支持了维里亚狐蜥采用资源最大化策略的假设。有了对动物空间利用策略的更深入了解,管理者就可以模拟环境和/或人为资源萧条情况下的预期变化。这些发现证明了长期数据对于描述和了解觅食和活动模式的价值。我们还说明了利用卫星数据描述食叶灵长类食物资源特征的益处。
{"title":"Consistency in Verreaux's sifaka home range and core area size despite seasonal variation in resource availability as assessed by Enhanced Vegetation Index (EVI)","authors":"Anne C. Axel,&nbsp;Brynn M. Harshbarger,&nbsp;Rebecca J. Lewis,&nbsp;Stacey R. Tecot","doi":"10.1002/ajp.23617","DOIUrl":"10.1002/ajp.23617","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Primates are adept at dealing with fluctuating availability of resources and display a range of responses to minimize the effects of food scarcity. An important component of primate conservation is to understand how primates adapt their foraging and ranging patterns in response to fluctuating food resources. Animals optimize resource acquisition within the home range through the selection of resource-bearing patches and choose between contrasting foraging strategies (resource-maximizing vs. area-minimizing). Our study aimed to characterize the foraging strategy of a folivorous primate, Verreaux's sifaka (<i>Propithecus verreauxi</i>), by evaluating whether group home range size varied between peak and lean leaf seasons within a seasonally dry tropical forest in Madagascar. We hypothesized that Verreaux's sifaka used the resource maximization strategy to select high-value resource patches so that during periods of resource depression, the home range area did not significantly change in size. We characterized resource availability (i.e., primary productivity) by season at Kirindy Mitea National Park using remotely-sensed Enhanced Vegetation Index data. We calculated group home ranges using 10 years of focal animal sampling data collected on eight groups using both 95% and 50% kernel density estimation. We used area accumulation curves to ensure each group had an adequate number of locations to reach seasonal home range asymptotes. Neither 95% home ranges nor 50% core areas differed across peak and lean leaf resource seasons, supporting the hypothesis that Verreaux's sifaka use a resource maximization strategy. With a better understanding of animal space use strategies, managers can model anticipated changes under environmental and/or anthropogenic resource depression scenarios. These findings demonstrate the value of long-term data for characterizing and understanding foraging and ranging patterns. We also illustrate the benefits of using satellite data for characterizing food resources for folivorous primates.</p>","PeriodicalId":7662,"journal":{"name":"American Journal of Primatology","volume":"86 6","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.4,"publicationDate":"2024-03-11","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/ajp.23617","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140100762","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
American Journal of Primatology
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1