首页 > 最新文献

Animal Cognition最新文献

英文 中文
Serial reversal learning in nectar-feeding bats 食蜜蝙蝠的连续反向学习
IF 1.9 2区 生物学 Q3 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-03-07 DOI: 10.1007/s10071-024-01836-y
Shambhavi Chidambaram, Sabine Wintergerst, Alex Kacelnik, Vladislav Nachev, York Winter

We explored the behavioral flexibility of Commissaris’s long-tongued bats through a spatial serial reversal foraging task. Bats kept in captivity for short periods were trained to obtain nectar rewards from two artificial flowers. At any given time, only one of the flowers provided rewards and these reward contingencies reversed in successive blocks of 50 flower visits. All bats detected and responded to reversals by making most of their visits to the currently active flower. As the bats experienced repeated reversals, their preference re-adjusted faster. Although the flower state reversals were theoretically predictable, we did not detect anticipatory behavior, that is, frequency of visits to the alternative flower did not increase within each block as the programmed reversal approached. The net balance of these changes was a progressive improvement in performance in terms of the total proportion of visits allocated to the active flower. The results are compatible with, but do not depend on, the bats displaying an ability to ‘learn to learn’ and show that the dynamics of allocation of effort between food sources can change flexibly according to circumstances.

我们通过空间序列反转觅食任务探索了长舌蝠的行为灵活性。我们训练短期圈养的蝙蝠从两朵人造花中获取花蜜奖励。在任何给定时间内,只有其中一朵花能提供奖励,这些奖励或然性在连续的 50 次花访问中发生逆转。所有蝙蝠都能检测到逆转,并对逆转做出反应,它们的大部分访问都是在当前活跃的花朵上进行的。随着蝙蝠多次经历逆转,它们的偏好重新调整的速度也越来越快。尽管花的状态逆转在理论上是可以预测的,但我们并没有检测到蝙蝠的预期行为,也就是说,随着程序逆转的临近,蝙蝠在每个区块内访问替代花的频率并没有增加。从分配给活动花朵的总访问比例来看,这些变化的净平衡是性能的逐步提高。这些结果与蝙蝠 "学会学习 "的能力相吻合,但并不依赖于这种能力。
{"title":"Serial reversal learning in nectar-feeding bats","authors":"Shambhavi Chidambaram,&nbsp;Sabine Wintergerst,&nbsp;Alex Kacelnik,&nbsp;Vladislav Nachev,&nbsp;York Winter","doi":"10.1007/s10071-024-01836-y","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10071-024-01836-y","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>We explored the behavioral flexibility of Commissaris’s long-tongued bats through a spatial serial reversal foraging task. Bats kept in captivity for short periods were trained to obtain nectar rewards from two artificial flowers. At any given time, only one of the flowers provided rewards and these reward contingencies reversed in successive blocks of 50 flower visits. All bats detected and responded to reversals by making most of their visits to the currently active flower. As the bats experienced repeated reversals, their preference re-adjusted faster. Although the flower state reversals were theoretically predictable, we did not detect anticipatory behavior, that is, frequency of visits to the alternative flower did not increase within each block as the programmed reversal approached. The net balance of these changes was a progressive improvement in performance in terms of the total proportion of visits allocated to the active flower. The results are compatible with, but do not depend on, the bats displaying an ability to ‘learn to learn’ and show that the dynamics of allocation of effort between food sources can change flexibly according to circumstances.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":7879,"journal":{"name":"Animal Cognition","volume":"27 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2024-03-07","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10920430/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140048618","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Maze runners: monkeys show restricted Arabic numeral summation during computerized two-arm maze performance 迷宫选手:猴子在计算机化双臂迷宫中表现出受限的阿拉伯数字求和能力。
IF 1.9 2区 生物学 Q3 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-03-06 DOI: 10.1007/s10071-024-01853-x
Elizabeth L. Haseltine, Michael J. Beran

Mazes have been used in many forms to provide compelling results showcasing nonhuman animals’ capacities for spatial navigation, planning, and numerical competence. The current study presented computerized two-arm mazes to four rhesus macaques. Using these mazes, we assessed whether the monkeys could maximize rewards by overcoming mild delays in gratification and sum the values of Arabic numerals. Across four test phases, monkeys used a joystick controller to choose one of two maze arms on the screen. Each maze arm contained zero, one or two Arabic numerals, and any numerals in the chosen maze arm provided the monkeys with rewards equivalent to the value of those numerals. When deciding which arm to enter, monkeys had to consider distance to numerals and numeral value. In some tests, gaining the maximum reward required summing the value of two numerals within a given arm. All four monkeys successfully maximized reward when comparing single numerals and when comparing arms that each contained two numerals. However, some biases occurred that were suboptimal: the largest single numeral and the delay of reward (by placing numerals farther into an arm from the start location) sometimes interfered with the monkeys’ abilities to optimize. These results indicate that monkeys experience difficulties with inhibition toward single, high valence stimuli in tasks where those stimuli must be considered in relation to overall value when represented by symbolic stimuli such as numerals.

迷宫以多种形式被用于展示非人类动物的空间导航、规划和计算能力,其结果令人信服。本研究向四只猕猴展示了计算机化的双臂迷宫。利用这些迷宫,我们评估了猕猴能否通过克服轻微的满足延迟来获得最大奖励,以及能否求出阿拉伯数字的数值总和。在四个测试阶段中,猴子使用操纵杆控制器从屏幕上的两个迷宫臂中选择一个。每个迷宫臂包含零、一个或两个阿拉伯数字,所选迷宫臂中的任何数字都会给猴子带来与这些数字等值的奖励。在决定进入哪个迷宫臂时,猴子必须考虑与数字的距离和数字的价值。在某些测试中,要获得最大奖励,就必须将特定臂内两个数字的价值相加。四只猴子在比较单个数字和比较各包含两个数字的手臂时,都成功地获得了最大奖励。不过,也出现了一些次优偏差:最大的单个数字和奖励延迟(将数字放在离起始位置更远的臂内)有时会干扰猴子的优化能力。这些结果表明,在一些任务中,猴子很难抑制单一的高价值刺激,因为这些刺激必须与数字等符号刺激的整体价值联系起来考虑。
{"title":"Maze runners: monkeys show restricted Arabic numeral summation during computerized two-arm maze performance","authors":"Elizabeth L. Haseltine,&nbsp;Michael J. Beran","doi":"10.1007/s10071-024-01853-x","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10071-024-01853-x","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Mazes have been used in many forms to provide compelling results showcasing nonhuman animals’ capacities for spatial navigation, planning, and numerical competence. The current study presented computerized two-arm mazes to four rhesus macaques. Using these mazes, we assessed whether the monkeys could maximize rewards by overcoming mild delays in gratification and sum the values of Arabic numerals. Across four test phases, monkeys used a joystick controller to choose one of two maze arms on the screen. Each maze arm contained zero, one or two Arabic numerals, and any numerals in the chosen maze arm provided the monkeys with rewards equivalent to the value of those numerals. When deciding which arm to enter, monkeys had to consider distance to numerals and numeral value. In some tests, gaining the maximum reward required summing the value of two numerals within a given arm. All four monkeys successfully maximized reward when comparing single numerals and when comparing arms that each contained two numerals. However, some biases occurred that were suboptimal: the largest single numeral and the delay of reward (by placing numerals farther into an arm from the start location) sometimes interfered with the monkeys’ abilities to optimize. These results indicate that monkeys experience difficulties with inhibition toward single, high valence stimuli in tasks where those stimuli must be considered in relation to overall value when represented by symbolic stimuli such as numerals.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":7879,"journal":{"name":"Animal Cognition","volume":"27 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2024-03-06","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10914902/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140038601","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Metacognition in wild Japanese macaques: cost and stakes influencing information-seeking behavior 野生日本猕猴的元认知:影响信息搜寻行为的成本和利害关系。
IF 1.9 2区 生物学 Q3 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-03-05 DOI: 10.1007/s10071-024-01851-z
Lorraine Subias, Noriko Katsu, Kazunori Yamada

Metacognition allows us to evaluate memories and knowledge, thus enabling us to distinguish between what we know and what we do not. Studies have shown that species other than humans may possess similar abilities. However, the number of species tested was limited. Testing ten free-ranging Japanese macaques (Macaca fuscata) on a task in which they had to find food hidden inside one of the four opaque tubes, we investigated whether these subjects would seek information when needed. The monkeys could look inside the tubes before selecting one. We varied three parameters: the baiting process, the cost that monkeys had to pay to look inside the tubes, and the reward at stake. We assessed whether and how these parameters would affect the monkeys’ tendency to look inside the tube before selecting one. When they were not shown which tube contained the reward, nine monkeys looked significantly more frequently in at least one condition. Half of them tended to reduce their looks when the cost was high, but only when they already knew the location of the reward. When a high-quality reward was at stake, four monkeys tended to look more inside the tubes, even though they already knew the reward’s location. Our results are consistent with those of rhesus macaques, suggesting that metacognitive-like abilities may be shared by Cercopithecidae, and that, at least some monkeys may be aware of their lack of knowledge.

元认知使我们能够评估记忆和知识,从而区分我们知道什么和不知道什么。研究表明,人类以外的物种也可能拥有类似的能力。然而,测试的物种数量有限。我们对十只自由放养的日本猕猴(Macaca fuscata)进行了一项任务测试,要求它们找到藏在四个不透明管子中的一个管子里的食物。猴子可以先观察管子内部,然后再选择其中一个。我们改变了三个参数:诱饵过程、猴子查看管子内部所需的成本以及相关奖励。我们评估了这些参数是否以及如何影响猴子在选择前观察管子内部的倾向。在不向猴子展示哪个管子里有奖励的情况下,九只猴子在至少一种情况下看管的频率明显增加。当成本较高时,其中一半的猴子倾向于减少看的次数,但只有当它们已经知道奖励的位置时才会这样做。当有高额奖励时,四只猴子倾向于更多地观察管子内部,即使它们已经知道奖励的位置。我们的研究结果与猕猴的研究结果一致,表明ercopithecidae可能具有类似元认知的能力,而且至少有些猴子可能意识到自己缺乏知识。
{"title":"Metacognition in wild Japanese macaques: cost and stakes influencing information-seeking behavior","authors":"Lorraine Subias,&nbsp;Noriko Katsu,&nbsp;Kazunori Yamada","doi":"10.1007/s10071-024-01851-z","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10071-024-01851-z","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Metacognition allows us to evaluate memories and knowledge, thus enabling us to distinguish between what we know and what we do not. Studies have shown that species other than humans may possess similar abilities. However, the number of species tested was limited. Testing ten free-ranging Japanese macaques (<i>Macaca fuscata</i>) on a task in which they had to find food hidden inside one of the four opaque tubes, we investigated whether these subjects would seek information when needed. The monkeys could look inside the tubes before selecting one. We varied three parameters: the baiting process, the cost that monkeys had to pay to look inside the tubes, and the reward at stake. We assessed whether and how these parameters would affect the monkeys’ tendency to look inside the tube before selecting one. When they were not shown which tube contained the reward, nine monkeys looked significantly more frequently in at least one condition. Half of them tended to reduce their looks when the cost was high, but only when they already knew the location of the reward. When a high-quality reward was at stake, four monkeys tended to look more inside the tubes, even though they already knew the reward’s location. Our results are consistent with those of rhesus macaques, suggesting that metacognitive-like abilities may be shared by Cercopithecidae, and that, at least some monkeys may be aware of their lack of knowledge.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":7879,"journal":{"name":"Animal Cognition","volume":"27 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2024-03-05","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10914859/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140027191","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Habituation to a predatory stimulus in a harvester (Arachnida, Opiliones) 收割机(蛛形纲,Opiliones)对捕食性刺激的习性。
IF 1.9 2区 生物学 Q3 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-03-05 DOI: 10.1007/s10071-024-01857-7
Guilherme Ferreira Pagoti, Jerry A. Hogan, Rodrigo Hirata Willemart

Several studies have investigated habituation in a defensive context, but few have addressed responses to dangerous stimuli. In such cases, animals should not habituate since this could cost their lives. Here we have stimulated individuals of the harvester Mischonyx squalidus with a predatory stimulus (squeezing with tweezers) in repeated trials within and between days, and measured the occurrence and magnitude of nipping, a defensive behavior. Contrary to our expectations, they did habituate to this stimulus. The probability and magnitude of response declined over trials during each of three days of testing in a typical habituation pattern. During the trials we also observed other defensive behaviors. We discuss our results mainly considering alternative defensive responses. Our data show that we lack information on (1) the role played by the ambiguity of stimuli, (2) the role played by subsequent stimuli and (3) the importance of the array of defensive behaviors of a species in understanding habituation. Although ubiquitous across animals and therefore expected, habituation is described for the first time in the order Opiliones.

有几项研究调查了防卫情况下的习惯化,但很少有研究涉及对危险刺激的反应。在这种情况下,动物不应该习惯化,因为这可能会导致它们丧命。在这里,我们用一种捕食性刺激(用镊子挤压)在几天内和几天之间重复试验,刺激鹞鲼的个体,并测量它们咬人(一种防御行为)的发生率和程度。与我们的预期相反,它们确实习惯了这种刺激。在三天的试验中,每次试验的反应概率和幅度都在下降,这是典型的习惯化模式。在试验过程中,我们还观察到了其他防御行为。我们讨论的结果主要考虑了其他防御反应。我们的数据表明,我们在以下方面缺乏信息:(1) 刺激的模糊性所起的作用;(2) 后续刺激所起的作用;(3) 一个物种的一系列防御行为对理解习惯化的重要性。尽管习性在动物中无处不在,因此也在意料之中,但这是第一次在鸦雀目动物中描述习性。
{"title":"Habituation to a predatory stimulus in a harvester (Arachnida, Opiliones)","authors":"Guilherme Ferreira Pagoti,&nbsp;Jerry A. Hogan,&nbsp;Rodrigo Hirata Willemart","doi":"10.1007/s10071-024-01857-7","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10071-024-01857-7","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Several studies have investigated habituation in a defensive context, but few have addressed responses to dangerous stimuli. In such cases, animals should not habituate since this could cost their lives. Here we have stimulated individuals of the harvester <i>Mischonyx squalidus</i> with a predatory stimulus (squeezing with tweezers) in repeated trials within and between days, and measured the occurrence and magnitude of nipping, a defensive behavior. Contrary to our expectations, they did habituate to this stimulus. The probability and magnitude of response declined over trials during each of three days of testing in a typical habituation pattern. During the trials we also observed other defensive behaviors. We discuss our results mainly considering alternative defensive responses. Our data show that we lack information on (1) the role played by the ambiguity of stimuli, (2) the role played by subsequent stimuli and (3) the importance of the array of defensive behaviors of a species in understanding habituation. Although ubiquitous across animals and therefore expected, habituation is described for the first time in the order Opiliones.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":7879,"journal":{"name":"Animal Cognition","volume":"27 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2024-03-05","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10914851/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140027190","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Do domestic budgerigars perceive predation risk? 家养虎皮鹦鹉能感知捕食风险吗?
IF 1.9 2区 生物学 Q3 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-03-02 DOI: 10.1007/s10071-024-01847-9
Chang Wang, Xueqi Zhao, Baodan Tao, Jiaqi Peng, Haitao Wang, Jiangping Yu, Longru Jin

Predation risk may affect the foraging behavior of birds. However, there has been little research on the ability of domestic birds to perceive predation risk and thus adjust their feeding behavior. In this study, we tested whether domestic budgerigars (Melopsittacus undulatus) perceived predation risk after the presentation of specimens and sounds of sparrowhawks (Accipiter nisus), domestic cats (Felis catus), and humans, and whether this in turn influenced their feeding behavior. When exposed to visual or acoustic stimuli, budgerigars showed significantly longer latency to feed under sparrowhawk, domestic cat, and human treatments than with controls. Budgerigars responded more strongly to acoustic stimuli than visual stimuli, and they showed the longest latency to feed and the least number of feeding times in response to sparrowhawk calls. Moreover, budgerigars showed shorter latency to feed and greater numbers of feeding times in response to human voices than to sparrowhawk or domestic cat calls. Our results suggest that domestic budgerigars may identify predation risk through visual or acoustic signals and adjust their feeding behavior accordingly.

捕食风险可能会影响鸟类的觅食行为。然而,有关家养鸟类感知捕食风险从而调整觅食行为的能力的研究却很少。在这项研究中,我们测试了在呈现雀鹰(Accipiter nisus)、家猫(Felis catus)和人类的标本和声音后,家养虎皮鹦鹉(Melopsittacus undulatus)是否感知到捕食风险,以及这是否会反过来影响它们的觅食行为。当受到视觉或声音刺激时,虎皮鹦鹉在麻雀鹰、家猫和人类处理下的进食潜伏期明显长于对照组。虎皮鹦鹉对声音刺激的反应比对视觉刺激的反应更强烈,它们对雀鹰的叫声表现出最长的进食潜伏期和最少的进食次数。此外,虎皮鹦鹉对人声的取食潜伏期和取食次数都比对雀鹰或家猫叫声的取食潜伏期和取食次数要短。我们的研究结果表明,虎皮鹦鹉可能会通过视觉或声音信号来识别捕食风险,并相应地调整其捕食行为。
{"title":"Do domestic budgerigars perceive predation risk?","authors":"Chang Wang, Xueqi Zhao, Baodan Tao, Jiaqi Peng, Haitao Wang, Jiangping Yu, Longru Jin","doi":"10.1007/s10071-024-01847-9","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10071-024-01847-9","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Predation risk may affect the foraging behavior of birds. However, there has been little research on the ability of domestic birds to perceive predation risk and thus adjust their feeding behavior. In this study, we tested whether domestic budgerigars (Melopsittacus undulatus) perceived predation risk after the presentation of specimens and sounds of sparrowhawks (Accipiter nisus), domestic cats (Felis catus), and humans, and whether this in turn influenced their feeding behavior. When exposed to visual or acoustic stimuli, budgerigars showed significantly longer latency to feed under sparrowhawk, domestic cat, and human treatments than with controls. Budgerigars responded more strongly to acoustic stimuli than visual stimuli, and they showed the longest latency to feed and the least number of feeding times in response to sparrowhawk calls. Moreover, budgerigars showed shorter latency to feed and greater numbers of feeding times in response to human voices than to sparrowhawk or domestic cat calls. Our results suggest that domestic budgerigars may identify predation risk through visual or acoustic signals and adjust their feeding behavior accordingly.</p>","PeriodicalId":7879,"journal":{"name":"Animal Cognition","volume":"27 1","pages":"8"},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2024-03-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10907484/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140012048","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Personality and cognition: shoal size discrimination performance is related to boldness and sociability among ten freshwater fish species. 性格与认知:十种淡水鱼类的鱼群大小辨别能力与胆量和交际能力有关。
IF 1.9 2区 生物学 Q3 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-03-02 DOI: 10.1007/s10071-024-01837-x
Shi-Jian Fu, Na Zhang, Jie Fan

Several studies have reported that animals' personalities are often correlated with individual differences in cognition. Here, we tested whether personality is related to cognition across species, focusing on 10 freshwater fishes and a task relevant for fitness, the ability to discriminate shoal size. Bolder species exhibited more 'shuttle' behavior for information sampling during shoal selection and showed high performance (HP) in the numerical discrimination than shyer species, i.e., low performance (LP) species. Species at both the high and low ends of sociability showed LP, possibly due to loosened selection pressure because of either no need to perform shoal size discrimination tasks frequently in nature for very high sociability species or decreased willingness and motivation to join and stay within shoals for very low sociability species. Notably, the numerical discrimination was sensitive to the numerical contrast ratio in LP species but not in HP species, suggesting that the numerical system used for size discrimination also varied between species. Overall, we demonstrated the interspecies relationship between personality and shoal size discrimination across fish species, suggesting an evolutionary link between numerical abilities and behavior.

多项研究表明,动物的个性往往与认知的个体差异相关。在这里,我们以10种淡水鱼和一项与体能相关的任务--辨别鱼群大小的能力--为研究对象,检验了性格是否与不同物种的认知相关。在选择鱼群的过程中,胆大的鱼类表现出更多的信息采样 "穿梭 "行为,与胆怯的鱼类(即低性能鱼类)相比,它们在数字辨别方面表现出高性能(HP)。交际能力高低两端的物种都表现出低能,这可能是由于选择压力减弱,因为交际能力极高的物种在自然界中不需要经常执行鱼群大小辨别任务,或者交际能力极低的物种加入和留在鱼群中的意愿和动机降低。值得注意的是,在低社会性物种中,数字辨别对数字对比度很敏感,而在高社会性物种中则不然,这表明不同物种之间用于大小辨别的数字系统也是不同的。总之,我们证明了鱼类物种间性格与鱼群大小分辨之间的关系,表明数字能力与行为之间存在进化联系。
{"title":"Personality and cognition: shoal size discrimination performance is related to boldness and sociability among ten freshwater fish species.","authors":"Shi-Jian Fu, Na Zhang, Jie Fan","doi":"10.1007/s10071-024-01837-x","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10071-024-01837-x","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Several studies have reported that animals' personalities are often correlated with individual differences in cognition. Here, we tested whether personality is related to cognition across species, focusing on 10 freshwater fishes and a task relevant for fitness, the ability to discriminate shoal size. Bolder species exhibited more 'shuttle' behavior for information sampling during shoal selection and showed high performance (HP) in the numerical discrimination than shyer species, i.e., low performance (LP) species. Species at both the high and low ends of sociability showed LP, possibly due to loosened selection pressure because of either no need to perform shoal size discrimination tasks frequently in nature for very high sociability species or decreased willingness and motivation to join and stay within shoals for very low sociability species. Notably, the numerical discrimination was sensitive to the numerical contrast ratio in LP species but not in HP species, suggesting that the numerical system used for size discrimination also varied between species. Overall, we demonstrated the interspecies relationship between personality and shoal size discrimination across fish species, suggesting an evolutionary link between numerical abilities and behavior.</p>","PeriodicalId":7879,"journal":{"name":"Animal Cognition","volume":"27 1","pages":"6"},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2024-03-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10907435/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140011984","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The irreconcilability of insight. 洞察力的不可调和性。
IF 1.9 2区 生物学 Q3 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-03-02 DOI: 10.1007/s10071-024-01844-y
Eli Shupe

We are said to experience insight when we suddenly and unexpectedly become aware of the solution to a problem that we previously took ourselves to be unable to solve. In the field of comparative cognition, there is rising interest in the question of whether non-human animals are capable of insightful problem-solving. Putative cases of animals demonstrating insight have generally attracted two types of criticism: first, that insight is being conflated with other cognitive capacities (e.g., causal cognition, or mental trial and error); and, second, that the relevant performances merely reflect associative learning-and on the received understanding of insight within comparative cognition, insight necessarily involves non-associative processes. I argue that even if we grant that some cases of animal insight do withstand these two criticisms, these cases of purported animal insight cannot shed light on the nature of insightful problem-solving in humans. For the phenomenon studied by cognitive psychologists under the heading of insight is fundamentally different from that studied in comparative cognition. In light of this impasse, I argue that the reinterpretation of the extant research on animal insight in terms of other high-level cognitive capacities (means-end reasoning in particular) can improve the prospect of a successful comparative research program.

据说,当我们突然意外地意识到一个问题的解决方案时,我们就会体验到洞察力,而我们之前一直认为自己无法解决这个问题。在比较认知领域,人们对非人类动物是否能够有洞察力地解决问题越来越感兴趣。动物表现出洞察力的推测案例通常会招致两类批评:第一,洞察力被与其他认知能力(如因果认知或心理试错)混为一谈;第二,相关表现仅仅反映了联想学习--而根据比较认知中对洞察力的理解,洞察力必然涉及非联想过程。我认为,即使我们承认某些动物洞察力的案例确实经得起这两点批评,这些所谓动物洞察力的案例也不能揭示人类洞察力解决问题的本质。因为认知心理学家在 "洞察力 "标题下所研究的现象与比较认知所研究的现象有着本质的不同。鉴于这种僵局,我认为,从其他高级认知能力(尤其是手段-目的推理)的角度重新解释现有的动物洞察力研究,可以改善比较研究计划取得成功的前景。
{"title":"The irreconcilability of insight.","authors":"Eli Shupe","doi":"10.1007/s10071-024-01844-y","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10071-024-01844-y","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>We are said to experience insight when we suddenly and unexpectedly become aware of the solution to a problem that we previously took ourselves to be unable to solve. In the field of comparative cognition, there is rising interest in the question of whether non-human animals are capable of insightful problem-solving. Putative cases of animals demonstrating insight have generally attracted two types of criticism: first, that insight is being conflated with other cognitive capacities (e.g., causal cognition, or mental trial and error); and, second, that the relevant performances merely reflect associative learning-and on the received understanding of insight within comparative cognition, insight necessarily involves non-associative processes. I argue that even if we grant that some cases of animal insight do withstand these two criticisms, these cases of purported animal insight cannot shed light on the nature of insightful problem-solving in humans. For the phenomenon studied by cognitive psychologists under the heading of insight is fundamentally different from that studied in comparative cognition. In light of this impasse, I argue that the reinterpretation of the extant research on animal insight in terms of other high-level cognitive capacities (means-end reasoning in particular) can improve the prospect of a successful comparative research program.</p>","PeriodicalId":7879,"journal":{"name":"Animal Cognition","volume":"27 1","pages":"16"},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2024-03-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10907412/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140011988","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
A first exploratory comparison of the behaviour of wolves (Canis lupus) and wolf-dog hybrids in captivity. 首次探索性比较狼(Canis lupus)和狼狗杂交种在圈养条件下的行为。
IF 1.9 2区 生物学 Q3 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-03-02 DOI: 10.1007/s10071-024-01849-7
Federica Amici, Simone Meacci, Emmeline Caray, Linda Oña, Katja Liebal, Paolo Ciucci

Extensive introgression of genes from domesticated taxa may be a serious threat for the genomic integrity and adaptability of wild populations. Grey wolves (Canis lupus) are especially vulnerable to this phenomenon, but there are no studies yet assessing the potential behavioural effects of dog-introgression in wolves. In this study, we conducted a first systematic comparison of admixed (N = 11) and non-admixed (N = 14) wolves in captivity, focusing on their reaction to unfamiliar humans and novel objects, and the cohesiveness of their social groups. When exposed to unfamiliar humans in the experimental task, wolves were more vigilant, fearful and aggressive than admixed wolves, and less likely to approach humans, but also more likely to spend time in human proximity. When exposed to novel objects, wolves were more aggressive than admixed wolves, less likely to spend time in object proximity, and more likely to interact with objects, but also less vigilant and as fearful as admixed wolves. Finally, social networks were more cohesive in wolves than in admixed wolves. Although caution is needed when comparing groups of captive individuals with different life experiences, our study suggests that dog admixture may lead to important behavioural changes in wolves, with possible implications for conservation strategies.

驯化类群基因的广泛引入可能会严重威胁野生种群基因组的完整性和适应性。灰狼(Canis lupus)尤其容易受到这种现象的影响,但目前还没有研究评估狗对狼的基因导入可能产生的行为影响。在这项研究中,我们首次对人工饲养的混血狼(11 头)和非混血狼(14 头)进行了系统比较,重点研究了它们对陌生人类和新事物的反应,以及其社会群体的凝聚力。在实验任务中,当接触到陌生人类时,狼比混血狼更加警惕、恐惧和具有攻击性,更少接近人类,但也更愿意在人类附近逗留。当接触到新的物体时,狼比混血狼更具攻击性,不太可能花时间接近物体,也更可能与物体互动,但警惕性和恐惧感也不如混血狼。最后,狼的社会网络比混血狼更有凝聚力。尽管在比较具有不同生活经历的圈养个体群体时需要谨慎,但我们的研究表明,狗的混血可能会导致狼的重要行为变化,并可能对保护策略产生影响。
{"title":"A first exploratory comparison of the behaviour of wolves (Canis lupus) and wolf-dog hybrids in captivity.","authors":"Federica Amici, Simone Meacci, Emmeline Caray, Linda Oña, Katja Liebal, Paolo Ciucci","doi":"10.1007/s10071-024-01849-7","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10071-024-01849-7","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Extensive introgression of genes from domesticated taxa may be a serious threat for the genomic integrity and adaptability of wild populations. Grey wolves (Canis lupus) are especially vulnerable to this phenomenon, but there are no studies yet assessing the potential behavioural effects of dog-introgression in wolves. In this study, we conducted a first systematic comparison of admixed (N = 11) and non-admixed (N = 14) wolves in captivity, focusing on their reaction to unfamiliar humans and novel objects, and the cohesiveness of their social groups. When exposed to unfamiliar humans in the experimental task, wolves were more vigilant, fearful and aggressive than admixed wolves, and less likely to approach humans, but also more likely to spend time in human proximity. When exposed to novel objects, wolves were more aggressive than admixed wolves, less likely to spend time in object proximity, and more likely to interact with objects, but also less vigilant and as fearful as admixed wolves. Finally, social networks were more cohesive in wolves than in admixed wolves. Although caution is needed when comparing groups of captive individuals with different life experiences, our study suggests that dog admixture may lead to important behavioural changes in wolves, with possible implications for conservation strategies.</p>","PeriodicalId":7879,"journal":{"name":"Animal Cognition","volume":"27 1","pages":"9"},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2024-03-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10907477/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140012042","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
How do animals weigh conflicting information about reward sources over time? Comparing dynamic averaging models. 动物如何权衡奖励来源随时间变化的冲突信息?比较动态平均模型
IF 1.9 2区 生物学 Q3 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-03-02 DOI: 10.1007/s10071-024-01840-2
Jack Van Allsburg, Timothy A Shahan

Optimal foraging theory suggests that animals make decisions which maximize their food intake per unit time when foraging, but the mechanisms animals use to track the value of behavioral alternatives and choose between them remain unclear. Several models for how animals integrate past experience have been suggested. However, these models make differential predictions for the occurrence of spontaneous recovery of choice: a behavioral phenomenon in which a hiatus from the experimental environment results in animals reverting to a behavioral allocation consistent with a reward distribution from the more distant past, rather than one consistent with their most recently experienced distribution. To explore this phenomenon and compare these models, three free-operant experiments with rats were conducted using a serial reversal design. In Phase 1, two responses (A and B) were baited with pellets on concurrent variable interval schedules, favoring option A. In Phase 2, lever baiting was reversed to favor option B. Rats then entered a delay period, where they were maintained at weight in their home cages and no experimental sessions took place. Following this delay, preference was assessed using initial responding in test sessions where levers were presented, but not baited. Models were compared in performance, including an exponentially weighted moving average, the Temporal Weighting Rule, and variants of these models. While the data provided strong evidence of spontaneous recovery of choice, the form and extent of recovery was inconsistent with the models under investigation. Potential interpretations are discussed in relation to both the decision rule and valuation functions employed.

最佳觅食理论认为,动物在觅食时会做出使单位时间内食物摄入量最大化的决定,但动物用来跟踪行为选择的价值并在它们之间做出选择的机制仍不清楚。关于动物如何整合过去的经验,已经提出了几种模型。然而,这些模型对自发恢复选择的发生做出了不同的预测:在这种行为现象中,实验环境的间断会导致动物恢复到与更久远的过去的奖励分配相一致的行为分配,而不是与它们最近经历的分配相一致的行为分配。为了探索这一现象并对这些模型进行比较,我们采用序列反转设计对大鼠进行了三次自由操作实验。在第一阶段,在同时进行的可变间隔计划中,用颗粒作为诱饵诱导大鼠做出两种反应(A 和 B),大鼠倾向于选择 A;在第二阶段,杠杆诱导反转,倾向于选择 B。延缓期后,在出现杠杆但没有诱饵的测试环节中,使用初始反应来评估偏好。对各种模型的性能进行了比较,包括指数加权移动平均法、时间加权法则以及这些模型的变体。虽然数据提供了选择自发恢复的有力证据,但恢复的形式和程度与所研究的模型并不一致。本文讨论了与所采用的决策规则和估值函数有关的潜在解释。
{"title":"How do animals weigh conflicting information about reward sources over time? Comparing dynamic averaging models.","authors":"Jack Van Allsburg, Timothy A Shahan","doi":"10.1007/s10071-024-01840-2","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10071-024-01840-2","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Optimal foraging theory suggests that animals make decisions which maximize their food intake per unit time when foraging, but the mechanisms animals use to track the value of behavioral alternatives and choose between them remain unclear. Several models for how animals integrate past experience have been suggested. However, these models make differential predictions for the occurrence of spontaneous recovery of choice: a behavioral phenomenon in which a hiatus from the experimental environment results in animals reverting to a behavioral allocation consistent with a reward distribution from the more distant past, rather than one consistent with their most recently experienced distribution. To explore this phenomenon and compare these models, three free-operant experiments with rats were conducted using a serial reversal design. In Phase 1, two responses (A and B) were baited with pellets on concurrent variable interval schedules, favoring option A. In Phase 2, lever baiting was reversed to favor option B. Rats then entered a delay period, where they were maintained at weight in their home cages and no experimental sessions took place. Following this delay, preference was assessed using initial responding in test sessions where levers were presented, but not baited. Models were compared in performance, including an exponentially weighted moving average, the Temporal Weighting Rule, and variants of these models. While the data provided strong evidence of spontaneous recovery of choice, the form and extent of recovery was inconsistent with the models under investigation. Potential interpretations are discussed in relation to both the decision rule and valuation functions employed.</p>","PeriodicalId":7879,"journal":{"name":"Animal Cognition","volume":"27 1","pages":"11"},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2024-03-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10907467/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140012050","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Chunking as a function of sequence length 分块是序列长度的函数。
IF 1.9 2区 生物学 Q3 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-03-02 DOI: 10.1007/s10071-024-01835-z
Laure Tosatto, Joël Fagot, Dezso Nemeth, Arnaud Rey

Chunking mechanisms are central to several cognitive processes. During the acquisition of visuo-motor sequences, it is commonly reported that these sequences are segmented into chunks leading to more fluid, rapid, and accurate performances. The question of a chunk’s storage capacity has been often investigated but little is known about the dynamics of chunk size evolution relative to sequence length. In two experiments, we studied the dynamics and the evolution of a sequence’s chunking pattern as a function of sequence length in a non-human primate species (Guinea baboons, Papio papio). Using an operant conditioning device, baboons had to point on a touch screen to a moving target. In Experiment 1, they had to produce repeatedly the same sequence of 4 movements during 2000 trials. In Experiment 2, the sequence was composed of 5 movements and was repeated 4000 times. For both lengths, baboons initially produced small chunks that became fewer and longer with practice. Moreover, the dynamics and the evolution of the chunking pattern varied as a function of sequence length. Finally, with extended practice (i.e., more than 2000 trials), we observed that the mean chunk size reached a plateau indicating that there are fundamental limits to chunking processes that also depend on sequence length. These data therefore provide new empirical evidence for understanding the general properties of chunking mechanisms in sequence learning.

分块机制是若干认知过程的核心。据报道,在视觉运动序列的习得过程中,这些序列通常会被分割成块,从而获得更流畅、快速和准确的表现。人们经常研究块的存储容量问题,但对块的大小相对于序列长度的动态演变却知之甚少。在两项实验中,我们研究了非人灵长类动物(几内亚狒狒,Papio papio)序列的分块模式随序列长度变化的动态和演变过程。通过操作性条件反射装置,狒狒必须在触摸屏上指向一个移动的目标。在实验 1 中,狒狒必须在 2000 次试验中重复做出相同的 4 个动作序列。在实验 2 中,该序列由 5 个动作组成,重复 4000 次。对于这两种长度的动作,狒狒最初都能做出小块的动作,随着练习的进行,动作的数量越来越少,长度也越来越长。此外,分块模式的动态和演变也随序列长度的变化而变化。最后,随着练习时间的延长(即超过2000次试验),我们观察到平均分块大小达到了一个高点,这表明分块过程存在基本限制,而这些限制也取决于序列长度。因此,这些数据为理解序列学习中分块机制的一般特性提供了新的经验证据。
{"title":"Chunking as a function of sequence length","authors":"Laure Tosatto,&nbsp;Joël Fagot,&nbsp;Dezso Nemeth,&nbsp;Arnaud Rey","doi":"10.1007/s10071-024-01835-z","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10071-024-01835-z","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Chunking mechanisms are central to several cognitive processes. During the acquisition of visuo-motor sequences, it is commonly reported that these sequences are segmented into chunks leading to more fluid, rapid, and accurate performances. The question of a chunk’s storage capacity has been often investigated but little is known about the dynamics of chunk size evolution relative to sequence length. In two experiments, we studied the dynamics and the evolution of a sequence’s chunking pattern as a function of sequence length in a non-human primate species (Guinea baboons, <i>Papio papio</i>). Using an operant conditioning device, baboons had to point on a touch screen to a moving target. In Experiment 1, they had to produce repeatedly the same sequence of 4 movements during 2000 trials. In Experiment 2, the sequence was composed of 5 movements and was repeated 4000 times. For both lengths, baboons initially produced small chunks that became fewer and longer with practice. Moreover, the dynamics and the evolution of the chunking pattern varied as a function of sequence length. Finally, with extended practice (i.e., more than 2000 trials), we observed that the mean chunk size reached a plateau indicating that there are fundamental limits to chunking processes that also depend on sequence length. These data therefore provide new empirical evidence for understanding the general properties of chunking mechanisms in sequence learning.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":7879,"journal":{"name":"Animal Cognition","volume":"28 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2024-03-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://link.springer.com/content/pdf/10.1007/s10071-024-01835-z.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140011990","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Animal Cognition
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1