首页 > 最新文献

American Journal of Primatology最新文献

英文 中文
Prosocial or photo preferences? Gorillas' prosocial choices using a touchscreen 亲社会偏好还是照片偏好?大猩猩使用触摸屏时的亲社会选择
IF 2.4 3区 生物学 Q1 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2024-02-29 DOI: 10.1002/ajp.23612
Jennifer Vonk

Three male Western lowland gorillas (Gorilla gorilla gorilla) were given the opportunity to select their own or conspecific photos on a touchscreen to indicate whether they wished the experimenter to deliver a food reward only to them or to them and the selected conspecific(s). This is only the second symbolic test of prosocial preferences with apes using a touchscreen, and the first with gorillas. The use of self and other photographs as symbols of prosocial choices was intuitive while controlling for the distraction of visible food rewards, and allowing for tests of transfer to further validate apparent prosocial intentions. Gorillas rapidly learned to avoid selecting a photograph of an empty enclosure that resulted in no rewards for any of the gorillas and transferred this learning to a novel photograph. The gorillas did not behave in a consistently self-interested or prosocial manner but they clearly rejected the opportunity to choose spitefully. Their preferences for certain photographs did not necessarily reflect a preference to be prosocial toward that particular individual because these preferences did not transfer to novel photographs of the same individuals. The results call into question whether gorillas recognize themselves and conspecifics in photographs but cannot conclusively speak to whether gorillas have prosocial preferences. They do stress the importance of carefully probing alternative explanations when inferring intentions from observable behaviors.

三只雄性西部低地大猩猩(Gorilla gorilla gorilla)有机会在触摸屏上选择自己或同类的照片,以表示它们希望实验者只向它们提供食物奖励,还是向它们和所选的同类提供食物奖励。这是用触摸屏对猿类的亲社会偏好进行的第二次象征性测试,也是对大猩猩进行的第一次测试。使用自己和其他动物的照片作为亲社会选择的象征是直观的,同时控制了可见食物奖励的干扰,并允许进行转移测试以进一步验证明显的亲社会意图。大猩猩很快就学会了避免选择一张空围栏的照片,因为这张照片不会给任何一只大猩猩带来任何奖励。大猩猩的行为并不总是利己或亲社会的,但它们显然拒绝了唾弃性选择的机会。它们对某些照片的偏好并不一定反映出对特定个体的亲社会偏好,因为这些偏好并没有转移到相同个体的新照片上。这些结果使人们对大猩猩是否能识别照片中的自己和同类产生了怀疑,但并不能对大猩猩是否具有亲社会偏好作出定论。这些结果确实强调了从可观察到的行为推断意图时仔细探究其他解释的重要性。
{"title":"Prosocial or photo preferences? Gorillas' prosocial choices using a touchscreen","authors":"Jennifer Vonk","doi":"10.1002/ajp.23612","DOIUrl":"10.1002/ajp.23612","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Three male Western lowland gorillas (<i>Gorilla gorilla gorilla</i>) were given the opportunity to select their own or conspecific photos on a touchscreen to indicate whether they wished the experimenter to deliver a food reward only to them or to them and the selected conspecific(s). This is only the second symbolic test of prosocial preferences with apes using a touchscreen, and the first with gorillas. The use of self and other photographs as symbols of prosocial choices was intuitive while controlling for the distraction of visible food rewards, and allowing for tests of transfer to further validate apparent prosocial intentions. Gorillas rapidly learned to avoid selecting a photograph of an empty enclosure that resulted in no rewards for any of the gorillas and transferred this learning to a novel photograph. The gorillas did not behave in a consistently self-interested or prosocial manner but they clearly rejected the opportunity to choose spitefully. Their preferences for certain photographs did not necessarily reflect a preference to be prosocial toward that particular individual because these preferences did not transfer to novel photographs of the same individuals. The results call into question whether gorillas recognize themselves and conspecifics in photographs but cannot conclusively speak to whether gorillas have prosocial preferences. They do stress the importance of carefully probing alternative explanations when inferring intentions from observable behaviors.</p>","PeriodicalId":7662,"journal":{"name":"American Journal of Primatology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.4,"publicationDate":"2024-02-29","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139995319","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Dietary flexibility of the greater bamboo lemur (Prolemur simus), a specialized feeder, in eastern Madagascar 马达加斯加东部专食大竹叶狐猴(Prolemur simus)的饮食灵活性。
IF 2.4 3区 生物学 Q1 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2024-02-26 DOI: 10.1002/ajp.23609
T. Hasimija Mihaminekena, Ando N. Rakotonanahary, Cynthia L. Frasier, Hery N. T. Randriahaingo, Timothy M. Sefczek, Jen Tinsman, H. Lucien Randrianarimanana, Maholy Ravaloharimanitra, Toky Hery Rakotoarinivo, Jonah Ratsimbazafy, Tony King, Edward E. Louis

The degree of dietary flexibility in primates is species specific; some incorporate a wider array of resources than others. Extreme interannual weather variability in Madagascar results in seasonal resource scarcity which has been linked to specialized behaviors in lemurs. Prolemur simus, for example, has been considered an obligate specialist on large culm bamboo with >60% of its diet composed of woody bamboos requiring morphological and physiological adaptations to process. Recent studies reported an ever-expanding list of dietary items, suggesting that this species may not be an obligate specialist. However, long-term quantitative feeding data are unavailable across this species’ range. To explore the dietary flexibility of P. simus, we collected data at two northern sites, Ambalafary and Sahavola, and one southern site, Vatovavy, from September 2010 to January 2016 and May 2017 to September 2018, respectively. In total, we recorded 4022 h of behavioral data using instantaneous sampling of adult males and females from one group in Ambalafary, and two groups each in Sahavola and Vatovavy. We recorded 45 plant species eaten by P. simus over 7 years. We also observed significant differences in seasonal dietary composition between study sites. In Ambalafary, bamboo was the most frequently observed resource consumed (92.2%); however, non-bamboo resources comprised nearly one-third of the diet of P. simus in Sahavola and over 60% in Vatovavy. Consumption of all bamboo resources increased during the dry season at Ambalafary and during the wet season at Vatovavy, but never exceeded non-bamboo feeding at the latter. Culm pith feeding was only observed at Ambalafary, where it was more common during the dry season. We identify P. simus as a bamboo facultative specialist capable of adjusting its feeding behavior to its environment, indicating greater dietary flexibility than previously documented, which may enable the species to survive in increasingly degraded habitats.

灵长类动物饮食的灵活程度因物种而异;有些灵长类动物比其他灵长类动物吸收更多的资源。马达加斯加年际极端天气变化导致季节性资源稀缺,这与狐猴的专门行为有关。例如,狐猴(Prolemur simus)一直被认为是大型秆竹的专食动物,其食物的 60% 以上都是木质竹子,需要在形态和生理上进行适应性处理。最近的研究报告显示,该物种的食性不断扩大,这表明它可能并不是唯一的专食动物。然而,在该物种的分布区内还没有长期的定量摄食数据。为了探索西马鸥的食性灵活性,我们分别于2010年9月至2016年1月和2017年5月至2018年9月在两个北部地点(安巴拉法里和萨哈沃拉)和一个南部地点(瓦托瓦维)收集了数据。我们通过对安巴拉法里的一个群、萨哈沃拉和瓦托瓦维各两个群的成年雄性和雌性进行瞬时取样,总共记录了4022小时的行为数据。我们记录了 7 年间西马豹食用的 45 种植物。我们还观察到研究地点之间季节性食物组成的明显差异。在安巴拉法里,竹子是观察到的最常食用的资源(92.2%);然而,在萨哈沃拉和瓦托瓦维,非竹子资源占褐马鸡食物的近三分之一,超过60%。在Ambalafary的旱季和Vatovavy的雨季,所有竹子资源的摄食量都有所增加,但在后者从未超过非竹子的摄食量。只有在安巴拉法里观察到秆髓取食,而且在旱季更为常见。我们认为竹荚鲉是一种竹子专食动物,能够根据环境调整其取食行为,这表明它的食性比以前记录的更灵活,这可能使该物种能够在日益退化的生境中生存。
{"title":"Dietary flexibility of the greater bamboo lemur (Prolemur simus), a specialized feeder, in eastern Madagascar","authors":"T. Hasimija Mihaminekena,&nbsp;Ando N. Rakotonanahary,&nbsp;Cynthia L. Frasier,&nbsp;Hery N. T. Randriahaingo,&nbsp;Timothy M. Sefczek,&nbsp;Jen Tinsman,&nbsp;H. Lucien Randrianarimanana,&nbsp;Maholy Ravaloharimanitra,&nbsp;Toky Hery Rakotoarinivo,&nbsp;Jonah Ratsimbazafy,&nbsp;Tony King,&nbsp;Edward E. Louis","doi":"10.1002/ajp.23609","DOIUrl":"10.1002/ajp.23609","url":null,"abstract":"<p>The degree of dietary flexibility in primates is species specific; some incorporate a wider array of resources than others. Extreme interannual weather variability in Madagascar results in seasonal resource scarcity which has been linked to specialized behaviors in lemurs. <i>Prolemur simus</i>, for example, has been considered an obligate specialist on large culm bamboo with &gt;60% of its diet composed of woody bamboos requiring morphological and physiological adaptations to process. Recent studies reported an ever-expanding list of dietary items, suggesting that this species may not be an obligate specialist. However, long-term quantitative feeding data are unavailable across this species’ range. To explore the dietary flexibility of <i>P. simus</i>, we collected data at two northern sites, Ambalafary and Sahavola, and one southern site, Vatovavy, from September 2010 to January 2016 and May 2017 to September 2018, respectively. In total, we recorded 4022 h of behavioral data using instantaneous sampling of adult males and females from one group in Ambalafary, and two groups each in Sahavola and Vatovavy. We recorded 45 plant species eaten by <i>P. simus</i> over 7 years. We also observed significant differences in seasonal dietary composition between study sites. In Ambalafary, bamboo was the most frequently observed resource consumed (92.2%); however, non-bamboo resources comprised nearly one-third of the diet of <i>P. simus</i> in Sahavola and over 60% in Vatovavy. Consumption of all bamboo resources increased during the dry season at Ambalafary and during the wet season at Vatovavy, but never exceeded non-bamboo feeding at the latter. Culm pith feeding was only observed at Ambalafary, where it was more common during the dry season. We identify <i>P. simus</i> as a bamboo facultative specialist capable of adjusting its feeding behavior to its environment, indicating greater dietary flexibility than previously documented, which may enable the species to survive in increasingly degraded habitats.</p>","PeriodicalId":7662,"journal":{"name":"American Journal of Primatology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.4,"publicationDate":"2024-02-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/ajp.23609","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139970657","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Sniffing behavior of semi free-ranging Barbary macaques (Macaca sylvanus) 半自由放养的巴巴里猕猴(Macaca sylvanus)的嗅探行为。
IF 2.4 3区 生物学 Q1 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2024-02-26 DOI: 10.1002/ajp.23611
Miriam Simon, Anja Widdig, Brigitte M. Weiß

Olfaction is one of the evolutionarily oldest senses and plays a fundamental role in foraging and social interactions across mammals. In primates, the role of olfaction is now well recognized, but better investigated in strepsirrhine and platyrrhine primates than in catarrhines. We observed the sniffing behavior of semi-free ranging Barbary macaques, Macaca sylvanus, at Affenberg Salem, Germany, to assess how frequently macaques sniff and in which contexts, and how sniffing is affected by sex and age. Focal observations of 24 males and 24 females aged 1–25 years showed that Barbary macaques sniffed, on average, 5.24 times per hour, with more than 80% of sniffs directed at food. Irrespective of the context, younger individuals sniffed more often than older ones. Females’ sniffs were more often directed at food than male sniffs, while males sniffed more often than females in a social context. Sniffs at conspecifics occurred primarily in a sexual context, with 70% of social sniffs directed at female anogenital swellings performed by males. Of the observed 176 anogenital inspections, 51 involved sniffing of the swelling. Olfactory inspections were followed by copulation significantly less often than merely visual inspections, suggesting that anogenital odors may play a role in male mating decisions, but the role of olfaction in sexual interactions warrants further investigations. In sum, results show that Barbary macaques routinely use olfaction during feeding, but also in a socio-sexual context, corroborating the relevance of the olfactory sense in the lives of catarrhine primates.

嗅觉是进化过程中最古老的感官之一,在哺乳动物的觅食和社会交往中扮演着重要角色。在灵长类动物中,嗅觉的作用现已得到广泛认可,但对链胸类和板胸类灵长类动物的研究要好于猫科动物。我们在德国阿芬贝格塞勒姆(Affenberg Salem)观察了半自由活动的巴巴里猕猴(Macaca sylvanus)的嗅探行为,以评估猕猴嗅探的频率和环境,以及嗅探受性别和年龄的影响。对 24 只 1-25 岁的雄性和 24 只雌性猕猴进行的重点观察表明,巴巴利猕猴平均每小时嗅闻 5.24 次,其中超过 80% 的嗅闻是针对食物的。无论在什么情况下,年幼的猕猴都比年长的猕猴嗅得更勤。雌性嗅闻食物的次数多于雄性,而雄性在社交场合的嗅闻次数多于雌性。对同类的嗅闻主要发生在性场合,70%的社交嗅闻是由雄性针对雌性生殖器肿胀进行的。在观察到的 176 次肛门检查中,有 51 次涉及嗅闻肿胀部位。嗅觉检查之后进行交配的频率明显低于视觉检查,这表明雌性生殖器的气味可能在雄性交配决定中起到一定作用,但嗅觉在性互动中的作用还需要进一步研究。总之,研究结果表明,巴巴利猕猴不仅在进食过程中经常使用嗅觉,而且在社会性活动中也经常使用嗅觉,这证实了嗅觉在猫科灵长类动物生活中的重要性。
{"title":"Sniffing behavior of semi free-ranging Barbary macaques (Macaca sylvanus)","authors":"Miriam Simon,&nbsp;Anja Widdig,&nbsp;Brigitte M. Weiß","doi":"10.1002/ajp.23611","DOIUrl":"10.1002/ajp.23611","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Olfaction is one of the evolutionarily oldest senses and plays a fundamental role in foraging and social interactions across mammals. In primates, the role of olfaction is now well recognized, but better investigated in strepsirrhine and platyrrhine primates than in catarrhines. We observed the sniffing behavior of semi-free ranging Barbary macaques, <i>Macaca sylvanus</i>, at Affenberg Salem, Germany, to assess how frequently macaques sniff and in which contexts, and how sniffing is affected by sex and age. Focal observations of 24 males and 24 females aged 1–25 years showed that Barbary macaques sniffed, on average, 5.24 times per hour, with more than 80% of sniffs directed at food. Irrespective of the context, younger individuals sniffed more often than older ones. Females’ sniffs were more often directed at food than male sniffs, while males sniffed more often than females in a social context. Sniffs at conspecifics occurred primarily in a sexual context, with 70% of social sniffs directed at female anogenital swellings performed by males. Of the observed 176 anogenital inspections, 51 involved sniffing of the swelling. Olfactory inspections were followed by copulation significantly less often than merely visual inspections, suggesting that anogenital odors may play a role in male mating decisions, but the role of olfaction in sexual interactions warrants further investigations. In sum, results show that Barbary macaques routinely use olfaction during feeding, but also in a socio-sexual context, corroborating the relevance of the olfactory sense in the lives of catarrhine primates.</p>","PeriodicalId":7662,"journal":{"name":"American Journal of Primatology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.4,"publicationDate":"2024-02-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/ajp.23611","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139970658","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Edge effects and social behavior in three platyrrhines. 边缘效应和三种板羽目动物的社会行为。
IF 2.4 3区 生物学 Q1 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2024-02-24 DOI: 10.1002/ajp.23610
Laura M Bolt, Jenna L Owens, Madison Taylor Grant, Elizabeth M C Coggeshall, Dorian G Russell, Carrie Merrigan-Johnson, Zachary S Jacobson, Zachary T Schmidt, Francesca V E Kaser, Amy L Schreier

Social behavior is a key adaptation for group-living primates. It is important to assess changes to social behavior in human-impacted landscape zones to better understand the impact of anthropogenic disturbance on primate species. We investigated social behavior rate and type in three species of platyrrhines across 100 m anthropogenic edge and interior zones of a fragmented forest in Costa Rica, La Suerte Biological Research Station (LSBRS). Following results from other sites, we predicted that spider monkeys (Ateles geoffroyi), capuchin monkeys (Cebus imitator) and howler monkeys (Alouatta palliata) would show lower rates and fewer types of social behavior in forest edge compared to interior. We collected 1341 h of instantaneous focal data from 2017 to 2023 across the three monkey species. We found mixed support for our predictions, with spider and capuchin monkeys modifying some but not all aspects of social behavior across forest zones at LSBRS. Spider monkeys had lower rates of social behavior and capuchin monkeys performed different types of social behaviors in forest edge compared to interior at LSBRS. In contrast, howler monkeys did not modify social behavior. Two out of three platyrrhine species altered their social behavior when in anthropogenic edges, indicating behavioral adjustment when in human-altered habitat areas at LSBRS.

对于群居灵长类动物来说,社会行为是一种关键的适应能力。为了更好地了解人为干扰对灵长类物种的影响,评估人类影响景观带中社会行为的变化非常重要。我们调查了哥斯达黎加拉苏埃特生物研究站(LSBRS)一片破碎森林的100米人为边缘区和内部区中三种板翅类灵长类动物的社会行为率和类型。根据其他研究地点的结果,我们预测蜘蛛猴(Ateles geoffroyi)、卷尾猴(Cebus imitator)和吼猴(Alouatta palliata)在森林边缘地带的社会行为发生率和类型将低于森林内部地带。我们收集了这三种猴子从 2017 年到 2023 年 1341 小时的瞬时焦点数据。我们发现,我们的预测得到了不同程度的支持,蜘蛛猴和卷尾猴在LSBRS的不同林区会改变社会行为的某些方面,但不是所有方面。在LSBRS,蜘蛛猴的社会行为发生率较低,卷尾猴在森林边缘与森林内部的社会行为类型不同。相比之下,吼猴没有改变社会行为。三种长尾猴中有两种在人为边缘地带的社会行为发生了改变,这表明长尾猴在LSBRS被人类改变的栖息地区域进行了行为调整。
{"title":"Edge effects and social behavior in three platyrrhines.","authors":"Laura M Bolt, Jenna L Owens, Madison Taylor Grant, Elizabeth M C Coggeshall, Dorian G Russell, Carrie Merrigan-Johnson, Zachary S Jacobson, Zachary T Schmidt, Francesca V E Kaser, Amy L Schreier","doi":"10.1002/ajp.23610","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1002/ajp.23610","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Social behavior is a key adaptation for group-living primates. It is important to assess changes to social behavior in human-impacted landscape zones to better understand the impact of anthropogenic disturbance on primate species. We investigated social behavior rate and type in three species of platyrrhines across 100 m anthropogenic edge and interior zones of a fragmented forest in Costa Rica, La Suerte Biological Research Station (LSBRS). Following results from other sites, we predicted that spider monkeys (Ateles geoffroyi), capuchin monkeys (Cebus imitator) and howler monkeys (Alouatta palliata) would show lower rates and fewer types of social behavior in forest edge compared to interior. We collected 1341 h of instantaneous focal data from 2017 to 2023 across the three monkey species. We found mixed support for our predictions, with spider and capuchin monkeys modifying some but not all aspects of social behavior across forest zones at LSBRS. Spider monkeys had lower rates of social behavior and capuchin monkeys performed different types of social behaviors in forest edge compared to interior at LSBRS. In contrast, howler monkeys did not modify social behavior. Two out of three platyrrhine species altered their social behavior when in anthropogenic edges, indicating behavioral adjustment when in human-altered habitat areas at LSBRS.</p>","PeriodicalId":7662,"journal":{"name":"American Journal of Primatology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.4,"publicationDate":"2024-02-24","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139943780","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Rapid facial mimicry in Platyrrhini: Play face replication in spider monkeys (Ateles fusciceps, Ateles hybridus, and Ateles paniscus) Platyrrhini 的快速面部模仿:蜘蛛猴(Ateles fusciceps、Ateles hybridus 和 Ateles paniscus)的面部模仿游戏。
IF 2.4 3区 生物学 Q1 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2024-02-18 DOI: 10.1002/ajp.23607
Giada Cordoni, Annalisa Ciantia, Jean-Pascal Guéry, Baptiste Mulot, Ivan Norscia

Rapid facial mimicry (RFM), the rapid and automatic replication of facial expression perceived, is considered a basic form of empathy and was investigated mainly during play. RFM occurs in Catarrhini (Old World primates), but it is not still demonstrated in Platyrrhini (New World primates). For this reason, we collected video data on playful interactions (Nplay_interactions = 149) in three species of spider monkeys (Ateles fuscicepsN = 11, Ateles hybridusN = 14, and Ateles paniscusN = 6) housed at La Vallée des Singes and the ZooParc de Beauval (France). For the first time, we demonstrated the occurrence of RFM in Platyrrhini (analyzing 175 events). Players' sex, age, species, relationship quality, and kinship did not modulate RFM probably due to the species' complex fission–fusion dynamics and flexible interindividual social relationships. Compared to the absence of any playful expressions or the presence of only not replicated play face, RFM prolonged the session duration and was sequentially associated with more types of more intense offensive playful patterns (patterns aimed at attacking/pursuing the playmate). We proposed that RFM may favor synchronization and context sharing between players, thus decreasing the risk of behavior misinterpretation while simultaneously fostering a more competitive nature of play. In conclusion, this study stimulates additional research on the evolutionary origins of motor mimicry in primates, possibly dating back to before the divergence of New and Old World monkeys. Furthermore, it also points toward the possibility that RFM may not always lead to cooperation but also to competition, depending on the context and species' social and cognitive features.

快速面部模仿(RFM),即快速自动复制所感知的面部表情,被认为是移情的一种基本形式,主要是在游戏中进行研究。RFM在猫科动物(旧大陆灵长类)中出现,但在蝶形灵长类(新大陆灵长类)中还没有表现出来。为此,我们收集了三种蜘蛛猴(Ateles fusciceps-N = 11、Ateles hybridus-N = 14 和 Ateles paniscus-N = 6)的游戏互动视频数据(Nplay_interactions = 149),这些蜘蛛猴分别饲养在法国辛格山谷和博瓦尔动物园(ZooParc de Beauval)。我们首次证明了RFM在鸻中的出现(分析了175个事件)。嬉戏者的性别、年龄、种类、关系质量和亲缘关系对 RFM 没有调节作用,这可能是由于该物种复杂的裂变融合动态和灵活的个体间社会关系。与没有任何嬉戏表情或仅有不重复的嬉戏表情相比,RFM 延长了游戏持续时间,并依次与更多类型、更强烈的攻击性嬉戏模式(旨在攻击/追逐玩伴的模式)相关联。我们认为,RFM 可能有利于游戏者之间的同步和情境共享,从而降低行为被误解的风险,同时促进游戏的竞争性。总之,这项研究激发了对灵长类动物运动模仿进化起源的更多研究,可能可以追溯到新世界猴和旧世界猴分化之前。此外,这项研究还指出,RFM 可能并不总是导致合作,也可能导致竞争,这取决于环境和物种的社会和认知特征。
{"title":"Rapid facial mimicry in Platyrrhini: Play face replication in spider monkeys (Ateles fusciceps, Ateles hybridus, and Ateles paniscus)","authors":"Giada Cordoni,&nbsp;Annalisa Ciantia,&nbsp;Jean-Pascal Guéry,&nbsp;Baptiste Mulot,&nbsp;Ivan Norscia","doi":"10.1002/ajp.23607","DOIUrl":"10.1002/ajp.23607","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Rapid facial mimicry (RFM), the rapid and automatic replication of facial expression perceived, is considered a basic form of empathy and was investigated mainly during play. RFM occurs in Catarrhini (Old World primates), but it is not still demonstrated in Platyrrhini (New World primates). For this reason, we collected video data on playful interactions (<i>N</i><sub>play_interactions</sub> = 149) in three species of spider monkeys (<i>Ateles fusciceps</i>—<i>N</i> = 11, <i>Ateles hybridus</i>—<i>N</i> = 14, and <i>Ateles paniscus</i>—<i>N</i> = 6) housed at La Vallée des Singes and the ZooParc de Beauval (France). For the first time, we demonstrated the occurrence of RFM in Platyrrhini (analyzing 175 events). Players' sex, age, species, relationship quality, and kinship did not modulate RFM probably due to the species' complex fission–fusion dynamics and flexible interindividual social relationships. Compared to the absence of any playful expressions or the presence of only not replicated play face, RFM prolonged the session duration and was sequentially associated with more types of more intense offensive playful patterns (patterns aimed at attacking/pursuing the playmate). We proposed that RFM may favor synchronization and context sharing between players, thus decreasing the risk of behavior misinterpretation while simultaneously fostering a more competitive nature of play. In conclusion, this study stimulates additional research on the evolutionary origins of motor mimicry in primates, possibly dating back to before the divergence of New and Old World monkeys. Furthermore, it also points toward the possibility that RFM may not always lead to cooperation but also to competition, depending on the context and species' social and cognitive features.</p>","PeriodicalId":7662,"journal":{"name":"American Journal of Primatology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.4,"publicationDate":"2024-02-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139899157","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Stubby versus stabby: A preliminary analysis of canine microwear in primates: Implication for inferring ingestive behaviors 粗壮与粗壮:灵长类动物犬类微磨损的初步分析:推断摄食行为的意义。
IF 2.4 3区 生物学 Q1 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2024-02-14 DOI: 10.1002/ajp.23608
Putu Pujiantari, Lucas K. Delezene, J. Michael Plavcan, Mark F. Teaford, Peter S. Ungar

Molar and incisor microwear reflect aspects of food choice and ingestive behaviors in living primates and have both been used to infer the same for fossil samples. Canine microwear, however, has received less attention, perhaps because of the prominent role canines play in social display and because they are used as weapons–while outside of a few specialized cases, their involvement in diet related behaviors has not been obvious. Here, we posit that microwear can also provide glimpses into canine tooth use in ingestion. Canines of Sumatran long-tailed macaques (Macaca fascicularis), agile gibbons (Hylobates agilis), lar gibbons (Hylobates lar), Thomas' leaf monkeys (Presbytis thomasi), and orangutans (Pongo abelii), and two African great apes, bonobos (Pan paniscus) and common chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii), were considered. The labial tips of maxillary canine replicas were scanned using a white-light confocal profiler, and both feature and texture analyses were used to characterize microwear surface patterning. The taxa exhibited significant differences in canine microwear. In some cases, these were consistent with variation in reported anterior tooth use such that, for example, the orangutans, known to use their front teeth extensively in ingestion, had the highest median number of microwear features on their canines, whereas the gibbons, reported to use their front teeth infrequently in food acquisition, had the lowest.

臼齿和门齿的微观磨损反映了生活灵长类动物的食物选择和摄食行为,也被用来推断化石样本的相同情况。然而,犬齿的微观磨损却较少受到关注,这可能是因为犬齿在社交展示中扮演着重要角色,而且还被用作武器--同时,除了少数特殊情况外,犬齿与饮食相关行为的关系并不明显。在这里,我们认为,微磨损也能让人窥见犬齿在摄食中的用途。我们研究了苏门答腊长尾猕猴(Macaca fascicularis)、敏捷长臂猿(Hylobates agilis)、长臂猿(Hylobates lar)、托马斯叶猴(Presbytis thomasi)、猩猩(Pongo abelii)以及两种非洲类人猿倭黑猩猩(Pan paniscus)和普通黑猩猩(Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii)的犬齿。使用白光共聚焦轮廓仪对上颌犬齿复制品的唇尖进行扫描,并使用特征和纹理分析来确定微磨损表面图案的特征。这些类群在犬齿微磨损方面表现出明显的差异。在某些情况下,这些差异与所报道的前牙使用情况的差异是一致的,例如,已知猩猩在摄取食物时会大量使用前牙,因此它们的犬齿微磨损特征的中位数最高,而据报道长臂猿在获取食物时很少使用前牙,因此它们的犬齿微磨损特征的中位数最低。
{"title":"Stubby versus stabby: A preliminary analysis of canine microwear in primates: Implication for inferring ingestive behaviors","authors":"Putu Pujiantari,&nbsp;Lucas K. Delezene,&nbsp;J. Michael Plavcan,&nbsp;Mark F. Teaford,&nbsp;Peter S. Ungar","doi":"10.1002/ajp.23608","DOIUrl":"10.1002/ajp.23608","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Molar and incisor microwear reflect aspects of food choice and ingestive behaviors in living primates and have both been used to infer the same for fossil samples. Canine microwear, however, has received less attention, perhaps because of the prominent role canines play in social display and because they are used as weapons–while outside of a few specialized cases, their involvement in diet related behaviors has not been obvious. Here, we posit that microwear can also provide glimpses into canine tooth use in ingestion. Canines of Sumatran long-tailed macaques (<i>Macaca fascicularis</i>), agile gibbons (<i>Hylobates agilis</i>), lar gibbons (<i>Hylobates lar</i>), Thomas' leaf monkeys (<i>Presbytis thomasi</i>), and orangutans (<i>Pongo abelii</i>), and two African great apes, bonobos (<i>Pan paniscus</i>) and common chimpanzees (<i>Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii</i>), were considered. The labial tips of maxillary canine replicas were scanned using a white-light confocal profiler, and both feature and texture analyses were used to characterize microwear surface patterning. The taxa exhibited significant differences in canine microwear. In some cases, these were consistent with variation in reported anterior tooth use such that, for example, the orangutans, known to use their front teeth extensively in ingestion, had the highest median number of microwear features on their canines, whereas the gibbons, reported to use their front teeth infrequently in food acquisition, had the lowest.</p>","PeriodicalId":7662,"journal":{"name":"American Journal of Primatology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.4,"publicationDate":"2024-02-14","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139728781","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Excess prenatal loss and respiratory illnesses of infant macaques living outdoors and exposed to wildfire smoke 生活在户外并暴露于野火烟雾中的猕猴婴儿产前损失过多和呼吸道疾病。
IF 2.4 3区 生物学 Q1 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2024-02-11 DOI: 10.1002/ajp.23605
Kathryn Berns, Andrew J. Haertel

Global climate change has transformed predictions of fire seasons in the near future, and record-breaking wildfire events have had catastrophic consequences in recent years. In September 2020, multiple wildfires subjected Oregon to hazardous air quality for several days. In this retrospective cohort study, we aimed to examine prenatal loss, morbidity, and mortality of rhesus (Macaca mulatta) and Japanese macaques (Macaca fuscata) exposed to poor air quality from the nearby wildfires. Detailed medical records from 2014 to 2020 of 580 macaques housed outdoors at a research facility in Beaverton, Oregon were used to evaluate the association between these health outcomes and wildfire smoke exposure. Logistic regression models estimated excess prenatal loss, hospitalization rates, respiratory problems, and mortality during and following the wildfire event, and Kruskal–Wallis statistics were used to determine if infant growth was affected by wildfire smoke exposure. Risk of pregnancy loss (relative risk = 4.1; p < 0.001) and odds of diagnosis with a respiratory problem (odds ratio = 4.47; p = 0.003) were higher in exposed infant macaques compared to nonexposed infants. Infant growth was not affected by poor air quality exposure. Our findings suggest wildfire smoke exposure poses a risk to the health of infants and pregnant individuals and should be monitored more closely in the future.

全球气候变化改变了对近期火灾季节的预测,近年来破纪录的野火事件造成了灾难性后果。2020 年 9 月,俄勒冈州的多场野火导致空气质量连续数天处于危险状态。在这项回顾性队列研究中,我们旨在研究暴露于附近野火造成的恶劣空气质量下的猕猴(Macaca mulatta)和日本猕猴(Macaca fuscata)的产前损失、发病率和死亡率。研究人员利用俄勒冈州比弗顿一家研究机构室外饲养的 580 只猕猴在 2014 年至 2020 年期间的详细医疗记录,评估了这些健康结果与野火烟雾暴露之间的关联。逻辑回归模型估算了野火事件期间和之后的超额产前损失、住院率、呼吸系统问题和死亡率,并使用 Kruskal-Wallis 统计法确定婴儿生长是否受到野火烟雾暴露的影响。妊娠损失风险(相对风险 = 4.1; p
{"title":"Excess prenatal loss and respiratory illnesses of infant macaques living outdoors and exposed to wildfire smoke","authors":"Kathryn Berns,&nbsp;Andrew J. Haertel","doi":"10.1002/ajp.23605","DOIUrl":"10.1002/ajp.23605","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Global climate change has transformed predictions of fire seasons in the near future, and record-breaking wildfire events have had catastrophic consequences in recent years. In September 2020, multiple wildfires subjected Oregon to hazardous air quality for several days. In this retrospective cohort study, we aimed to examine prenatal loss, morbidity, and mortality of rhesus (<i>Macaca mulatta</i>) and Japanese macaques (<i>Macaca fuscata</i>) exposed to poor air quality from the nearby wildfires. Detailed medical records from 2014 to 2020 of 580 macaques housed outdoors at a research facility in Beaverton, Oregon were used to evaluate the association between these health outcomes and wildfire smoke exposure. Logistic regression models estimated excess prenatal loss, hospitalization rates, respiratory problems, and mortality during and following the wildfire event, and Kruskal–Wallis statistics were used to determine if infant growth was affected by wildfire smoke exposure. Risk of pregnancy loss (relative risk = 4.1; <i>p</i> &lt; 0.001) and odds of diagnosis with a respiratory problem (odds ratio = 4.47; <i>p</i> = 0.003) were higher in exposed infant macaques compared to nonexposed infants. Infant growth was not affected by poor air quality exposure. Our findings suggest wildfire smoke exposure poses a risk to the health of infants and pregnant individuals and should be monitored more closely in the future.</p>","PeriodicalId":7662,"journal":{"name":"American Journal of Primatology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.4,"publicationDate":"2024-02-11","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139717320","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Pied tamarins change their vocal behavior in response to noise levels in the largest city in the Amazon 在亚马逊最大的城市里,雌狨会根据噪音水平改变发声行为。
IF 2.4 3区 生物学 Q1 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2024-02-10 DOI: 10.1002/ajp.23606
Tainara Venturini Sobroza, Marcelo Gordo, Jacob C. Dunn, Pedro Aurélio Costa Lima Pequeno, Bruna Mendel Naissinger, Adrian Paul Ashton Barnett

Many animal species depend on sound to communicate with conspecifics. However, human-generated (anthropogenic) noise may mask acoustic signals and so disrupt behavior. Animals may use various strategies to circumvent this, including shifts in the timing of vocal activity and changes to the acoustic parameters of their calls. We tested whether pied tamarins (Saguinus bicolor) adjust their vocal behavior in response to city noise. We predicted that both the probability of occurrence and the number of long calls would increase in response to anthropogenic noise and that pied tamarins would temporally shift their vocal activity to avoid noisier periods. At a finer scale, we anticipated that the temporal parameters of tamarin calls (e.g., call duration and syllable repetition rate) would increase with noise amplitude. We collected information on the acoustic environment and the emission of long calls in nine wild pied tamarin groups in Manaus, Brazil. We found that the probability of long-call occurrence increased with higher levels of anthropogenic noise, though the number of long calls did not. The number of long calls was related to the time of day and the distance from home range borders—a proxy for the distance to neighboring groups. Neither long-call occurrence nor call rate was related to noise levels at different times of day. We found that pied tamarins decreased their syllable repetition rate in response to anthropogenic noise. Long calls are important for group cohesion and intergroup communication. Thus, it is possible that the tamarins emit one long call with lower syllable repetition, which might facilitate signal reception. The occurrence and quantity of pied tamarin' long calls, as well as their acoustic proprieties, seem to be governed by anthropogenic noise, time of the day, and social mechanisms such as proximity to neighboring groups.

许多动物依靠声音与同类交流。然而,人类产生的(人为)噪音可能会掩盖声音信号,从而干扰行为。动物可能会使用各种策略来规避这种情况,包括改变发声活动的时间和改变叫声的声学参数。我们测试了双色狨(Saguinus bicolor)是否会调整其发声行为以应对城市噪音。我们预测,长叫声的出现概率和数量都会随着人为噪声的增加而增加,而且雌狮驼会在时间上改变它们的发声活动,以避开噪声较高的时段。在更小的尺度上,我们预计狨叫声的时间参数(如叫声持续时间和音节重复率)将随噪声幅度的增加而增加。我们收集了巴西玛瑙斯 9 个野生鲮鱼群的声学环境和长叫声发射信息。我们发现,随着人为噪声水平的升高,长叫声出现的概率也会增加,但长叫声的数量却没有增加。长叫声的数量与一天中的时间和距离家园边界的远近有关--家园边界代表与邻近群体的距离。长叫发生率和长叫率都与一天中不同时间的噪音水平无关。我们发现,在人为噪声的影响下,姬松鼠的音节重复率降低了。长叫对于群体凝聚力和群体间交流非常重要。因此,玳瑁可能会发出一种音节重复率较低的长叫声,这可能有助于信号接收。禺狨长叫的发生和数量以及它们的声学特性似乎受人为噪声、一天中的时间和社会机制(如与邻近群体的距离)的影响。
{"title":"Pied tamarins change their vocal behavior in response to noise levels in the largest city in the Amazon","authors":"Tainara Venturini Sobroza,&nbsp;Marcelo Gordo,&nbsp;Jacob C. Dunn,&nbsp;Pedro Aurélio Costa Lima Pequeno,&nbsp;Bruna Mendel Naissinger,&nbsp;Adrian Paul Ashton Barnett","doi":"10.1002/ajp.23606","DOIUrl":"10.1002/ajp.23606","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Many animal species depend on sound to communicate with conspecifics. However, human-generated (anthropogenic) noise may mask acoustic signals and so disrupt behavior. Animals may use various strategies to circumvent this, including shifts in the timing of vocal activity and changes to the acoustic parameters of their calls. We tested whether pied tamarins (<i>Saguinus bicolor</i>) adjust their vocal behavior in response to city noise. We predicted that both the probability of occurrence and the number of long calls would increase in response to anthropogenic noise and that pied tamarins would temporally shift their vocal activity to avoid noisier periods. At a finer scale, we anticipated that the temporal parameters of tamarin calls (e.g., call duration and syllable repetition rate) would increase with noise amplitude. We collected information on the acoustic environment and the emission of long calls in nine wild pied tamarin groups in Manaus, Brazil. We found that the probability of long-call occurrence increased with higher levels of anthropogenic noise, though the number of long calls did not. The number of long calls was related to the time of day and the distance from home range borders—a proxy for the distance to neighboring groups. Neither long-call occurrence nor call rate was related to noise levels at different times of day. We found that pied tamarins decreased their syllable repetition rate in response to anthropogenic noise. Long calls are important for group cohesion and intergroup communication. Thus, it is possible that the tamarins emit one long call with lower syllable repetition, which might facilitate signal reception. The occurrence and quantity of pied tamarin' long calls, as well as their acoustic proprieties, seem to be governed by anthropogenic noise, time of the day, and social mechanisms such as proximity to neighboring groups.</p>","PeriodicalId":7662,"journal":{"name":"American Journal of Primatology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.4,"publicationDate":"2024-02-10","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139715737","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Correction to “Leopard predation on gelada monkeys at Guassa, Ethiopia” 对 "埃塞俄比亚瓜萨的豹子捕食狮尾狒 "的更正。
IF 2.4 3区 生物学 Q1 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2024-02-08 DOI: 10.1002/ajp.23604

Lin B, Foxfoot IR, Miller CM, et al. Leopard predation on gelada monkeys at Guassa, Ethiopia. Am J Primatol. 2020;82:e23098.

In the originally published version of this article, the fourth author's name was incorrectly spelled as Vivek V. Venkatamaran. The correct spelling is Vivek V. Venkataraman.

We apologize for this error.

Lin B、Foxfoot IR、Miller CM 等:豹对埃塞俄比亚 Guassa 的狮尾狒的捕食。Am J Primatol.2020;82:e23098.In the original published version of this article, the fourth author's name was incorrectly spelled as Vivek V. Venkatamaran.Venkatamaran。正确的拼写是 Vivek V. Venkataraman。Venkataraman.我们对此错误深表歉意。
{"title":"Correction to “Leopard predation on gelada monkeys at Guassa, Ethiopia”","authors":"","doi":"10.1002/ajp.23604","DOIUrl":"10.1002/ajp.23604","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Lin B, Foxfoot IR, Miller CM, et al. Leopard predation on gelada monkeys at Guassa, Ethiopia. <i>Am J Primatol</i>. 2020;82:e23098.</p><p>In the originally published version of this article, the fourth author's name was incorrectly spelled as Vivek V. Venkatamaran. The correct spelling is Vivek V. Venkataraman.</p><p>We apologize for this error.</p>","PeriodicalId":7662,"journal":{"name":"American Journal of Primatology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.4,"publicationDate":"2024-02-08","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/ajp.23604","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139705834","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Manual preference, performance, and dexterity for bimanual grass-feeding behavior in wild geladas (Theropithecus gelada) 野生狮尾狒(Theropithecus gelada)双足食草行为的手动偏好、表现和灵巧性。
IF 2.4 3区 生物学 Q1 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2024-02-01 DOI: 10.1002/ajp.23602
Valentina Truppa, Marco Gamba, Roberta Togliatto, Marta Caselli, Anna Zanoli, Elisabetta Palagi, Ivan Norscia

We assessed whether wild geladas, highly specialized terrestrial grass eaters, are lateralized for bimanual grass-plucking behavior. According to the literature, we expected that complex motor movements in grass feeding would favor the emergence of a population-level hand bias in these primates. In addition, we described geladas' manual behavior based on systematic observations of several individuals. Our study group included 28 individuals belonging to a population of free-ranging geladas frequenting the Kundi plateau, Ethiopia. We filmed monkeys while feeding on grass, and hand preference and performance were coded. Geladas performed more plucking movements per second with their left hand (LH) compared to the right one and preferred their LH both to start and finish collection bouts. Also, the rhythmic movements of each hand had a significant tendency toward isochrony. Finally, geladas used forceful pad-to-pad precision grips, in-hand movements, and compound grips to pluck and collect grass blades, considered the most advanced manual skills in primate species. The LH's leading role suggests an advantage of the right hemisphere in regulating geladas' bimanual grass-feeding behavior. The tactile input from the hands and/or rhythmic hand movements might contribute to explaining this pattern of laterality. Our findings highlighted the importance of adopting multiple laterality measures to investigate manual laterality. Moreover, the need to speed up the execution time of manual foraging might be a further important factor in studying the evolution of manual laterality and dexterity in primates.

我们评估了野生狮尾狒这种高度专业化的陆生食草动物是否在双手刨草行为上存在侧向性。根据文献资料,我们预计在这些灵长类动物中,吃草时复杂的运动会有利于出现群体水平的手部偏向。此外,我们还根据对几个个体的系统观察,描述了明胶的徒手行为。我们的研究小组包括28只个体,它们属于经常在埃塞俄比亚昆迪高原自由觅食的狮尾狒种群。我们拍摄了猴子吃草时的情景,并对它们的手部偏好和表现进行了编码。与右手相比,明胶猴每秒用左手(LH)做更多的采摘动作,并且在开始和结束采摘时都更喜欢用左手。此外,两只手的节奏动作有明显的等时性倾向。最后,猿猴使用有力的垫对垫精确抓握、手部运动和复合抓握来摘取和收集草叶,这被认为是灵长类动物中最先进的手工技能。LH的主导作用表明,右半球在调节猿类的双臂采草行为方面具有优势。来自手部的触觉输入和/或有节奏的手部运动可能有助于解释这种侧向模式。我们的研究结果突显了采用多种侧向性测量方法来研究徒手侧向性的重要性。此外,在研究灵长类的徒手侧向性和灵巧性进化时,加快徒手觅食执行时间的需要可能是另一个重要因素。
{"title":"Manual preference, performance, and dexterity for bimanual grass-feeding behavior in wild geladas (Theropithecus gelada)","authors":"Valentina Truppa,&nbsp;Marco Gamba,&nbsp;Roberta Togliatto,&nbsp;Marta Caselli,&nbsp;Anna Zanoli,&nbsp;Elisabetta Palagi,&nbsp;Ivan Norscia","doi":"10.1002/ajp.23602","DOIUrl":"10.1002/ajp.23602","url":null,"abstract":"<p>We assessed whether wild geladas, highly specialized terrestrial grass eaters, are lateralized for bimanual grass-plucking behavior. According to the literature, we expected that complex motor movements in grass feeding would favor the emergence of a population-level hand bias in these primates. In addition, we described geladas' manual behavior based on systematic observations of several individuals. Our study group included 28 individuals belonging to a population of free-ranging geladas frequenting the Kundi plateau, Ethiopia. We filmed monkeys while feeding on grass, and hand preference and performance were coded. Geladas performed more plucking movements per second with their left hand (LH) compared to the right one and preferred their LH both to start and finish collection bouts. Also, the rhythmic movements of each hand had a significant tendency toward isochrony. Finally, geladas used forceful pad-to-pad precision grips, in-hand movements, and compound grips to pluck and collect grass blades, considered the most advanced manual skills in primate species. The LH's leading role suggests an advantage of the right hemisphere in regulating geladas' bimanual grass-feeding behavior. The tactile input from the hands and/or rhythmic hand movements might contribute to explaining this pattern of laterality. Our findings highlighted the importance of adopting multiple laterality measures to investigate manual laterality. Moreover, the need to speed up the execution time of manual foraging might be a further important factor in studying the evolution of manual laterality and dexterity in primates.</p>","PeriodicalId":7662,"journal":{"name":"American Journal of Primatology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.4,"publicationDate":"2024-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139650106","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
American Journal of Primatology
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1