首页 > 最新文献

Animal Cognition最新文献

英文 中文
Bolder together: conformity drives behavioral plasticity in eastern gartersnakes. 更大胆地团结在一起:顺应潮流是东部袜带蛇行为可塑性的驱动力。
IF 2.7 2区 生物学 Q1 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2024-02-22 DOI: 10.1007/s10071-024-01859-5
Morgan Skinner, Gokulan Nagabaskaran, Tom Gantert, Noam Miller

Personality traits drive individual differences in behaviour that are consistent across time and context. Personality limits behavioural plasticity, which could lead to maladaptive choices if animals cannot adapt their behavior to changing conditions. Here, we assessed consistency and flexibility in one personality trait, boldness, across non-social and social contexts in eastern gartersnakes (Thamnophis sirtalis sirtalis). Snakes explored a novel open arena either alone or in a pair. Pairs were assigned based on the data from the solo trials, such that each snake was paired once with a bolder and once with a less bold partner. We predicted that snakes would conform when in a social context, displaying plasticity in their personality, and causing boldness scores to converge. We found that snakes were consistent within contexts (solo or paired), but changed their behavior across contexts (from solo to paired). Plasticity in boldness resulted from an interaction between conformity and repeatable individual differences in plasticity. In line with some data on other species, snakes conformed more when they were the less bold partner. Personality reflects a consistent bias in decision-making, but our results highlight that the cognitive processes that drive the expression of personality traits in behavior are flexible and sensitive to social context. We show that both consistency and plasticity combine to shape snake social behavior in ways that are responsive to competition. This pattern of behavior may be particularly beneficial for species in which group-living is seasonal.

性格特征会导致行为的个体差异,这种差异在不同的时间和环境下都是一致的。性格限制了行为的可塑性,如果动物不能使自己的行为适应不断变化的条件,就可能导致不适应性选择。在这里,我们评估了东部吊环蛇(Thamnophis sirtalis sirtalis)的一种个性特征--胆量--在非社会和社会环境中的一致性和灵活性。蛇单独或结对探索一个新颖的开放场。根据单独试验的数据分配配对,每条蛇与胆大的蛇和胆小的蛇配对一次。我们预测蛇在社会环境中会表现出一致性,从而显示出其个性的可塑性,并使胆量得分趋于一致。我们发现,蛇在不同的情境下(单人或配对)表现一致,但在不同的情境下(从单人到配对)行为会发生变化。胆量的可塑性是一致性与可重复的个体可塑性差异相互作用的结果。与其他物种的一些数据相一致,当蛇类是胆量较小的伙伴时,它们会更加顺从。人格反映了决策中的一致性偏差,但我们的研究结果突出表明,驱动人格特征在行为中表现的认知过程是灵活的,并且对社会环境非常敏感。我们的研究结果表明,一致性和可塑性共同塑造了蛇的社会行为,使其对竞争做出反应。这种行为模式可能对群体生活具有季节性的物种特别有益。
{"title":"Bolder together: conformity drives behavioral plasticity in eastern gartersnakes.","authors":"Morgan Skinner, Gokulan Nagabaskaran, Tom Gantert, Noam Miller","doi":"10.1007/s10071-024-01859-5","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10071-024-01859-5","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Personality traits drive individual differences in behaviour that are consistent across time and context. Personality limits behavioural plasticity, which could lead to maladaptive choices if animals cannot adapt their behavior to changing conditions. Here, we assessed consistency and flexibility in one personality trait, boldness, across non-social and social contexts in eastern gartersnakes (Thamnophis sirtalis sirtalis). Snakes explored a novel open arena either alone or in a pair. Pairs were assigned based on the data from the solo trials, such that each snake was paired once with a bolder and once with a less bold partner. We predicted that snakes would conform when in a social context, displaying plasticity in their personality, and causing boldness scores to converge. We found that snakes were consistent within contexts (solo or paired), but changed their behavior across contexts (from solo to paired). Plasticity in boldness resulted from an interaction between conformity and repeatable individual differences in plasticity. In line with some data on other species, snakes conformed more when they were the less bold partner. Personality reflects a consistent bias in decision-making, but our results highlight that the cognitive processes that drive the expression of personality traits in behavior are flexible and sensitive to social context. We show that both consistency and plasticity combine to shape snake social behavior in ways that are responsive to competition. This pattern of behavior may be particularly beneficial for species in which group-living is seasonal.</p>","PeriodicalId":7879,"journal":{"name":"Animal Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.7,"publicationDate":"2024-02-22","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10884060/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139929647","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Human social buffer in goats and dogs. 山羊和狗的人类社会缓冲区。
IF 2.7 2区 生物学 Q1 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2024-02-14 DOI: 10.1007/s10071-024-01861-x
Anna Scandurra, Biagio D'Aniello, Maria Elena Pero, Claudia Pinelli, Alfredo Di Lucrezia, Raffaella Tudisco, Piera Iommelli, Vincenzo Mastellone, Pietro Lombardi

The primary goal of this study was to explore the social buffering effect that humans offer to goats and dogs with limited exposure to human socialization, particularly in situations involving interactions with unfamiliar humans. A total of 13 dogs and 14 goats were selected for the study, all of which had limited prior socialization with humans. Each animal was placed in a testing room with unfamiliar humans for 15 min. Three experimenters aimed to establish a comfortable environment, encouraging social interaction by offering food to the animals and assessing the animals' willingness to accept food and their response to being approached and petted. If both conditions were satisfied, the animals were classified as "social". If one or none of the conditions were met, the animals were classified as "not social". Cortisol levels were measured by collecting blood samples before and after the test. Non-parametric tests together with a GzLM showed that the effect of human social buffering in goats was different in comparison to dogs: goats exhibited higher cortisol levels after the test, while dogs did not show a significant change. Further analysis demonstrated that "social" goats had a lower likelihood of experiencing significant changes in cortisol levels than dogs. Thus, once human interactions are accepted, both species could benefit from social buffering. In summary, this study enhances our understanding of how dogs and goats respond to social interactions with humans in the social buffering effect.

本研究的主要目的是探索人类对山羊和狗的社交缓冲作用,尤其是在与陌生人类互动的情况下。本研究共选取了 13 只狗和 14 只山羊,它们之前与人类的社交接触都很有限。每只动物都被放置在一个测试室中,与不熟悉的人类相处 15 分钟。三位实验员旨在建立一个舒适的环境,通过提供食物鼓励动物进行社交互动,并评估动物接受食物的意愿以及对接近和抚摸的反应。如果两个条件都满足,动物就被归类为 "社交 "动物。如果满足其中一个条件或一个条件都不满足,则被归类为 "不社交"。通过在测试前后采集血液样本来测量皮质醇水平。非参数检验和 GzLM 显示,人类社交缓冲对山羊和狗的影响不同:山羊在测试后表现出更高的皮质醇水平,而狗则没有明显变化。进一步的分析表明,与狗相比,"社交 "山羊皮质醇水平发生显著变化的可能性较低。因此,一旦接受了人际交往,两种动物都能从社交缓冲中受益。总之,这项研究加深了我们对狗和山羊在社会缓冲效应中如何应对与人类的社会互动的理解。
{"title":"Human social buffer in goats and dogs.","authors":"Anna Scandurra, Biagio D'Aniello, Maria Elena Pero, Claudia Pinelli, Alfredo Di Lucrezia, Raffaella Tudisco, Piera Iommelli, Vincenzo Mastellone, Pietro Lombardi","doi":"10.1007/s10071-024-01861-x","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10071-024-01861-x","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The primary goal of this study was to explore the social buffering effect that humans offer to goats and dogs with limited exposure to human socialization, particularly in situations involving interactions with unfamiliar humans. A total of 13 dogs and 14 goats were selected for the study, all of which had limited prior socialization with humans. Each animal was placed in a testing room with unfamiliar humans for 15 min. Three experimenters aimed to establish a comfortable environment, encouraging social interaction by offering food to the animals and assessing the animals' willingness to accept food and their response to being approached and petted. If both conditions were satisfied, the animals were classified as \"social\". If one or none of the conditions were met, the animals were classified as \"not social\". Cortisol levels were measured by collecting blood samples before and after the test. Non-parametric tests together with a GzLM showed that the effect of human social buffering in goats was different in comparison to dogs: goats exhibited higher cortisol levels after the test, while dogs did not show a significant change. Further analysis demonstrated that \"social\" goats had a lower likelihood of experiencing significant changes in cortisol levels than dogs. Thus, once human interactions are accepted, both species could benefit from social buffering. In summary, this study enhances our understanding of how dogs and goats respond to social interactions with humans in the social buffering effect.</p>","PeriodicalId":7879,"journal":{"name":"Animal Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.7,"publicationDate":"2024-02-14","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10866781/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139728823","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
On being a Hydra with, and without, a nervous system: what do neurons add? 有神经系统和没有神经系统的九头蛇:神经元能增加什么?
IF 2.7 2区 生物学 Q1 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2023-11-01 Epub Date: 2023-08-04 DOI: 10.1007/s10071-023-01816-8
Alison Hanson

The small freshwater cnidarian Hydra has been the subject of scientific inquiry for over 300 years due to its remarkable regenerative capacities and apparent immortality. More recently, Hydra has been recognized as an excellent model system within neuroscience because of its small size, transparency, and simple nervous system, which allow high-resolution imaging of its entire nerve net while behaving. In less than a decade, studies of Hydra's nervous system have yielded insights into the activity of neural circuits in vivo unobtainable in most other animals. In addition to these unique attributes, there is yet another lesser-known feature of Hydra that makes it even more intriguing: it does not require its neural hardware to live. The extraordinary ability to survive the removal and replacement of its entire nervous system makes Hydra uniquely suited to address the question of what neurons add to an extant organism. Here, I will review what early work on nerve-free Hydra reveals about the potential role of the nervous system in these animals and point towards future directions for this work.

300 多年来,小型淡水刺胞动物水螅一直是科学研究的主题,因为它具有非凡的再生能力和明显的永生性。最近,水螅因其体积小、透明度高和神经系统简单而被认为是神经科学领域的一个极好的模型系统。在不到十年的时间里,通过对水螅神经系统的研究,人们深入了解了体内神经回路的活动,这是其他大多数动物所无法企及的。除了这些独特的属性外,水螅还有一个鲜为人知的特点,使其更加引人入胜:它不需要神经硬件就能生存。水螅在整个神经系统被移除和替换后仍能存活的非凡能力,使它成为解决 "神经元能为现存生物体带来什么 "这一问题的独一无二的选择。在这里,我将回顾关于无神经水螅的早期研究揭示了神经系统在这些动物中的潜在作用,并指出这项工作的未来方向。
{"title":"On being a Hydra with, and without, a nervous system: what do neurons add?","authors":"Alison Hanson","doi":"10.1007/s10071-023-01816-8","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10071-023-01816-8","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The small freshwater cnidarian Hydra has been the subject of scientific inquiry for over 300 years due to its remarkable regenerative capacities and apparent immortality. More recently, Hydra has been recognized as an excellent model system within neuroscience because of its small size, transparency, and simple nervous system, which allow high-resolution imaging of its entire nerve net while behaving. In less than a decade, studies of Hydra's nervous system have yielded insights into the activity of neural circuits in vivo unobtainable in most other animals. In addition to these unique attributes, there is yet another lesser-known feature of Hydra that makes it even more intriguing: it does not require its neural hardware to live. The extraordinary ability to survive the removal and replacement of its entire nervous system makes Hydra uniquely suited to address the question of what neurons add to an extant organism. Here, I will review what early work on nerve-free Hydra reveals about the potential role of the nervous system in these animals and point towards future directions for this work.</p>","PeriodicalId":7879,"journal":{"name":"Animal Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.7,"publicationDate":"2023-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10770230/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9935106","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Bioelectric networks: the cognitive glue enabling evolutionary scaling from physiology to mind. 生物电网络:实现从生理到心智进化的认知粘合剂。
IF 2.7 2区 生物学 Q1 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2023-11-01 Epub Date: 2023-05-19 DOI: 10.1007/s10071-023-01780-3
Michael Levin

Each of us made the remarkable journey from mere matter to mind: starting life as a quiescent oocyte ("just chemistry and physics"), and slowly, gradually, becoming an adult human with complex metacognitive processes, hopes, and dreams. In addition, even though we feel ourselves to be a unified, single Self, distinct from the emergent dynamics of termite mounds and other swarms, the reality is that all intelligence is collective intelligence: each of us consists of a huge number of cells working together to generate a coherent cognitive being with goals, preferences, and memories that belong to the whole and not to its parts. Basal cognition is the quest to understand how Mind scales-how large numbers of competent subunits can work together to become intelligences that expand the scale of their possible goals. Crucially, the remarkable trick of turning homeostatic, cell-level physiological competencies into large-scale behavioral intelligences is not limited to the electrical dynamics of the brain. Evolution was using bioelectric signaling long before neurons and muscles appeared, to solve the problem of creating and repairing complex bodies. In this Perspective, I review the deep symmetry between the intelligence of developmental morphogenesis and that of classical behavior. I describe the highly conserved mechanisms that enable the collective intelligence of cells to implement regulative embryogenesis, regeneration, and cancer suppression. I sketch the story of an evolutionary pivot that repurposed the algorithms and cellular machinery that enable navigation of morphospace into the behavioral navigation of the 3D world which we so readily recognize as intelligence. Understanding the bioelectric dynamics that underlie construction of complex bodies and brains provides an essential path to understanding the natural evolution, and bioengineered design, of diverse intelligences within and beyond the phylogenetic history of Earth.

我们每个人都经历了从单纯的物质到心灵的非凡历程:从静止的卵细胞("只是化学和物理")开始,慢慢地,逐渐地,成为一个拥有复杂的元认知过程、希望和梦想的成年人。此外,尽管我们认为自己是一个统一的、单一的 "我",有别于白蚁蚁丘和其他蚁群的突发动态,但实际上,所有的智能都是集体智能:我们每个人都由大量细胞组成,共同产生一个连贯的认知存在,具有属于整体而非部分的目标、偏好和记忆。基础认知就是要了解 "心智 "是如何扩展的--大量有能力的亚单位如何协同工作,成为扩大其可能目标规模的智能体。最重要的是,将细胞水平的生理能力转化为大规模行为智能的非凡技巧并不局限于大脑的电动力。早在神经元和肌肉出现之前,进化论就已经利用生物电信号来解决创造和修复复杂身体的问题了。在本视角中,我将回顾发育形态发生智能与经典行为智能之间的深刻对称性。我描述了高度保守的机制,这些机制使细胞的集体智慧能够实现调节性胚胎发生、再生和癌症抑制。我勾勒了一个进化支点的故事,它将实现形态空间导航的算法和细胞机制重新用于三维世界的行为导航,而我们很容易将其视为智能。了解构成复杂身体和大脑的生物电动态,为了解地球系统发育历史内外各种智能体的自然进化和生物工程设计提供了重要途径。
{"title":"Bioelectric networks: the cognitive glue enabling evolutionary scaling from physiology to mind.","authors":"Michael Levin","doi":"10.1007/s10071-023-01780-3","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10071-023-01780-3","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Each of us made the remarkable journey from mere matter to mind: starting life as a quiescent oocyte (\"just chemistry and physics\"), and slowly, gradually, becoming an adult human with complex metacognitive processes, hopes, and dreams. In addition, even though we feel ourselves to be a unified, single Self, distinct from the emergent dynamics of termite mounds and other swarms, the reality is that all intelligence is collective intelligence: each of us consists of a huge number of cells working together to generate a coherent cognitive being with goals, preferences, and memories that belong to the whole and not to its parts. Basal cognition is the quest to understand how Mind scales-how large numbers of competent subunits can work together to become intelligences that expand the scale of their possible goals. Crucially, the remarkable trick of turning homeostatic, cell-level physiological competencies into large-scale behavioral intelligences is not limited to the electrical dynamics of the brain. Evolution was using bioelectric signaling long before neurons and muscles appeared, to solve the problem of creating and repairing complex bodies. In this Perspective, I review the deep symmetry between the intelligence of developmental morphogenesis and that of classical behavior. I describe the highly conserved mechanisms that enable the collective intelligence of cells to implement regulative embryogenesis, regeneration, and cancer suppression. I sketch the story of an evolutionary pivot that repurposed the algorithms and cellular machinery that enable navigation of morphospace into the behavioral navigation of the 3D world which we so readily recognize as intelligence. Understanding the bioelectric dynamics that underlie construction of complex bodies and brains provides an essential path to understanding the natural evolution, and bioengineered design, of diverse intelligences within and beyond the phylogenetic history of Earth.</p>","PeriodicalId":7879,"journal":{"name":"Animal Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.7,"publicationDate":"2023-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10770221/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9480677","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Chemical cognition: chemoconnectomics and convergent evolution of integrative systems in animals. 化学认知:动物整合系统的化学连接组学和趋同进化。
IF 2.7 2区 生物学 Q1 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2023-11-01 Epub Date: 2023-11-28 DOI: 10.1007/s10071-023-01833-7
Leonid L Moroz, Daria Y Romanova

Neurons underpin cognition in animals. However, the roots of animal cognition are elusive from both mechanistic and evolutionary standpoints. Two conceptual frameworks both highlight and promise to address these challenges. First, we discuss evidence that animal neural and other integrative systems evolved more than once (convergent evolution) within basal metazoan lineages, giving us unique experiments by Nature for future studies. The most remarkable examples are neural systems in ctenophores and neuroid-like systems in placozoans and sponges. Second, in addition to classical synaptic wiring, a chemical connectome mediated by hundreds of signal molecules operates in tandem with neurons and is the most information-rich source of emerging properties and adaptability. The major gap-dynamic, multifunctional chemical micro-environments in nervous systems-is not understood well. Thus, novel tools and information are needed to establish mechanistic links between orchestrated, yet cell-specific, volume transmission and behaviors. Uniting what we call chemoconnectomics and analyses of the cellular bases of behavior in basal metazoan lineages arguably would form the foundation for deciphering the origins and early evolution of elementary cognition and intelligence.

神经元是动物认知的基础。然而,从机械和进化的角度来看,动物认知的根源都是难以捉摸的。两个概念性框架都强调并承诺解决这些挑战。首先,我们讨论了动物神经系统和其他综合系统在基础后生动物谱系中进化不止一次(趋同进化)的证据,为我们未来的研究提供了独特的自然实验。最显著的例子是栉水母的神经系统,以及placozoa和海绵的类神经系统。其次,除了经典的突触连接外,由数百个信号分子介导的化学连接组与神经元协同工作,是新兴特性和适应性的最丰富的信息来源。主要的空白——神经系统中动态的、多功能的化学微环境——还没有被很好地理解。因此,需要新的工具和信息来建立协调的、细胞特异性的体积传输和行为之间的机制联系。将我们所说的化学连接组学和对基础后生动物谱系中行为的细胞基础的分析结合起来,可能会为破译基本认知和智力的起源和早期进化奠定基础。
{"title":"Chemical cognition: chemoconnectomics and convergent evolution of integrative systems in animals.","authors":"Leonid L Moroz, Daria Y Romanova","doi":"10.1007/s10071-023-01833-7","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10071-023-01833-7","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Neurons underpin cognition in animals. However, the roots of animal cognition are elusive from both mechanistic and evolutionary standpoints. Two conceptual frameworks both highlight and promise to address these challenges. First, we discuss evidence that animal neural and other integrative systems evolved more than once (convergent evolution) within basal metazoan lineages, giving us unique experiments by Nature for future studies. The most remarkable examples are neural systems in ctenophores and neuroid-like systems in placozoans and sponges. Second, in addition to classical synaptic wiring, a chemical connectome mediated by hundreds of signal molecules operates in tandem with neurons and is the most information-rich source of emerging properties and adaptability. The major gap-dynamic, multifunctional chemical micro-environments in nervous systems-is not understood well. Thus, novel tools and information are needed to establish mechanistic links between orchestrated, yet cell-specific, volume transmission and behaviors. Uniting what we call chemoconnectomics and analyses of the cellular bases of behavior in basal metazoan lineages arguably would form the foundation for deciphering the origins and early evolution of elementary cognition and intelligence.</p>","PeriodicalId":7879,"journal":{"name":"Animal Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.7,"publicationDate":"2023-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11106658/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138443576","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Hoarding titmice predominantly use Familiarity, and not Recollection, when remembering cache locations. 囤积山雀在记忆缓存位置时主要使用熟悉度,而不是重新收集。
IF 2.7 2区 生物学 Q1 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2023-11-01 Epub Date: 2023-10-21 DOI: 10.1007/s10071-023-01829-3
Tom V Smulders, Laura J Douglas, Daniel Reza, Lucinda H Male, Alexander Prysce, Amélie Alix, Alexander de Guzman Dodd, Jenny C A Read

Scatter-hoarding birds find their caches using spatial memory and have an enlarged hippocampus. Finding a cache site could be achieved using either Recollection (a discrete recalling of previously experienced information) or Familiarity (a feeling of "having encountered something before"). In humans, these two processes can be distinguished using receiver operating characteristic (ROC) curves. ROC curves for olfactory memory in rats have shown the hippocampus is involved in Recollection, but not Familiarity. We test the hypothesis that food-hoarding birds, having a larger hippocampus, primarily use Recollection to find their caches. We validate a novel method of constructing ROC curves in humans and apply this method to cache retrieval by coal tits (Periparus ater). Both humans and birds mainly use Familiarity in finding their caches, with lower contribution of Recollection. This contribution is not significantly different from chance in birds, but a small contribution cannot be ruled out. Memory performance decreases with increasing retention interval in birds. The ecology of food-hoarding Parids makes it plausible that they mainly use Familiarity in the memory for caches. The larger hippocampus could be related to associating cache contents and temporal context with cache locations, rather than Recollection of the spatial information itself.

零散的囤积鸟类利用空间记忆找到它们的藏匿处,并有一个扩大的海马体。可以使用“回忆”(对以前经历过的信息的离散回忆)或“熟悉”(“以前遇到过什么”的感觉)来找到缓存站点。在人类中,这两个过程可以通过受试者工作特性(ROC)曲线来区分。大鼠嗅觉记忆的ROC曲线显示,海马体参与回忆,但不参与熟悉。我们检验了这样一种假设,即具有较大海马体的囤积食物的鸟类主要使用回忆来寻找它们的储藏处。我们验证了一种在人类中构建ROC曲线的新方法,并将该方法应用于山雀(Periparus ater)的缓存检索。人类和鸟类在寻找藏匿处时都主要使用熟悉度,而重新收集的贡献较小。这种贡献与鸟类的偶然性没有显著差异,但不能排除一小部分贡献。鸟类的记忆性能随着停留时间的增加而下降。食物囤积区的生态环境使得他们主要使用记忆中的熟悉度作为储藏室是合理的。较大的海马体可能与缓存内容和时间上下文与缓存位置的关联有关,而不是空间信息本身的重新收集。
{"title":"Hoarding titmice predominantly use Familiarity, and not Recollection, when remembering cache locations.","authors":"Tom V Smulders, Laura J Douglas, Daniel Reza, Lucinda H Male, Alexander Prysce, Amélie Alix, Alexander de Guzman Dodd, Jenny C A Read","doi":"10.1007/s10071-023-01829-3","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10071-023-01829-3","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Scatter-hoarding birds find their caches using spatial memory and have an enlarged hippocampus. Finding a cache site could be achieved using either Recollection (a discrete recalling of previously experienced information) or Familiarity (a feeling of \"having encountered something before\"). In humans, these two processes can be distinguished using receiver operating characteristic (ROC) curves. ROC curves for olfactory memory in rats have shown the hippocampus is involved in Recollection, but not Familiarity. We test the hypothesis that food-hoarding birds, having a larger hippocampus, primarily use Recollection to find their caches. We validate a novel method of constructing ROC curves in humans and apply this method to cache retrieval by coal tits (Periparus ater). Both humans and birds mainly use Familiarity in finding their caches, with lower contribution of Recollection. This contribution is not significantly different from chance in birds, but a small contribution cannot be ruled out. Memory performance decreases with increasing retention interval in birds. The ecology of food-hoarding Parids makes it plausible that they mainly use Familiarity in the memory for caches. The larger hippocampus could be related to associating cache contents and temporal context with cache locations, rather than Recollection of the spatial information itself.</p>","PeriodicalId":7879,"journal":{"name":"Animal Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.7,"publicationDate":"2023-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10769918/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"49673449","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
First acoustic evidence of signature whistle production by spinner dolphins (Stenella longirostris). 飞旋海豚(Stenella longirostris)发出特征性哨声的首个声学证据。
IF 2.7 2区 生物学 Q1 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2023-11-01 Epub Date: 2023-09-07 DOI: 10.1007/s10071-023-01824-8
Raul Rio

A dolphin's signature whistle (SW) is a distinctive acoustic signal, issued in a bout pattern of unique frequency modulation contours; it allows individuals belonging to a given group to recognize each other and, consequently, to maintain contact and cohesion. The current study is the first scientific evidence that spinner dolphins (Stenella longirostris) produce SWs. Acoustic data were recorded at a shallow rest bay called "Biboca", in Fernando de Noronha Archipelago, Brazil. In total, 1902 whistles were analyzed; 40% (753/1,902) of them were classified as stereotyped whistles (STW). Based on the SIGID method, 63% (472/753) of all STWs were identified as SWs; subsequently, they were categorized into one of 18 SW types. SWs accounted for 25% (472/1,902) of the acoustic repertoire. External observers have shown near perfect agreement to classify whistles into the adopted SW categorization. Most acoustic and temporal variables measured for SWs showed mean values similar to those recorded in other studies with spinner dolphins, whose authors did not differentiate SWs from non-SWs. Principal component analysis has explained 78% of total SW variance, and it emphasized the relevance of shape/contour and frequency variables to SW variance. This scientific discovery helps improving bioacoustics knowledge about the investigated species. Future studies to be conducted in Fernando de Noronha Archipelago should focus on continuous investigations about SW development and use by S. longirostris, expanding individuals' identifications (Photo ID and SW Noronha Catalog), assessing long-term whistle stability and emission rates, and making mother-offspring comparisons with sex-based differences.

海豚的标志性哨音(SW)是一种独特的声学信号,以独特的频率调制轮廓的阵列模式发出;它使属于特定群体的个体能够相互识别,从而保持联系和凝聚力。目前的研究首次从科学角度证明了飞旋海豚(Stenella longirostris)会发出 SWs。声学数据是在巴西费尔南多-德诺罗尼亚群岛一个名为 "比博卡 "的浅水休憩湾记录的。共分析了 1902 个哨音,其中 40% (753/1,902)被归类为定型哨音(STW)。根据 SIGID 方法,所有 STW 中的 63%(472/753)被确定为 SW;随后,它们被归入 18 种 SW 类型之一。SW 占声谱的 25%(472/1,902)。外部观察者在将口哨归入所采用的 SW 分类中显示出近乎完美的一致性。对 SW 类口哨测量的大多数声学和时间变量的平均值与其他针对飞旋海豚的研究中记录的平均值相似,而这些研究的作者并未将 SW 类口哨与非 SW 类口哨区分开来。主成分分析解释了 78% 的 SW 总变异,并强调了形状/轮廓和频率变量与 SW 变异的相关性。这一科学发现有助于提高有关所调查物种的生物声学知识。未来在费尔南多-德诺罗尼亚群岛开展的研究应侧重于继续调查长尾鳕(S. longirostris)的SW发展和使用情况,扩大个体识别范围(照片ID和诺罗尼亚西南群岛目录),评估长期哨声稳定性和发射率,并根据性别差异进行母子比较。
{"title":"First acoustic evidence of signature whistle production by spinner dolphins (Stenella longirostris).","authors":"Raul Rio","doi":"10.1007/s10071-023-01824-8","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10071-023-01824-8","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>A dolphin's signature whistle (SW) is a distinctive acoustic signal, issued in a bout pattern of unique frequency modulation contours; it allows individuals belonging to a given group to recognize each other and, consequently, to maintain contact and cohesion. The current study is the first scientific evidence that spinner dolphins (Stenella longirostris) produce SWs. Acoustic data were recorded at a shallow rest bay called \"Biboca\", in Fernando de Noronha Archipelago, Brazil. In total, 1902 whistles were analyzed; 40% (753/1,902) of them were classified as stereotyped whistles (STW). Based on the SIGID method, 63% (472/753) of all STWs were identified as SWs; subsequently, they were categorized into one of 18 SW types. SWs accounted for 25% (472/1,902) of the acoustic repertoire. External observers have shown near perfect agreement to classify whistles into the adopted SW categorization. Most acoustic and temporal variables measured for SWs showed mean values similar to those recorded in other studies with spinner dolphins, whose authors did not differentiate SWs from non-SWs. Principal component analysis has explained 78% of total SW variance, and it emphasized the relevance of shape/contour and frequency variables to SW variance. This scientific discovery helps improving bioacoustics knowledge about the investigated species. Future studies to be conducted in Fernando de Noronha Archipelago should focus on continuous investigations about SW development and use by S. longirostris, expanding individuals' identifications (Photo ID and SW Noronha Catalog), assessing long-term whistle stability and emission rates, and making mother-offspring comparisons with sex-based differences.</p>","PeriodicalId":7879,"journal":{"name":"Animal Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.7,"publicationDate":"2023-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10162327","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Thoughts from the forest floor: a review of cognition in the slime mould Physarum polycephalum. 来自森林地面的思考:粘菌 Physarum polycephalum 的认知综述。
IF 2.7 2区 生物学 Q1 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2023-11-01 Epub Date: 2023-05-11 DOI: 10.1007/s10071-023-01782-1
Chris R Reid

Sensing, communication, navigation, decision-making, memory and learning are key components in a standard cognitive tool-kit that enhance an animal's ability to successfully survive and reproduce. However, these tools are not only useful for, or accessible to, animals-they evolved long ago in simpler organisms using mechanisms which may be either unique or widely conserved across diverse taxa. In this article, I review the recent research that demonstrates these key cognitive abilities in the plasmodial slime mould Physarum polycephalum, which has emerged as a model for non-animal cognition. I discuss the benefits and limitations of comparisons drawn between neural and non-neural systems, and the implications of common mechanisms across wide taxonomic divisions. I conclude by discussing future avenues of research that will draw the most benefit from a closer integration of Physarum and animal cognition research.

感知、交流、导航、决策、记忆和学习是标准认知工具包中的关键组成部分,可提高动物成功生存和繁殖的能力。然而,这些工具不仅对动物有用,动物也可以使用,它们很早以前就在较简单的生物体内进化了,使用的机制可能是独特的,也可能是在不同类群中广泛保守的。在这篇文章中,我回顾了最近在质粘菌 Physarum polycephalum 中证明这些关键认知能力的研究。我将讨论在神经系统和非神经系统之间进行比较的益处和局限性,以及不同分类学之间共同机制的意义。最后,我将讨论未来的研究方向,这些方向将从更紧密地整合 Physarum 和动物认知研究中获益匪浅。
{"title":"Thoughts from the forest floor: a review of cognition in the slime mould Physarum polycephalum.","authors":"Chris R Reid","doi":"10.1007/s10071-023-01782-1","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10071-023-01782-1","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Sensing, communication, navigation, decision-making, memory and learning are key components in a standard cognitive tool-kit that enhance an animal's ability to successfully survive and reproduce. However, these tools are not only useful for, or accessible to, animals-they evolved long ago in simpler organisms using mechanisms which may be either unique or widely conserved across diverse taxa. In this article, I review the recent research that demonstrates these key cognitive abilities in the plasmodial slime mould Physarum polycephalum, which has emerged as a model for non-animal cognition. I discuss the benefits and limitations of comparisons drawn between neural and non-neural systems, and the implications of common mechanisms across wide taxonomic divisions. I conclude by discussing future avenues of research that will draw the most benefit from a closer integration of Physarum and animal cognition research.</p>","PeriodicalId":7879,"journal":{"name":"Animal Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.7,"publicationDate":"2023-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10770251/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9498810","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
What is it like to be a choanoflagellate? Sensation, processing and behavior in the closest unicellular relatives of animals. 藻鞭毛虫是什么样的?动物的单细胞近亲的感觉、处理和行为。
IF 2.7 2区 生物学 Q1 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2023-11-01 Epub Date: 2023-04-17 DOI: 10.1007/s10071-023-01776-z
Núria Ros-Rocher, Thibaut Brunet

All animals evolved from a single lineage of unicellular precursors more than 600 million years ago. Thus, the biological and genetic foundations for animal sensation, cognition and behavior must necessarily have arisen by modifications of pre-existing features in their unicellular ancestors. Given that the single-celled ancestors of the animal kingdom are extinct, the only way to reconstruct how these features evolved is by comparing the biology and genomic content of extant animals to their closest living relatives. Here, we reconstruct the Umwelt (the subjective, perceptive world) inhabited by choanoflagellates, a group of unicellular (or facultatively multicellular) aquatic microeukaryotes that are the closest living relatives of animals. Although behavioral research on choanoflagellates remains patchy, existing evidence shows that they are capable of chemosensation, photosensation and mechanosensation. These processes often involve specialized sensorimotor cellular appendages (cilia, microvilli, and/or filopodia) that resemble those that underlie perception in most animal sensory cells. Furthermore, comparative genomics predicts an extensive "sensory molecular toolkit" in choanoflagellates, which both provides a potential basis for known behaviors and suggests the existence of a largely undescribed behavioral complexity that presents exciting avenues for future research. Finally, we discuss how facultative multicellularity in choanoflagellates might help us understand how evolution displaced the locus of decision-making from a single cell to a collective, and how a new space of behavioral complexity might have become accessible in the process.

所有动物都是在 6 亿多年前由单细胞前身单系进化而来的。因此,动物感觉、认知和行为的生物学和遗传学基础必然是通过对其单细胞祖先原有特征的改造而产生的。鉴于动物界的单细胞祖先已经灭绝,要重建这些特征是如何进化的,唯一的方法就是将现存动物的生物学和基因组内容与它们的近亲进行比较。在这里,我们重建了绒毛鞭毛虫居住的Umwelt(主观、感知的世界),绒毛鞭毛虫是一类单细胞(或表面多细胞)水生微核生物,是动物的近亲。尽管有关藻鞭毛虫的行为学研究仍很零散,但现有证据表明,它们能够进行化学感应、光感应和机械感应。这些过程通常涉及专门的感觉运动细胞附属物(纤毛、微绒毛和/或丝状体),与大多数动物感觉细胞的感知原理相似。此外,比较基因组学还预测鹅鞭毛虫具有广泛的 "感官分子工具包",这既为已知行为提供了潜在的基础,也表明存在着基本未被描述的行为复杂性,为未来的研究提供了令人兴奋的途径。最后,我们讨论了绒毛鞭毛虫的变性多细胞性如何帮助我们理解进化如何将决策中心从单细胞转移到集体,以及如何在此过程中获得行为复杂性的新空间。
{"title":"What is it like to be a choanoflagellate? Sensation, processing and behavior in the closest unicellular relatives of animals.","authors":"Núria Ros-Rocher, Thibaut Brunet","doi":"10.1007/s10071-023-01776-z","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10071-023-01776-z","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>All animals evolved from a single lineage of unicellular precursors more than 600 million years ago. Thus, the biological and genetic foundations for animal sensation, cognition and behavior must necessarily have arisen by modifications of pre-existing features in their unicellular ancestors. Given that the single-celled ancestors of the animal kingdom are extinct, the only way to reconstruct how these features evolved is by comparing the biology and genomic content of extant animals to their closest living relatives. Here, we reconstruct the Umwelt (the subjective, perceptive world) inhabited by choanoflagellates, a group of unicellular (or facultatively multicellular) aquatic microeukaryotes that are the closest living relatives of animals. Although behavioral research on choanoflagellates remains patchy, existing evidence shows that they are capable of chemosensation, photosensation and mechanosensation. These processes often involve specialized sensorimotor cellular appendages (cilia, microvilli, and/or filopodia) that resemble those that underlie perception in most animal sensory cells. Furthermore, comparative genomics predicts an extensive \"sensory molecular toolkit\" in choanoflagellates, which both provides a potential basis for known behaviors and suggests the existence of a largely undescribed behavioral complexity that presents exciting avenues for future research. Finally, we discuss how facultative multicellularity in choanoflagellates might help us understand how evolution displaced the locus of decision-making from a single cell to a collective, and how a new space of behavioral complexity might have become accessible in the process.</p>","PeriodicalId":7879,"journal":{"name":"Animal Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.7,"publicationDate":"2023-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10770216/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9309969","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Orientation by environmental geometry and feature cues in the green and black poison frog (Dendrobates auratus). 绿色和黑色毒蛙(Dendrobates auratus)通过环境几何和特征线索确定方向。
IF 2.7 2区 生物学 Q1 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2023-11-01 Epub Date: 2023-09-12 DOI: 10.1007/s10071-023-01820-y
Cody A Sorrell, Sabrina S Burmeister

The ability to use environmental geometry when orienting in space reflects an animal's ability to use a global, allocentric framework. Therefore, understanding when and how animal's use geometry relative to other types of cues in the environment has interested comparative cognition researchers for decades. Yet, only two amphibians have been tested to date. We trained the poison frog Dendrobates auratus to find goal shelters in a rectangular arena, in the presence and absence of a feature cue, and assessed the relative influence of the two types of cues using probe trials. We chose D. auratus because the species has complex interactions with their physical and social environments, including parental care that requires navigating to and from distant locations. We found that, like many vertebrates, D. auratus are capable of using geometric information to relocate goals. In addition, the frogs preferentially used the more reliable feature cue when the location of the feature conflicted with the geometry of the arena. The frogs were equally successful at using the feature cue when it was proximal or distal to the goal shelter, consistent with prior studies that found that D. auratus can use distal cues in a flexible manner. Our results provide further evidence that amphibians can use environmental geometry during orientation. Future studies that examine when and how amphibians use geometry relative to other types of cues will contribute to a more complete picture of spatial cognition in this important, yet understudied, group.

在确定空间方向时使用环境几何图形的能力反映了动物使用全球分配中心框架的能力。因此,数十年来,比较认知研究人员一直对了解动物在何时以及如何使用几何图形与环境中的其他类型线索进行比较感兴趣。然而,迄今为止只有两种两栖动物接受过测试。我们训练毒蛙在有和没有特征线索的情况下在矩形场地中寻找目标避难所,并通过探究试验评估两种线索的相对影响。我们之所以选择无尾椎蛙,是因为该物种与它们的物理和社会环境有着复杂的相互作用,包括需要往返于遥远地点的父母照顾。我们发现,和许多脊椎动物一样,金蛙能够利用几何信息来重新定位目标。此外,当特征的位置与竞技场的几何形状发生冲突时,蛙类会优先使用更可靠的特征线索。当特征线索位于目标掩蔽物的近端或远端时,蛙类使用特征线索的成功率相同,这与之前的研究结果一致,这些研究发现蛙类可以灵活地使用远端线索。我们的研究结果进一步证明,两栖动物在定向过程中可以利用环境的几何特征。未来的研究将考察两栖动物在何时以及如何使用几何图形而不是其他类型的线索,这将有助于更全面地了解这一重要但研究不足的类群的空间认知。
{"title":"Orientation by environmental geometry and feature cues in the green and black poison frog (Dendrobates auratus).","authors":"Cody A Sorrell, Sabrina S Burmeister","doi":"10.1007/s10071-023-01820-y","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10071-023-01820-y","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The ability to use environmental geometry when orienting in space reflects an animal's ability to use a global, allocentric framework. Therefore, understanding when and how animal's use geometry relative to other types of cues in the environment has interested comparative cognition researchers for decades. Yet, only two amphibians have been tested to date. We trained the poison frog Dendrobates auratus to find goal shelters in a rectangular arena, in the presence and absence of a feature cue, and assessed the relative influence of the two types of cues using probe trials. We chose D. auratus because the species has complex interactions with their physical and social environments, including parental care that requires navigating to and from distant locations. We found that, like many vertebrates, D. auratus are capable of using geometric information to relocate goals. In addition, the frogs preferentially used the more reliable feature cue when the location of the feature conflicted with the geometry of the arena. The frogs were equally successful at using the feature cue when it was proximal or distal to the goal shelter, consistent with prior studies that found that D. auratus can use distal cues in a flexible manner. Our results provide further evidence that amphibians can use environmental geometry during orientation. Future studies that examine when and how amphibians use geometry relative to other types of cues will contribute to a more complete picture of spatial cognition in this important, yet understudied, group.</p>","PeriodicalId":7879,"journal":{"name":"Animal Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.7,"publicationDate":"2023-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10211703","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Animal Cognition
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1