首页 > 最新文献

Behavioral neuroscience最新文献

英文 中文
Supplemental Material for The Effects of Time Horizon and Guided Choices on Explore–Exploit Decisions in Rodents 时间范围和引导选择对啮齿动物探索-开发决策的影响补充材料
IF 1.9 4区 医学 Q3 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2023-01-09 DOI: 10.1037/bne0000549.supp
{"title":"Supplemental Material for The Effects of Time Horizon and Guided Choices on Explore–Exploit Decisions in Rodents","authors":"","doi":"10.1037/bne0000549.supp","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/bne0000549.supp","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":8739,"journal":{"name":"Behavioral neuroscience","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2023-01-09","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"48000089","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Lack of robust associations between prepandemic coping strategies and frontolimbic circuitry with depression and anxiety symptoms during the COVID-19 pandemic: A preregistered longitudinal study. 在COVID-19大流行期间,大流行前应对策略与前额叶回路与抑郁和焦虑症状之间缺乏强有力的关联:一项预注册的纵向研究。
IF 1.9 4区 医学 Q3 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2022-12-01 DOI: 10.1037/bne0000534
Bailey Holt-Gosselin, Emily M Cohodes, Sarah McCauley, Jordan C Foster, Paola Odriozola, Sadie J Zacharek, Sahana Kribakaran, Jason T Haberman, H R Hodges, Dylan G Gee

The COVID-19 pandemic is an ongoing stressor that has resulted in the exacerbation of mental health problems worldwide. However, longitudinal studies that identify preexisting behavioral and neurobiological factors associated with mental health outcomes during the pandemic are lacking. Here, we examined associations between prepandemic coping strategy engagement and frontolimbic circuitry with internalizing symptoms during the pandemic. In 85 adults (71.8% female; age 18-30 years), we assessed prototypically adaptive coping strategies (Connor-Davidson Resilience Scale), resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging functional connectivity (FC) of frontolimbic circuitry, and depression and anxiety symptoms (Beck Depression Inventory, Screen for Child Anxiety-Related Emotional Disorders-Adult, respectively). We conducted general linear models to test preregistered hypotheses that (1) lower coping engagement prepandemic and (2) weaker frontolimbic FC prepandemic would predict elevated symptoms during the pandemic; and (3) coping would interact with FC to predict symptoms during the pandemic. Depression and anxiety symptoms worsened during the pandemic (ps < .001). Prepandemic adaptive coping engagement and frontolimbic FC were not associated with depression or anxiety symptoms during the pandemic (uncorrected ps > .05). Coping interacted with insula-rostral anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) FC (p = .003, pFDR = .014) and with insula-ventral ACC FC (p < .001, pFDR < .001) to predict depression symptoms, but these findings did not survive FDR correction after removal of outliers. Findings from our preregistered study suggest that specific prepandemic factors, particularly adaptive coping and frontolimbic circuitry, are not robustly associated with emotional responses to the pandemic. Additional studies that identify preexisting neurobehavioral factors implicated in mental health outcomes during global health crises are needed. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).

2019冠状病毒病大流行是一个持续的压力源,导致全球精神卫生问题加剧。然而,缺乏确定大流行期间与心理健康结果相关的先前存在的行为和神经生物学因素的纵向研究。在这里,我们研究了大流行前应对策略参与与大流行期间内化症状的额叶边缘回路之间的关系。85例成人(71.8%为女性;年龄18-30岁),我们评估了原型适应性应对策略(康纳-戴维森弹性量表),静息状态功能磁共振成像功能连通性(FC)额叶边缘电路,抑郁和焦虑症状(贝克抑郁量表,儿童焦虑相关情绪障碍筛查-成人)。我们进行了一般线性模型来检验预先登记的假设,即:(1)大流行前较低的应对参与度和(2)大流行前较弱的额叶皮质区可以预测大流行期间症状的升高;(3)应对将与FC相互作用,以预测大流行期间的症状。大流行期间抑郁和焦虑症状加重(ps < 0.001)。大流行前的适应性应对参与和额叶边缘FC与大流行期间的抑郁或焦虑症状无关(未校正的ps > 0.05)。应对与岛吻侧前扣带皮层(ACC) FC (p = 0.003, pFDR = 0.014)和岛腹侧前扣带皮层FC (p < 0.001, pFDR < 0.001)相互作用,预测抑郁症状,但在去除异常值后,这些发现无法在FDR校正后存活。我们预先登记的研究结果表明,特定的大流行前因素,特别是适应性应对和额叶边缘电路,与对大流行的情绪反应没有密切联系。需要进行进一步的研究,以确定在全球健康危机期间与心理健康结果有关的预先存在的神经行为因素。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"Lack of robust associations between prepandemic coping strategies and frontolimbic circuitry with depression and anxiety symptoms during the COVID-19 pandemic: A preregistered longitudinal study.","authors":"Bailey Holt-Gosselin, Emily M Cohodes, Sarah McCauley, Jordan C Foster, Paola Odriozola, Sadie J Zacharek, Sahana Kribakaran, Jason T Haberman, H R Hodges, Dylan G Gee","doi":"10.1037/bne0000534","DOIUrl":"10.1037/bne0000534","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The COVID-19 pandemic is an ongoing stressor that has resulted in the exacerbation of mental health problems worldwide. However, longitudinal studies that identify preexisting behavioral and neurobiological factors associated with mental health outcomes during the pandemic are lacking. Here, we examined associations between prepandemic coping strategy engagement and frontolimbic circuitry with internalizing symptoms during the pandemic. In 85 adults (71.8% female; age 18-30 years), we assessed prototypically adaptive coping strategies (Connor-Davidson Resilience Scale), resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging functional connectivity (FC) of frontolimbic circuitry, and depression and anxiety symptoms (Beck Depression Inventory, Screen for Child Anxiety-Related Emotional Disorders-Adult, respectively). We conducted general linear models to test preregistered hypotheses that (1) lower coping engagement prepandemic and (2) weaker frontolimbic FC prepandemic would predict elevated symptoms during the pandemic; and (3) coping would interact with FC to predict symptoms during the pandemic. Depression and anxiety symptoms worsened during the pandemic (ps < .001). Prepandemic adaptive coping engagement and frontolimbic FC were not associated with depression or anxiety symptoms during the pandemic (uncorrected ps > .05). Coping interacted with insula-rostral anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) FC (p = .003, pFDR = .014) and with insula-ventral ACC FC (p < .001, pFDR < .001) to predict depression symptoms, but these findings did not survive FDR correction after removal of outliers. Findings from our preregistered study suggest that specific prepandemic factors, particularly adaptive coping and frontolimbic circuitry, are not robustly associated with emotional responses to the pandemic. Additional studies that identify preexisting neurobehavioral factors implicated in mental health outcomes during global health crises are needed. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":8739,"journal":{"name":"Behavioral neuroscience","volume":"136 6","pages":"528-540"},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2022-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC9884515/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9200173","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Chemogenetic inhibition of corticotropin-releasing factor neurons in the central amygdala alters binge-like ethanol consumption in male mice. 中央杏仁核促肾上腺皮质激素释放因子神经元的化学发生抑制改变了雄性小鼠的狂饮样乙醇消耗。
IF 1.9 4区 医学 Q3 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2022-12-01 Epub Date: 2022-06-30 DOI: 10.1037/bne0000522
S Alex Marshall, Stacey L Robinson, Suzahn E Ebert, Michel A Companion, Todd E Thiele

Repetitive bouts of binge drinking can lead to neuroplastic events that alter ethanol's pharmacologic effects and perpetuate excessive consumption. The corticotropin-releasing factor (CRF) system is an example of ethanol-induced neuroadaptations that drive excessive ethanol consumption. Our laboratory has previously shown that CRF antagonist, when infused into the central amygdala (CeA), reduces binge-like ethanol consumption. The present study extends this research by assessing the effects of silencing CRF-producing neurons in CeA on binge-like ethanol drinking stemming from "Drinking in the Dark" (DID) procedures. CRF-ires-Cre mice underwent surgery to infuse Gi/o-coupled Designer Receptors Exclusively Activated by Designer Drugs (DREADD) virus or a control virus into either the CeA or basolateral amygdala (BLA). Gi/o-DREADD-induced CRF-neuronal inhibition in the CeA resulted in a 33% decrease in binge-like ethanol consumption. However, no effect on ethanol consumption was seen after DREADD manipulation in the BLA. Moreover, CeA CRF-neuronal inhibition had no effect on sucrose consumption. The effects of silencing CRF neurons in the CeA on ethanol consumption are not secondary to changes in motor function or anxiety-like behaviors as assessed in the open-field test (OFT). Finally, the DREADD construct's functional ability to inhibit CRF-neuronal activity was demonstrated by reduced ethanol-induced c-Fos following DREADD activation. Together, these data suggest that the CRF neurons in the CeA play an important role in binge ethanol consumption and that inhibition of the CRF-signaling pathway remains a viable target for manipulating binge-like ethanol consumption. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).

反复的酗酒会导致神经可塑性事件,从而改变乙醇的药理作用,使过量饮酒永续下去。促肾上腺皮质激素释放因子(CRF)系统是乙醇诱导的神经适应驱动过量乙醇消耗的一个例子。我们的实验室之前已经证明,当注入中央杏仁核(CeA)时,CRF拮抗剂可以减少酗酒样的乙醇消耗。本研究通过评估沉默CeA中产生crf的神经元对“在黑暗中饮酒”(DID)过程中产生的酗酒样酒精饮酒的影响,扩展了这一研究。CRF-ires-Cre小鼠接受手术,将设计药物特异性激活的Gi/o偶联设计器受体(DREADD)病毒或对照病毒注入CeA或基底外侧杏仁核(BLA)。Gi/o- dreadd诱导的CeA中crf神经元抑制导致狂饮样乙醇消耗减少33%。然而,在BLA中进行DREADD操作后,对乙醇消耗量没有影响。此外,CeA crf神经元抑制对蔗糖消耗没有影响。在开放场试验(OFT)中评估,沉默CeA中CRF神经元对乙醇消耗的影响并非继发于运动功能或焦虑样行为的改变。最后,在DREADD激活后,通过减少乙醇诱导的c-Fos,证明了DREADD结构抑制crf神经元活性的功能能力。综上所述,这些数据表明,CeA中的CRF神经元在狂饮乙醇消费中起着重要作用,抑制CRF信号通路仍然是操纵狂饮乙醇消费的可行靶点。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"Chemogenetic inhibition of corticotropin-releasing factor neurons in the central amygdala alters binge-like ethanol consumption in male mice.","authors":"S Alex Marshall, Stacey L Robinson, Suzahn E Ebert, Michel A Companion, Todd E Thiele","doi":"10.1037/bne0000522","DOIUrl":"10.1037/bne0000522","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Repetitive bouts of binge drinking can lead to neuroplastic events that alter ethanol's pharmacologic effects and perpetuate excessive consumption. The corticotropin-releasing factor (CRF) system is an example of ethanol-induced neuroadaptations that drive excessive ethanol consumption. Our laboratory has previously shown that CRF antagonist, when infused into the central amygdala (CeA), reduces binge-like ethanol consumption. The present study extends this research by assessing the effects of silencing CRF-producing neurons in CeA on binge-like ethanol drinking stemming from \"Drinking in the Dark\" (DID) procedures. CRF-ires-Cre mice underwent surgery to infuse G<sub>i/o</sub>-coupled Designer Receptors Exclusively Activated by Designer Drugs (DREADD) virus or a control virus into either the CeA or basolateral amygdala (BLA). G<sub>i/o</sub>-DREADD-induced CRF-neuronal inhibition in the CeA resulted in a 33% decrease in binge-like ethanol consumption. However, no effect on ethanol consumption was seen after DREADD manipulation in the BLA. Moreover, CeA CRF-neuronal inhibition had no effect on sucrose consumption. The effects of silencing CRF neurons in the CeA on ethanol consumption are not secondary to changes in motor function or anxiety-like behaviors as assessed in the open-field test (OFT). Finally, the DREADD construct's functional ability to inhibit CRF-neuronal activity was demonstrated by reduced ethanol-induced c-Fos following DREADD activation. Together, these data suggest that the CRF neurons in the CeA play an important role in binge ethanol consumption and that inhibition of the CRF-signaling pathway remains a viable target for manipulating binge-like ethanol consumption. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":8739,"journal":{"name":"Behavioral neuroscience","volume":"136 6","pages":"541-550"},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2022-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC9671851/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9501360","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Differential effects in young and aged rats' navigational accuracy following instantaneous rotation of environmental cues. 环境线索瞬时旋转对年轻和年老大鼠导航精度的不同影响。
IF 1.9 4区 医学 Q3 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2022-12-01 DOI: 10.1037/bne0000536
Adam W Lester, Gianna A Jordan, Colton J Blum, Zachary P Philpot, Carol A Barnes

Successful navigation depends critically upon two broad categories of spatial navigation strategies that include allocentric and egocentric reference frames, relying on external or internal spatial information, respectively. As with older adults, aged rats show robust impairments on a number of different spatial navigation tasks. There is some evidence that these navigation impairments are accompanied by a bias toward relying on egocentric over allocentric navigation strategies. To test the degree to which young and aged animals utilize these two navigation approaches, a novel behavioral arena was used in which rats are trained to traverse a circular track and to stop at a learned goal location that is fixed with respect to a panorama of visual cues projected onto the surrounding walls. By instantaneously rotating the cues, allocentric and egocentric reference frames were put in direct and immediate conflict and goal navigation performance was assessed with respect to how accurately young and aged animals were able to utilize the rotated cues. Behavioral data collected from nine young and eight aged animals revealed that both age groups were able to update their navigation performance following cue rotation. Contrary to what was expected, however, aged animals showed more accurate overall goal navigation performance, stronger allocentric strategy use, and more evident changes in behavior in response to cue rotation compared to younger animals. The young rats appeared to mix egocentric and allocentric strategies for ICR task solution. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).

成功的导航主要依赖于两大类空间导航策略,分别依赖于外部或内部空间信息,包括非中心和自我中心参考框架。与老年人一样,老年大鼠在许多不同的空间导航任务上表现出明显的损伤。有一些证据表明,这些导航障碍伴随着依赖自我中心而不是非中心导航策略的偏见。为了测试幼龄和老年动物利用这两种导航方法的程度,研究人员使用了一个新的行为竞技场,在这个竞技场中,老鼠被训练穿过一个圆形轨道,并在一个习得的目标位置停下来,这个目标位置与投射到周围墙壁上的视觉线索的全景有关。通过即时旋转线索,将非中心和自我中心的参考框架置于直接和即时冲突中,并评估幼鼠和老年鼠利用旋转线索的准确程度。从9只年幼和8只年老的动物身上收集的行为数据显示,这两个年龄组都能够在线索旋转后更新他们的导航表现。然而,与预期相反,与年轻动物相比,老年动物表现出更准确的总体目标导航性能,更强的异中心策略使用,以及更明显的响应线索旋转的行为变化。年轻大鼠在ICR任务解决中表现出自我中心和非中心混合策略。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"Differential effects in young and aged rats' navigational accuracy following instantaneous rotation of environmental cues.","authors":"Adam W Lester, Gianna A Jordan, Colton J Blum, Zachary P Philpot, Carol A Barnes","doi":"10.1037/bne0000536","DOIUrl":"10.1037/bne0000536","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Successful navigation depends critically upon two broad categories of spatial navigation strategies that include allocentric and egocentric reference frames, relying on external or internal spatial information, respectively. As with older adults, aged rats show robust impairments on a number of different spatial navigation tasks. There is some evidence that these navigation impairments are accompanied by a bias toward relying on egocentric over allocentric navigation strategies. To test the degree to which young and aged animals utilize these two navigation approaches, a novel behavioral arena was used in which rats are trained to traverse a circular track and to stop at a learned goal location that is fixed with respect to a panorama of visual cues projected onto the surrounding walls. By instantaneously rotating the cues, allocentric and egocentric reference frames were put in direct and immediate conflict and goal navigation performance was assessed with respect to how accurately young and aged animals were able to utilize the rotated cues. Behavioral data collected from nine young and eight aged animals revealed that both age groups were able to update their navigation performance following cue rotation. Contrary to what was expected, however, aged animals showed more accurate overall goal navigation performance, stronger allocentric strategy use, and more evident changes in behavior in response to cue rotation compared to younger animals. The young rats appeared to mix egocentric and allocentric strategies for ICR task solution. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":8739,"journal":{"name":"Behavioral neuroscience","volume":"136 6","pages":"561-574"},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2022-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10482423/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10220283","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Supplemental Material for Cognitive and Arginine Metabolic Correlates of Temporal Dysfunction in the MIA Rat Model of Schizophrenia Risk MIA大鼠精神分裂症风险模型中认知和精氨酸代谢与颞功能障碍相关的补充材料
IF 1.9 4区 医学 Q3 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2022-10-24 DOI: 10.1037/bne0000540.supp
{"title":"Supplemental Material for Cognitive and Arginine Metabolic Correlates of Temporal Dysfunction in the MIA Rat Model of Schizophrenia Risk","authors":"","doi":"10.1037/bne0000540.supp","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/bne0000540.supp","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":8739,"journal":{"name":"Behavioral neuroscience","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2022-10-24","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"44311778","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Behavioral and neurochemical effects of nociceptin/orphanin FQ receptor activation in the social defeat protocol. 社交失败方案中伤害肽/孤啡肽FQ受体激活的行为和神经化学效应。
IF 1.9 4区 医学 Q3 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2022-10-24 DOI: 10.1037/bne0000539.supp
Alice Barros Câmara, Igor Augusto Brandão
The nociceptin/orphanin FQ receptor (NOP receptor) has wide expression in the nervous system and is involved in neurotransmitter release. However, the role of the NOPR in depression is not widely recognized. This study aims to evaluate behavioral and biochemical effects of the NOPR agonist Ro 65-6570 in mice submitted to social defeat protocol. The open-field test, social interaction test, and tail suspension test were applied to evaluate depressive behavior in male Swiss mice. Blood and brain tissue samples were obtained to evaluate the oxidative stress. The NOP agonist, Ro 65-6570 (1 mg/kg), or the social defeat stress reduced exploration rate in the open-field test. The social defeat stress and/or the NOP agonist also increased immobility time in the tail suspension test and the grooming time, as well as reduced the social interaction on the last day of social defeat protocol. Seven days after the end of the protocol, only the drug alone was able to affect the animals' interaction. Additionally, the NOP agonist increased the concentration of carbonyl groups (CGs) in hippocampus and malondialdehyde in serum. The stress of social defeat and the NOP agonist, together, increased malondialdehyde in animals' serum and prefrontal cortex, as well as increased the CGs concentration in the prefrontal cortex. These findings indicate a chronic depressive effect induced by the NOPR activation, sometimes regardless of the social defeat stress. We suggest that the NOPR signaling can activate pathways involved in cellular oxidative stress, contributing to the depression pathology. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).
伤害肽/孤啡肽FQ受体(NOP受体)在神经系统中广泛表达,并参与神经递质的释放。然而,NOPR在抑郁症中的作用并没有得到广泛的认识。本研究旨在评估NOPR激动剂Ro65-6570对接受社交失败方案的小鼠的行为和生化影响。采用开放场地试验、社会互动试验和尾部悬吊试验对雄性瑞士小鼠的抑郁行为进行评估。获得血液和脑组织样本以评估氧化应激。NOP激动剂Ro 65-6570(1 mg/kg)或社交失败应激降低了野外试验中的探索率。社交失败压力和/或NOP激动剂也增加了尾部悬吊测试中的不动时间和梳理时间,并减少了社交失败协议最后一天的社交互动。方案结束七天后,只有单独的药物能够影响动物的相互作用。此外,NOP激动剂增加了海马中羰基(CGs)的浓度和血清中丙二醛的浓度。社交失败的压力和NOP激动剂一起增加了动物血清和前额叶皮层中的丙二醛,并增加了前额叶皮层的CGs浓度。这些发现表明,NOPR激活会引起慢性抑郁效应,有时与社交失败压力无关。我们认为,NOPR信号可以激活参与细胞氧化应激的途径,从而促进抑郁症的病理学。(PsycInfo数据库记录(c)2022 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"Behavioral and neurochemical effects of nociceptin/orphanin FQ receptor activation in the social defeat protocol.","authors":"Alice Barros Câmara, Igor Augusto Brandão","doi":"10.1037/bne0000539.supp","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/bne0000539.supp","url":null,"abstract":"The nociceptin/orphanin FQ receptor (NOP receptor) has wide expression in the nervous system and is involved in neurotransmitter release. However, the role of the NOPR in depression is not widely recognized. This study aims to evaluate behavioral and biochemical effects of the NOPR agonist Ro 65-6570 in mice submitted to social defeat protocol. The open-field test, social interaction test, and tail suspension test were applied to evaluate depressive behavior in male Swiss mice. Blood and brain tissue samples were obtained to evaluate the oxidative stress. The NOP agonist, Ro 65-6570 (1 mg/kg), or the social defeat stress reduced exploration rate in the open-field test. The social defeat stress and/or the NOP agonist also increased immobility time in the tail suspension test and the grooming time, as well as reduced the social interaction on the last day of social defeat protocol. Seven days after the end of the protocol, only the drug alone was able to affect the animals' interaction. Additionally, the NOP agonist increased the concentration of carbonyl groups (CGs) in hippocampus and malondialdehyde in serum. The stress of social defeat and the NOP agonist, together, increased malondialdehyde in animals' serum and prefrontal cortex, as well as increased the CGs concentration in the prefrontal cortex. These findings indicate a chronic depressive effect induced by the NOPR activation, sometimes regardless of the social defeat stress. We suggest that the NOPR signaling can activate pathways involved in cellular oxidative stress, contributing to the depression pathology. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).","PeriodicalId":8739,"journal":{"name":"Behavioral neuroscience","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2022-10-24","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"48465774","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Sex and estrous cycle in memory for sequences of events in rats. 大鼠记忆中事件序列的性周期和发情周期。
IF 1.9 4区 医学 Q3 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2022-10-01 Epub Date: 2022-03-07 DOI: 10.1037/bne0000508
M Jayachandran, P Langius, F Pazos Rego, R P Vertes, T A Allen

The ability to remember sequences of events is fundamental to episodic memory. While rodent studies have examined sex and estrous cycle in episodic-like spatial memory tasks, little is known about these biological variables in memory for sequences of events that depend on representations of temporal context. We investigated the role of sex and estrous cycle in rats during training and testing stages of a cross-species validated sequence memory task (Jayachandran et al., 2019). Rats were trained on a two four-odor sequence memory task delivered on opposite ends of a linear track. Training occurred in six successive stages starting with learning to poke in a nose-port for ≥ 1.2 s; eventually demonstrating sequence memory by holding their nose in the port ≥ 1 s for in-sequence odors and < 1 s for out-of-sequence odors. Performance was analyzed across sex and estrous cycle (proestrus, estrus, metestrus, and diestrus), the latter being determined by cellular composition of a daily vaginal lavage. We found no evidence of sex differences in asymptotic sequence memory performance, similar to humans performing an analogous task (Reeders et al., 2021). Likewise, no differences in sequence memory performance were found across the estrous cycle. Some caveats are that males acquired out-of-sequence trials faster during training with a 3-odor sequence, but this apparent advantage did not carry over to the 4-odor sequence. Additionally, males had shorter poke times overall which seem consistent with a decreased overall response inhibition because they occurred regardless of sequence demands. Together, these results suggest sex and estrous cycle are not major factors in sequence memory capacities. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).

记忆事件序列的能力是情节记忆的基础。虽然啮齿类动物的研究已经在类似情景的空间记忆任务中检查了性和发情周期,但对于依赖于时间上下文表征的事件序列的记忆中的这些生物变量知之甚少。我们研究了在跨物种验证序列记忆任务的训练和测试阶段,性和发情周期在大鼠中的作用(Jayachandran等人,2019)。大鼠接受了在线性轨道两端进行的两个四气味序列记忆任务的训练。训练分为六个连续阶段,从学习戳鼻孔开始,持续时间≥1.2秒;最终通过将鼻子保持在端口中来证明序列记忆,对于序列内气味≥1秒,对于序列外气味<1s。分析了性别和发情周期(发情前期、发情期、中期和发情期)的表现,后者由每日阴道灌洗的细胞组成决定。我们没有发现渐进序列记忆表现存在性别差异的证据,类似于人类执行类似任务(Reeders等人,2021)。同样,在整个发情周期中,没有发现序列记忆表现的差异。一些注意事项是,雄性在使用3气味序列的训练中更快地获得了序列外试验,但这种明显的优势并没有延续到4气味序列中。此外,雄性的戳戳时间总体较短,这似乎与总体反应抑制的降低一致,因为它们的发生与序列需求无关。总之,这些结果表明性和发情周期不是序列记忆能力的主要因素。(PsycInfo数据库记录(c)2022 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"Sex and estrous cycle in memory for sequences of events in rats.","authors":"M Jayachandran,&nbsp;P Langius,&nbsp;F Pazos Rego,&nbsp;R P Vertes,&nbsp;T A Allen","doi":"10.1037/bne0000508","DOIUrl":"10.1037/bne0000508","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The ability to remember sequences of events is fundamental to episodic memory. While rodent studies have examined sex and estrous cycle in episodic-like spatial memory tasks, little is known about these biological variables in memory for sequences of events that depend on representations of temporal context. We investigated the role of sex and estrous cycle in rats during training and testing stages of a cross-species validated sequence memory task (Jayachandran et al., 2019). Rats were trained on a two four-odor sequence memory task delivered on opposite ends of a linear track. Training occurred in six successive stages starting with learning to poke in a nose-port for ≥ 1.2 s; eventually demonstrating sequence memory by holding their nose in the port ≥ 1 s for in-sequence odors and < 1 s for out-of-sequence odors. Performance was analyzed across sex and estrous cycle (proestrus, estrus, metestrus, and diestrus), the latter being determined by cellular composition of a daily vaginal lavage. We found no evidence of sex differences in asymptotic sequence memory performance, similar to humans performing an analogous task (Reeders et al., 2021). Likewise, no differences in sequence memory performance were found across the estrous cycle. Some caveats are that males acquired out-of-sequence trials faster during training with a 3-odor sequence, but this apparent advantage did not carry over to the 4-odor sequence. Additionally, males had shorter poke times overall which seem consistent with a decreased overall response inhibition because they occurred regardless of sequence demands. Together, these results suggest sex and estrous cycle are not major factors in sequence memory capacities. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":8739,"journal":{"name":"Behavioral neuroscience","volume":"136 5","pages":"349-363"},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2022-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC9448822/pdf/nihms-1825637.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9497883","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
A limited cerebellar contribution to suprasecond timing across differing task demands. 小脑对跨不同任务要求的超秒计时的有限贡献。
IF 1.9 4区 医学 Q3 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2022-10-01 DOI: 10.1037/bne0000531
Kelsey A Heslin, Jessica R Purnell, Benjamin J De Corte, Krystal L Parker

The involvement of the cerebellum in suprasecond interval timing (i.e., timing in the seconds to minutes range) is controversial. A limited amount of evidence from humans, nonhuman primates, and rodents has shown that the lateral cerebellum, including the lateral cerebellar nucleus (LCN), may be necessary for successful suprasecond timing performance. However, many existing studies have pitfalls, such as limited timing outcome measures and confounded task demands. In addition, many existing studies relied on well-trained subjects. This approach may be a drawback, as the cerebellum is hypothesized to carry out ongoing error correction to limit timing variability. By using only experienced subjects, past timing studies may have missed a critical window of cerebellar involvement. In the experiments described here, we pharmacologically inactivated the rat LCN across three different peak interval timing tasks. We structured our tasks to address past confounds, collect timing variability measures, and characterize performance during target duration acquisition. Across these various tasks, we did not find strong support for cerebellar involvement in suprasecond interval timing. Our findings support the existing distinction of the cerebellum as a subsecond interval timing brain region. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).

小脑参与超秒间隔计时(即秒到分钟范围内的计时)是有争议的。来自人类、非人类灵长类动物和啮齿动物的有限证据表明,外侧小脑,包括外侧小脑核(LCN),可能是成功的超秒计时性能所必需的。然而,许多现有的研究存在缺陷,如时间限制,结果测量和混淆的任务需求。此外,许多现有的研究依赖于训练有素的受试者。这种方法可能是一个缺点,因为假设小脑进行持续的错误纠正以限制时间变异性。由于只使用有经验的受试者,过去的时间研究可能错过了小脑参与的关键窗口。在这里描述的实验中,我们通过三种不同的峰间隔定时任务从药理学上灭活了大鼠LCN。我们组织了我们的任务,以解决过去的混乱,收集时间可变性测量,并在目标持续时间获取期间描述性能。在这些不同的任务中,我们没有发现小脑参与超秒间隔计时的有力支持。我们的发现支持小脑作为亚秒间隔计时脑区域的现有区别。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"A limited cerebellar contribution to suprasecond timing across differing task demands.","authors":"Kelsey A Heslin,&nbsp;Jessica R Purnell,&nbsp;Benjamin J De Corte,&nbsp;Krystal L Parker","doi":"10.1037/bne0000531","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/bne0000531","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The involvement of the cerebellum in suprasecond interval timing (i.e., timing in the seconds to minutes range) is controversial. A limited amount of evidence from humans, nonhuman primates, and rodents has shown that the lateral cerebellum, including the lateral cerebellar nucleus (LCN), may be necessary for successful suprasecond timing performance. However, many existing studies have pitfalls, such as limited timing outcome measures and confounded task demands. In addition, many existing studies relied on well-trained subjects. This approach may be a drawback, as the cerebellum is hypothesized to carry out ongoing error correction to limit timing variability. By using only experienced subjects, past timing studies may have missed a critical window of cerebellar involvement. In the experiments described here, we pharmacologically inactivated the rat LCN across three different peak interval timing tasks. We structured our tasks to address past confounds, collect timing variability measures, and characterize performance during target duration acquisition. Across these various tasks, we did not find strong support for cerebellar involvement in suprasecond interval timing. Our findings support the existing distinction of the cerebellum as a subsecond interval timing brain region. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":8739,"journal":{"name":"Behavioral neuroscience","volume":"136 5","pages":"479-494"},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2022-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10538789","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
Neural population clocks: Encoding time in dynamic patterns of neural activity. 神经群体时钟:以神经活动的动态模式编码时间。
IF 1.6 4区 医学 Q3 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2022-10-01 Epub Date: 2022-04-21 DOI: 10.1037/bne0000515
Shanglin Zhou, Dean V Buonomano

The ability to predict and prepare for near- and far-future events is among the most fundamental computations the brain performs. Because of the importance of time for prediction and sensorimotor processing, the brain has evolved multiple mechanisms to tell and encode time across scales ranging from microseconds to days and beyond. Converging experimental and computational data indicate that, on the scale of seconds, timing relies on diverse neural mechanisms distributed across different brain areas. Among the different encoding mechanisms on the scale of seconds, we distinguish between neural population clocks and ramping activity as distinct strategies to encode time. One instance of neural population clocks, neural sequences, represents in some ways an optimal and flexible dynamic regime for the encoding of time. Specifically, neural sequences comprise a high-dimensional representation that can be used by downstream areas to flexibly generate arbitrarily simple and complex output patterns using biologically plausible learning rules. We propose that high-level integration areas may use high-dimensional dynamics such as neural sequences to encode time, providing downstream areas information to build low-dimensional ramp-like activity that can drive movements and temporal expectation. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).

预测和准备近期和远期事件的能力是大脑执行的最基本的计算之一。由于时间对预测和感觉运动处理的重要性,大脑已经进化出了多种机制,可以在从微秒到几天甚至更长的时间范围内告诉和编码时间。汇集实验和计算数据表明,在秒的尺度上,时间依赖于分布在不同大脑区域的不同神经机制。在以秒为单位的不同编码机制中,我们将神经群体时钟和斜坡活动区分为不同的时间编码策略。神经群体时钟的一个例子,神经序列,在某些方面代表了时间编码的最佳和灵活的动态机制。具体而言,神经序列包括高维表示,下游区域可以使用该高维表示来使用生物学上合理的学习规则灵活地生成任意简单和复杂的输出模式。我们提出,高级集成区域可以使用高维动力学(如神经序列)来编码时间,为下游区域提供信息,以构建低维斜坡状活动,从而驱动运动和时间预期。(PsycInfo数据库记录(c)2022 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"Neural population clocks: Encoding time in dynamic patterns of neural activity.","authors":"Shanglin Zhou, Dean V Buonomano","doi":"10.1037/bne0000515","DOIUrl":"10.1037/bne0000515","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The ability to predict and prepare for near- and far-future events is among the most fundamental computations the brain performs. Because of the importance of time for prediction and sensorimotor processing, the brain has evolved multiple mechanisms to tell and encode time across scales ranging from microseconds to days and beyond. Converging experimental and computational data indicate that, on the scale of seconds, timing relies on diverse neural mechanisms distributed across different brain areas. Among the different encoding mechanisms on the scale of seconds, we distinguish between neural population clocks and ramping activity as distinct strategies to encode time. One instance of neural population clocks, neural sequences, represents in some ways an optimal and flexible dynamic regime for the encoding of time. Specifically, neural sequences comprise a high-dimensional representation that can be used by downstream areas to flexibly generate arbitrarily simple and complex output patterns using biologically plausible learning rules. We propose that high-level integration areas may use high-dimensional dynamics such as neural sequences to encode time, providing downstream areas information to build low-dimensional ramp-like activity that can drive movements and temporal expectation. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":8739,"journal":{"name":"Behavioral neuroscience","volume":"136 5","pages":"374-382"},"PeriodicalIF":1.6,"publicationDate":"2022-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC9561006/pdf/nihms-1825634.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9295264","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
How do real animals account for the passage of time during associative learning? 在联想学习过程中,真实的动物是如何解释时间的流逝的?
IF 1.6 4区 医学 Q3 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2022-10-01 Epub Date: 2022-04-28 DOI: 10.1037/bne0000516
Vijay Mohan K Namboodiri

Animals routinely learn to associate environmental stimuli and self-generated actions with their outcomes such as rewards. One of the most popular theoretical models of such learning is the reinforcement learning (RL) framework. The simplest form of RL, model-free RL, is widely applied to explain animal behavior in numerous neuroscientific studies. More complex RL versions assume that animals build and store an explicit model of the world in memory. To apply these approaches to explain animal behavior, typical neuroscientific RL models make implicit assumptions about how real animals represent the passage of time. In this perspective, I explicitly list these assumptions and show that they have several problematic implications. I hope that the explicit discussion of these problems encourages the field to seriously examine the assumptions underlying timing and reinforcement learning. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).

动物通常会学会将环境刺激和自我产生的行为与其结果(如奖励)联系起来。强化学习(RL)框架是这种学习最流行的理论模型之一。RL的最简单形式,无模型RL,在许多神经科学研究中被广泛应用于解释动物行为。更复杂的RL版本假设动物在记忆中构建并存储一个明确的世界模型。为了应用这些方法来解释动物的行为,典型的神经科学RL模型对真实动物如何代表时间的流逝做出了隐含的假设。从这个角度来看,我明确列出了这些假设,并表明它们有几个问题。我希望对这些问题的明确讨论能鼓励该领域认真研究时间和强化学习的基本假设。(PsycInfo数据库记录(c)2022 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"How do real animals account for the passage of time during associative learning?","authors":"Vijay Mohan K Namboodiri","doi":"10.1037/bne0000516","DOIUrl":"10.1037/bne0000516","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Animals routinely learn to associate environmental stimuli and self-generated actions with their outcomes such as rewards. One of the most popular theoretical models of such learning is the reinforcement learning (RL) framework. The simplest form of RL, model-free RL, is widely applied to explain animal behavior in numerous neuroscientific studies. More complex RL versions assume that animals build and store an explicit model of the world in memory. To apply these approaches to explain animal behavior, typical neuroscientific RL models make implicit assumptions about how real animals represent the passage of time. In this perspective, I explicitly list these assumptions and show that they have several problematic implications. I hope that the explicit discussion of these problems encourages the field to seriously examine the assumptions underlying timing and reinforcement learning. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":8739,"journal":{"name":"Behavioral neuroscience","volume":"136 5","pages":"383-391"},"PeriodicalIF":1.6,"publicationDate":"2022-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC9561011/pdf/nihms-1825994.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41101858","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Behavioral neuroscience
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1