首页 > 最新文献

Behavioral Ecology最新文献

英文 中文
Mating preferences act independently on different elements of visual signals in Heliconius butterflies 交配偏好独立作用于 Heliconius 蝴蝶视觉信号的不同元素
IF 2.4 3区 环境科学与生态学 Q2 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-07-13 DOI: 10.1093/beheco/arae056
Sophie Helen Smith, Lucie M Queste, Daniel Shane Wright, Caroline Nicole Bacquet, Richard M Merrill
Mating cues are often comprised of several elements, which can act independently, or in concert to attract a suitable partner. Individual elements may also function in other contexts, such as anti-predator defense or camouflage. In Heliconius butterflies, wing patterns comprise several individual color pattern elements, which advertise the butterflies’ toxicity to predators. These wing patterns are also mating cues, and males predominantly court females that possess the same wing pattern as their own. However, it is not known whether male preference is based on the full wing pattern or only individual pattern elements. We compared preferences of male H. erato lativitta between female models with the full wing pattern and those with some pattern elements removed. We found no differences in preference between the full wing pattern model and a model with pattern elements removed, indicating that the complete composition of all elements is not essential to the mating signal. Wing pattern preferences also contribute to pre-mating isolation between two other Heliconius taxa, H. erato cyrbia and H. himera, therefore, we next compared preferences for the same models in these species. H. erato cyrbia and H. himera strongly differed in preferences for the models, potentially providing a mechanism for how pre-mating isolation acts between these species. These findings suggest that contrasting levels of selective constraint act on elements across the wing pattern
交配线索通常由多个要素组成,这些要素可以独立作用,也可以协同作用,以吸引合适的伴侣。单个元素还可能在其他情况下发挥作用,如抵御捕食者或伪装。在 Heliconius 蝴蝶中,翅膀图案由多个单独的颜色图案元素组成,向捕食者宣传蝴蝶的毒性。这些翅纹也是交配线索,雄蝶主要追求与自己翅纹相同的雌蝶。然而,目前还不知道雄性的偏好是基于整个翅膀图案还是仅基于个别图案元素。我们比较了雄性 H. erato lativitta 对具有完整翅膀花纹的雌性模型和去除部分花纹元素的雌性模型的偏好。我们发现,完整翅膀图案模型与去掉图案元素的模型在偏好上没有差异,这表明所有元素的完整构成对交配信号并不重要。翼型偏好也有助于其他两个榛鸡类群(H. erato cyrbia和H. himera)之间的交配前隔离,因此,我们接下来比较了这两个物种对相同模式的偏好。H. erato cyrbia和H. himera对模型的偏好存在很大差异,这可能为这些物种之间的交配前隔离提供了一种机制。这些发现表明,不同程度的选择性限制作用于整个翅型的要素上
{"title":"Mating preferences act independently on different elements of visual signals in Heliconius butterflies","authors":"Sophie Helen Smith, Lucie M Queste, Daniel Shane Wright, Caroline Nicole Bacquet, Richard M Merrill","doi":"10.1093/beheco/arae056","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/arae056","url":null,"abstract":"Mating cues are often comprised of several elements, which can act independently, or in concert to attract a suitable partner. Individual elements may also function in other contexts, such as anti-predator defense or camouflage. In Heliconius butterflies, wing patterns comprise several individual color pattern elements, which advertise the butterflies’ toxicity to predators. These wing patterns are also mating cues, and males predominantly court females that possess the same wing pattern as their own. However, it is not known whether male preference is based on the full wing pattern or only individual pattern elements. We compared preferences of male H. erato lativitta between female models with the full wing pattern and those with some pattern elements removed. We found no differences in preference between the full wing pattern model and a model with pattern elements removed, indicating that the complete composition of all elements is not essential to the mating signal. Wing pattern preferences also contribute to pre-mating isolation between two other Heliconius taxa, H. erato cyrbia and H. himera, therefore, we next compared preferences for the same models in these species. H. erato cyrbia and H. himera strongly differed in preferences for the models, potentially providing a mechanism for how pre-mating isolation acts between these species. These findings suggest that contrasting levels of selective constraint act on elements across the wing pattern","PeriodicalId":8840,"journal":{"name":"Behavioral Ecology","volume":"4 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.4,"publicationDate":"2024-07-13","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141718637","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"环境科学与生态学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
How does viewing angle affect the perceived accuracy of Batesian mimicry in hoverflies? 观察角度如何影响食蚜蝇贝氏拟态的准确性?
IF 2.4 3区 环境科学与生态学 Q2 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-07-04 DOI: 10.1093/beheco/arae054
Lucy Baker, Chris Taylor, Francis Gilbert, Tom Reader
Despite Batesian mimicry often eliciting predator avoidance, many Batesian mimics, such as some species of hoverfly (Syrphidae), are considered to have an “imperfect” resemblance to their model. One possible explanation for the persistence of apparently imperfect mimicry is that human perceptions of mimicry are different from those of natural predators. Natural predators of hoverflies have different visual and cognitive systems from humans, and they may encounter mimics in a different way. For example, whilst humans often encounter hoverflies at rest on vegetation, or in photographs or textbooks, where they are typically viewed from above, natural predators may approach hoverflies from the side or below. To test how viewing angle affects the perception of mimicry, images of mimetic hoverflies and their models (wasps and bees) were shown from different angles in an online survey. Participants were asked to distinguish between the images of models and mimics. The results show that the viewing angle does affect perceived mimicry in some species, although it does not provide a complete explanation for the persistence of imperfect mimicry in nature. The effect is also highly species-specific. This suggests that to understand better how selection has shaped mimetic accuracy in hoverflies and other taxa, further study is required of the viewing angles that predators utilize most commonly in nature.
尽管贝茨拟态常常会引起捕食者的回避,但许多贝茨拟态,如某些种类的食蚜蝇(Syrphidae),被认为与其拟态有 "不完美 "的相似之处。表面上看似不完美的拟态之所以持续存在,一个可能的解释是人类对拟态的看法与天敌的看法不同。食蚜蝇的天敌拥有与人类不同的视觉和认知系统,它们可能会以不同的方式遇到拟态。例如,人类通常会在植被上、照片或教科书中遇到静止的食蚜蝇,它们通常是从上方观看的,而天敌可能会从侧面或下方接近食蚜蝇。为了测试观察角度如何影响对拟态的感知,我们在一项在线调查中从不同角度展示了拟态食蚜蝇及其模型(黄蜂和蜜蜂)的图像。参与者被要求区分模型和拟态的图像。结果表明,在某些物种中,观看角度确实会影响感知到的拟态,但这并不能完全解释自然界中持续存在的不完全拟态。这种影响还具有高度的物种特异性。这表明,要想更好地理解选择是如何塑造食蚜蝇和其他类群的拟态准确性的,还需要进一步研究捕食者在自然界中最常使用的观察角度。
{"title":"How does viewing angle affect the perceived accuracy of Batesian mimicry in hoverflies?","authors":"Lucy Baker, Chris Taylor, Francis Gilbert, Tom Reader","doi":"10.1093/beheco/arae054","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/arae054","url":null,"abstract":"Despite Batesian mimicry often eliciting predator avoidance, many Batesian mimics, such as some species of hoverfly (Syrphidae), are considered to have an “imperfect” resemblance to their model. One possible explanation for the persistence of apparently imperfect mimicry is that human perceptions of mimicry are different from those of natural predators. Natural predators of hoverflies have different visual and cognitive systems from humans, and they may encounter mimics in a different way. For example, whilst humans often encounter hoverflies at rest on vegetation, or in photographs or textbooks, where they are typically viewed from above, natural predators may approach hoverflies from the side or below. To test how viewing angle affects the perception of mimicry, images of mimetic hoverflies and their models (wasps and bees) were shown from different angles in an online survey. Participants were asked to distinguish between the images of models and mimics. The results show that the viewing angle does affect perceived mimicry in some species, although it does not provide a complete explanation for the persistence of imperfect mimicry in nature. The effect is also highly species-specific. This suggests that to understand better how selection has shaped mimetic accuracy in hoverflies and other taxa, further study is required of the viewing angles that predators utilize most commonly in nature.","PeriodicalId":8840,"journal":{"name":"Behavioral Ecology","volume":"6 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.4,"publicationDate":"2024-07-04","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141547151","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"环境科学与生态学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Influence of visual information on sniffing behavior in a routinely trichromatic primate 视觉信息对常规三色灵长类动物嗅闻行为的影响
IF 2.4 3区 环境科学与生态学 Q2 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-07-04 DOI: 10.1093/beheco/arae055
Brigitte M Weiß, Anja Widdig
Most catarrhine primates are considered to be strongly visually oriented, obtaining information about conspecifics and their environment from a diversity of visual cues. Other sensory modalities may provide information that is redundant and/or complimentary to visual cues. When cues from multiple sensory modalities are available, these may reinforce or suppress each other, as shown in several taxa ranging from insects to humans. Here, we tested how the presence and ambiguity of visual information affects the use of olfactory cues when exploring food and non-food items in semi-free ranging Barbary macaques at Affenberg Salem, Germany. We presented monkeys with pipes containing food (peanuts, popcorn), non-food (stones, feces) or no items in transparent or opaque containers, and assessed whether animals looked, sniffed and/or grabbed into the pipes depending on visibility of the contents (experiment 1). Visual information had no robust effect on sniffing probability, but monkeys were more likely to sniff before any other form of inspection if the can was opaque than if it was transparent. Both visual and olfactory information affected, whether or not monkeys attempted to retrieve the items from the pipes, whereby monkeys showed an overall decrease in the propensity to grab after sniffing. Furthermore, we manipulated the visual appearance of familiar food items (popcorn) with food colorant (experiment 2), which resulted in substantially increased olfactory inspections compared to unmanipulated popcorn. Taken together, reliance on the olfactory sense was modulated by the available visual information, emphasizing the interplay between different sensory modalities for obtaining information about the environment.
大多数猫科灵长类动物被认为具有强烈的视觉导向性,它们从多种视觉线索中获取关于同类和环境的信息。其他感觉模式可能会提供多余的信息和/或补充视觉线索的信息。正如从昆虫到人类的多个类群所显示的那样,当有多种感觉模式的线索时,这些线索可能会相互加强或相互抑制。在这里,我们测试了在德国阿芬贝格萨勒姆,半自由活动的巴巴利猕猴在探索食物和非食物时,视觉信息的存在和模糊性如何影响嗅觉线索的使用。我们向猕猴展示了装有食物(花生、爆米花)、非食物(石头、粪便)或不装任何物品的透明或不透明容器,并评估了猕猴是否会根据管内物品的可见度来观察、嗅闻和/或抓取管内物品(实验 1)。视觉信息对嗅觉概率没有明显影响,但如果罐子是不透明的,猴子更有可能先嗅后看。视觉和嗅觉信息都会影响猴子是否试图从管道中拾取物品,因此猴子在嗅闻后抓取物品的倾向性总体上有所下降。此外,我们用食物着色剂操纵了熟悉的食物(爆米花)的视觉外观(实验 2),结果与未操纵的爆米花相比,嗅觉检查大大增加。总之,对嗅觉的依赖受可用视觉信息的调节,这强调了不同感官模式在获取环境信息方面的相互作用。
{"title":"Influence of visual information on sniffing behavior in a routinely trichromatic primate","authors":"Brigitte M Weiß, Anja Widdig","doi":"10.1093/beheco/arae055","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/arae055","url":null,"abstract":"Most catarrhine primates are considered to be strongly visually oriented, obtaining information about conspecifics and their environment from a diversity of visual cues. Other sensory modalities may provide information that is redundant and/or complimentary to visual cues. When cues from multiple sensory modalities are available, these may reinforce or suppress each other, as shown in several taxa ranging from insects to humans. Here, we tested how the presence and ambiguity of visual information affects the use of olfactory cues when exploring food and non-food items in semi-free ranging Barbary macaques at Affenberg Salem, Germany. We presented monkeys with pipes containing food (peanuts, popcorn), non-food (stones, feces) or no items in transparent or opaque containers, and assessed whether animals looked, sniffed and/or grabbed into the pipes depending on visibility of the contents (experiment 1). Visual information had no robust effect on sniffing probability, but monkeys were more likely to sniff before any other form of inspection if the can was opaque than if it was transparent. Both visual and olfactory information affected, whether or not monkeys attempted to retrieve the items from the pipes, whereby monkeys showed an overall decrease in the propensity to grab after sniffing. Furthermore, we manipulated the visual appearance of familiar food items (popcorn) with food colorant (experiment 2), which resulted in substantially increased olfactory inspections compared to unmanipulated popcorn. Taken together, reliance on the olfactory sense was modulated by the available visual information, emphasizing the interplay between different sensory modalities for obtaining information about the environment.","PeriodicalId":8840,"journal":{"name":"Behavioral Ecology","volume":"25 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.4,"publicationDate":"2024-07-04","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141552557","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"环境科学与生态学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Highly defended nudibranchs ‘escape’ to visually distinct background habitats 高度防卫的裸鳃鱼 "逃 "到视觉上独特的背景栖息地
IF 2.4 3区 环境科学与生态学 Q2 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-07-03 DOI: 10.1093/beheco/arae053
Cedric P van den Berg, Matteo Santon, John A Endler, Karen L Cheney
The ‘escape and radiate’ hypothesis predicts that once species have evolved aposematism, defended species can utilise more visually diverse visual backgrounds as they ‘escape’ the need to be well camouflaged. This enables species to explore new ecological niches, resulting in increased diversification rates. To test this hypothesis’ ‘escape’ component, we examined whether the background habitats of 12 nudibranch mollusc species differed among species depending on the presence and strength of chemical defences. We obtained a rich array of colour pattern statistics using Quantitative Colour Pattern Analysis (QCPA) to analyse backgrounds viewed through the eyes of a potential predator (triggerfish, Rhinecanthus aculeatus). Colour pattern analysis was done at viewing distances simulating an escalating predation sequence. We identified four latent factors comprising 17 non-correlated colour pattern parameters, which captured the among-species variability associated with differences in chemical defences. We found that chemically defended species, indeed, were found on visually distinct backgrounds with increased colour and luminance contrast, independent of viewing distance. However, we found no evidence for increased among-species background diversity coinciding with the presence and strength of chemical defences. Our results agree with the ‘escape and radiate’ hypothesis, suggesting that potent chemical defences in Dorid nudibranchs coincide with spatiochromatic differences of visual background habitats perceived by potential predators.
逃逸和辐射 "假说预言,一旦物种进化出 "隐形 "能力,防御物种就能利用视觉上更加多样的视觉背景,因为它们 "逃逸 "了伪装的需要。这使物种能够探索新的生态位,从而提高物种多样化率。为了验证这一假说的 "逃避 "部分,我们研究了 12 个裸鳃软体动物物种的背景栖息地是否因化学防御的存在和强度而有所不同。我们利用定量色彩模式分析(QCPA)获得了丰富的色彩模式统计数据,以分析通过潜在捕食者(鲀,Rhinecanthus aculeatus)的眼睛观察到的背景。色彩模式分析是在模拟不断升级的捕食序列的观察距离下进行的。我们确定了由 17 个非相关颜色模式参数组成的四个潜在因子,它们捕捉到了与化学防御差异相关的物种间变异。我们发现,有化学防御能力的物种确实是在视觉上不同的背景上被发现的,其颜色和亮度对比增加,与观察距离无关。但是,我们没有发现任何证据表明物种间背景多样性的增加与化学防御的存在和强度有关。我们的研究结果与 "逃逸和辐射 "假说一致,表明多丽德海裸鳃动物的强化学防御与潜在捕食者感知到的视觉背景生境的空间色度差异相吻合。
{"title":"Highly defended nudibranchs ‘escape’ to visually distinct background habitats","authors":"Cedric P van den Berg, Matteo Santon, John A Endler, Karen L Cheney","doi":"10.1093/beheco/arae053","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/arae053","url":null,"abstract":"The ‘escape and radiate’ hypothesis predicts that once species have evolved aposematism, defended species can utilise more visually diverse visual backgrounds as they ‘escape’ the need to be well camouflaged. This enables species to explore new ecological niches, resulting in increased diversification rates. To test this hypothesis’ ‘escape’ component, we examined whether the background habitats of 12 nudibranch mollusc species differed among species depending on the presence and strength of chemical defences. We obtained a rich array of colour pattern statistics using Quantitative Colour Pattern Analysis (QCPA) to analyse backgrounds viewed through the eyes of a potential predator (triggerfish, Rhinecanthus aculeatus). Colour pattern analysis was done at viewing distances simulating an escalating predation sequence. We identified four latent factors comprising 17 non-correlated colour pattern parameters, which captured the among-species variability associated with differences in chemical defences. We found that chemically defended species, indeed, were found on visually distinct backgrounds with increased colour and luminance contrast, independent of viewing distance. However, we found no evidence for increased among-species background diversity coinciding with the presence and strength of chemical defences. Our results agree with the ‘escape and radiate’ hypothesis, suggesting that potent chemical defences in Dorid nudibranchs coincide with spatiochromatic differences of visual background habitats perceived by potential predators.","PeriodicalId":8840,"journal":{"name":"Behavioral Ecology","volume":"39 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.4,"publicationDate":"2024-07-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141547105","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"环境科学与生态学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Effects of maternal age and environmental enrichment on learning ability and brain size. 母体年龄和丰富环境对学习能力和大脑大小的影响
IF 2.5 3区 环境科学与生态学 Q2 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-06-17 eCollection Date: 2024-07-01 DOI: 10.1093/beheco/arae049
Náyade Álvarez-Quintero, Sin-Yeon Kim

It is well known that maternal age at reproduction affects offspring lifespan and some other fitness-related traits, but it remains understudied whether maternal senescence affects how offspring respond to their environments. Early environment often plays a significant role in the development of an animal's behavioral phenotype. For example, complex environments can promote changes in cognitive ability and brain morphology in young animals. Here, we study whether and how maternal effect senescence influences offspring plasticity in cognition, group behavior, and brain morphology in response to environmental complexity. For this, juvenile 3-spined sticklebacks from young and old mothers (i.e. 1-yr and 2-yr-old) were exposed to different levels of environmental enrichment and complexity (i.e. none, simple, and complex), and their behavior, cognitive ability, and brain size were measured. Exposing fish to enriched conditions improved individual learning ability assessed by a repeated detour-reaching task, increased the size of the whole brain, and decreased aggressive interactions in the shoal. Maternal age did not influence the inhibitory control, learning ability, and group behavioral responses of offspring to the experimental environmental change. However, maternal age affected how some brain regions of offspring changed in response to environmental complexity. In offspring from old mothers, those exposed to the complex environment had larger telencephalons and cerebellums than those who experienced simpler environments. Our results suggest that maternal effect senescence may influence how offspring invest in brain functions related to cognition in response to environmental complexity.

众所周知,繁殖时的母体年龄会影响后代的寿命和其他一些与体能相关的特征,但母体衰老是否会影响后代对环境的反应,目前仍未得到充分研究。早期环境往往对动物行为表型的形成起着重要作用。例如,复杂的环境会促进幼年动物认知能力和大脑形态的变化。在此,我们研究母体效应衰老是否以及如何影响后代在认知、群体行为和大脑形态方面的可塑性,以应对环境的复杂性。为此,我们将来自年轻和年老母亲(即 1 岁和 2 岁)的幼年三刺鱼暴露于不同程度的环境富集和复杂环境(即无、简单和复杂)中,并测量它们的行为、认知能力和脑部大小。将鱼暴露在丰富的环境中可以提高个体的学习能力(通过重复脱离-伸展任务进行评估),增加整个大脑的大小,并减少鱼群中的攻击性互动。母体年龄并不影响后代对实验环境变化的抑制控制、学习能力和群体行为反应。但是,母体年龄会影响后代某些脑区对环境复杂性的反应。在来自高龄母亲的后代中,那些暴露于复杂环境的后代的端脑和小脑比那些经历过简单环境的后代大。我们的研究结果表明,母体效应衰老可能会影响后代如何投资于与认知有关的大脑功能,以应对环境的复杂性。
{"title":"Effects of maternal age and environmental enrichment on learning ability and brain size.","authors":"Náyade Álvarez-Quintero, Sin-Yeon Kim","doi":"10.1093/beheco/arae049","DOIUrl":"10.1093/beheco/arae049","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>It is well known that maternal age at reproduction affects offspring lifespan and some other fitness-related traits, but it remains understudied whether maternal senescence affects how offspring respond to their environments. Early environment often plays a significant role in the development of an animal's behavioral phenotype. For example, complex environments can promote changes in cognitive ability and brain morphology in young animals. Here, we study whether and how maternal effect senescence influences offspring plasticity in cognition, group behavior, and brain morphology in response to environmental complexity. For this, juvenile 3-spined sticklebacks from young and old mothers (i.e. 1-yr and 2-yr-old) were exposed to different levels of environmental enrichment and complexity (i.e. none, simple, and complex), and their behavior, cognitive ability, and brain size were measured. Exposing fish to enriched conditions improved individual learning ability assessed by a repeated detour-reaching task, increased the size of the whole brain, and decreased aggressive interactions in the shoal. Maternal age did not influence the inhibitory control, learning ability, and group behavioral responses of offspring to the experimental environmental change. However, maternal age affected how some brain regions of offspring changed in response to environmental complexity. In offspring from old mothers, those exposed to the complex environment had larger telencephalons and cerebellums than those who experienced simpler environments. Our results suggest that maternal effect senescence may influence how offspring invest in brain functions related to cognition in response to environmental complexity.</p>","PeriodicalId":8840,"journal":{"name":"Behavioral Ecology","volume":"35 4","pages":"arae049"},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2024-06-17","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11215699/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141475817","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"环境科学与生态学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Archerfish foraging success varies with immediate competition level but not group size. 箭鱼的觅食成功率随直接竞争水平而变化,但与群体大小无关。
IF 2.5 3区 环境科学与生态学 Q2 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-05-21 eCollection Date: 2024-07-01 DOI: 10.1093/beheco/arae040
Dagmar der Weduwen, Nick A R Jones, Adèle Dubosque, Stefan Schuster, Keith T Sillar, Mike Webster, Luke Rendell

Group living can lead to kleptoparasitism, the theft of resources by competitors. Under such conditions, foragers may alter their behavior to minimize competition. However, it is unclear how such behavioral changes impact foraging performance. Archerfish (Toxotes spp.) are a good model for investigating the behavioral responses to kleptoparasitism, as their hunting method (shooting waterjets at insects perched above the water) leaves them vulnerable to theft. They must hit the target prey with sufficient force to dislodge it; thus, the prey may land some distance away from the shooter. Kleptoparasitism rates increase with group size in archerfish, and individuals alter their behavior around conspecifics. We investigated whether group size affected shooting success, using 7-spot archerfish T. chatareus. We considered a fish's shot to be successful if it knocked a fly, placed on a transparent platform above the tank, into the water. The probability of shooting success was modeled as a function of group size, aiming duration, nearest neighbor distance and position, and trial number. We found no effect of group size, aiming duration, or nearest neighbor distance or position on shooting success. Shooting success increased as trials progressed, likely due to the fish becoming more familiar with the task. We also found no change in the kleptoparasitism rate between group sizes. Instead, the likelihood of the shooter consuming the prey depended on the types of competition present at the time of shooting. We suggest that archerfish shooting behavior can be influenced by the presence of conspecifics in ways not previously considered.

群居生活可能导致偷窃寄生,即竞争对手偷窃资源。在这种情况下,觅食者可能会改变行为,以尽量减少竞争。然而,目前还不清楚这种行为变化如何影响觅食表现。箭鱼(Toxotes spp.)是研究偷窃寄生行为反应的一个很好的模型,因为它们的狩猎方法(向栖息在水面上的昆虫喷射水柱)使它们很容易被偷窃。它们必须以足够的力量击中目标猎物才能使其脱落;因此,猎物可能会落到离射手有一段距离的地方。箭鱼的爬行寄生率随群体大小而增加,个体在同类周围的行为也会改变。我们使用 7 点箭鱼 T. chatareus 研究了群体大小是否会影响射杀成功率。如果一条鱼将放在鱼缸上方透明平台上的苍蝇击落水中,我们就认为它的射击成功了。射击成功的概率被模拟为群体大小、瞄准持续时间、最近邻距离和位置以及试验次数的函数。我们发现,群体大小、瞄准持续时间、近邻距离或位置对射击成功率没有影响。随着试验次数的增加,射击成功率也在增加,这可能是由于鱼对任务越来越熟悉。我们还发现,不同大小的群体之间的偷食率也没有变化。相反,射手吃掉猎物的可能性取决于射箭时的竞争类型。我们认为,箭鱼的射击行为会受到同种鱼的影响,而这种影响是以前从未考虑过的。
{"title":"Archerfish foraging success varies with immediate competition level but not group size.","authors":"Dagmar der Weduwen, Nick A R Jones, Adèle Dubosque, Stefan Schuster, Keith T Sillar, Mike Webster, Luke Rendell","doi":"10.1093/beheco/arae040","DOIUrl":"10.1093/beheco/arae040","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Group living can lead to kleptoparasitism, the theft of resources by competitors. Under such conditions, foragers may alter their behavior to minimize competition. However, it is unclear how such behavioral changes impact foraging performance. Archerfish (<i>Toxotes</i> spp.) are a good model for investigating the behavioral responses to kleptoparasitism, as their hunting method (shooting waterjets at insects perched above the water) leaves them vulnerable to theft. They must hit the target prey with sufficient force to dislodge it; thus, the prey may land some distance away from the shooter. Kleptoparasitism rates increase with group size in archerfish, and individuals alter their behavior around conspecifics. We investigated whether group size affected shooting success, using 7-spot archerfish <i>T. chatareus</i>. We considered a fish's shot to be successful if it knocked a fly, placed on a transparent platform above the tank, into the water. The probability of shooting success was modeled as a function of group size, aiming duration, nearest neighbor distance and position, and trial number. We found no effect of group size, aiming duration, or nearest neighbor distance or position on shooting success. Shooting success increased as trials progressed, likely due to the fish becoming more familiar with the task. We also found no change in the kleptoparasitism rate between group sizes. Instead, the likelihood of the shooter consuming the prey depended on the types of competition present at the time of shooting. We suggest that archerfish shooting behavior can be influenced by the presence of conspecifics in ways not previously considered.</p>","PeriodicalId":8840,"journal":{"name":"Behavioral Ecology","volume":"35 4","pages":"arae040"},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2024-05-21","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11134209/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141174052","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"环境科学与生态学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Scope and adaptive value of modulating aggression over breeding stages in a competitive female bird 竞争性雌鸟在繁殖阶段调节攻击性的范围和适应价值
IF 2.4 3区 环境科学与生态学 Q2 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-05-20 DOI: 10.1093/beheco/arae042
Elizabeth M George, Abigail M Weber, Kimberly A Rosvall
In seasonally breeding animals, costs and benefits of territorial aggression should vary over time; however, little work thus far has directly examined the scope and adaptive value of individual-level plasticity in aggression across breeding stages. We explore these issues using the tree swallow (Tachycineta bicolor), a single-brooded bird species in which females compete for limited nesting sites. We measured aggressiveness in nearly 100 females within three different stages: (1) shortly after territory-establishment, (2) during incubation, and (3) while caring for young chicks. Based on the timing, direction, and magnitude of behavioral changes between stages, we used k-means clustering to categorize each female’s behavior into a ‘plasticity type’. We then tested whether plasticity type and stage-specific aggression varied with key performance metrics. About 40% of females decreased aggressiveness across consecutive breeding stages to some degree, consistent with population-level patterns. 33% of females exhibited comparatively little plasticity, with moderate to low levels of aggression in all stages. Finally, 27% of females displayed steep decreases and then increases in aggression between stages; females exhibiting this pattern had significantly lower body mass while parenting, they tended to hatch fewer eggs, and they had the lowest observed overwinter survival rates. Other patterns of among-stage changes in aggressiveness were not associated with performance. These results reveal substantial among-individual variation in behavioral plasticity, which may reflect diverse solutions to trade-offs between current reproduction and future survival.
在季节性繁殖的动物中,领地攻击的成本和收益应该随着时间的推移而变化;然而,迄今为止,很少有研究直接考察了不同繁殖阶段攻击的个体水平可塑性的范围和适应价值。我们利用树燕(Tachycineta bicolor)探讨了这些问题,树燕是一种单编码鸟类,雌鸟会争夺有限的筑巢地点。我们测量了近 100 只雌鸟在三个不同阶段的攻击性:(1)领地建立后不久;(2)孵化期间;(3)照顾幼雏期间。根据不同阶段行为变化的时间、方向和程度,我们使用k-均值聚类方法将每只雌蛙的行为划分为 "可塑性类型"。然后,我们测试了可塑性类型和特定阶段的攻击性是否随关键性能指标的变化而变化。大约40%的雌性在不同繁殖阶段的攻击性都有一定程度的下降,这与种群水平的模式一致。33%的雌性表现出相对较小的可塑性,在所有阶段都具有中等至较低的攻击性。最后,27%的雌性在不同阶段的攻击性急剧下降,然后又急剧上升;表现出这种模式的雌性在育儿期间的体重明显较低,孵化出的卵也往往较少,观察到的越冬存活率也最低。阶段间攻击性的其他变化模式与表现无关。这些结果揭示了行为可塑性在个体间的巨大差异,这可能反映了在当前繁殖和未来生存之间权衡的不同解决方案。
{"title":"Scope and adaptive value of modulating aggression over breeding stages in a competitive female bird","authors":"Elizabeth M George, Abigail M Weber, Kimberly A Rosvall","doi":"10.1093/beheco/arae042","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/arae042","url":null,"abstract":"In seasonally breeding animals, costs and benefits of territorial aggression should vary over time; however, little work thus far has directly examined the scope and adaptive value of individual-level plasticity in aggression across breeding stages. We explore these issues using the tree swallow (Tachycineta bicolor), a single-brooded bird species in which females compete for limited nesting sites. We measured aggressiveness in nearly 100 females within three different stages: (1) shortly after territory-establishment, (2) during incubation, and (3) while caring for young chicks. Based on the timing, direction, and magnitude of behavioral changes between stages, we used k-means clustering to categorize each female’s behavior into a ‘plasticity type’. We then tested whether plasticity type and stage-specific aggression varied with key performance metrics. About 40% of females decreased aggressiveness across consecutive breeding stages to some degree, consistent with population-level patterns. 33% of females exhibited comparatively little plasticity, with moderate to low levels of aggression in all stages. Finally, 27% of females displayed steep decreases and then increases in aggression between stages; females exhibiting this pattern had significantly lower body mass while parenting, they tended to hatch fewer eggs, and they had the lowest observed overwinter survival rates. Other patterns of among-stage changes in aggressiveness were not associated with performance. These results reveal substantial among-individual variation in behavioral plasticity, which may reflect diverse solutions to trade-offs between current reproduction and future survival.","PeriodicalId":8840,"journal":{"name":"Behavioral Ecology","volume":"46 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.4,"publicationDate":"2024-05-20","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141152580","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"环境科学与生态学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Relative telencephalon size does not affect collective motion in the guppy (Poecilia reticulata). 端脑的相对大小不会影响虹鳟(Poecilia reticulata)的集体运动。
IF 2.5 3区 环境科学与生态学 Q2 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-05-03 eCollection Date: 2024-07-01 DOI: 10.1093/beheco/arae033
Annika Boussard, Mikaela Ahlkvist, Alberto Corral-López, Stephanie Fong, John Fitzpatrick, Niclas Kolm

Collective motion is common across all animal taxa, from swarming insects to schools of fish. The collective motion requires intricate behavioral integration among individuals, yet little is known about how evolutionary changes in brain morphology influence the ability for individuals to coordinate behavior in groups. In this study, we utilized guppies that were selectively bred for relative telencephalon size, an aspect of brain morphology that is normally associated with advanced cognitive functions, to examine its role in collective motion using an open-field assay. We analyzed high-resolution tracking data of same-sex shoals consisting of 8 individuals to assess different aspects of collective motion, such as alignment, attraction to nearby shoal members, and swimming speed. Our findings indicate that variation in collective motion in guppy shoals might not be strongly affected by variation in relative telencephalon size. Our study suggests that group dynamics in collectively moving animals are likely not driven by advanced cognitive functions but rather by fundamental cognitive processes stemming from relatively simple rules among neighboring individuals.

从成群的昆虫到成群的鱼类,集体运动在所有动物类群中都很常见。集体运动需要个体间错综复杂的行为整合,但人们对大脑形态的进化变化如何影响个体在群体中协调行为的能力知之甚少。在这项研究中,我们利用针对相对端脑大小(大脑形态的一个方面通常与高级认知功能相关)进行选择性繁殖的河豚鼠,使用开放场试验来研究其在集体运动中的作用。我们分析了由 8 个个体组成的同性鱼群的高分辨率跟踪数据,以评估集体运动的不同方面,如排列、对附近鱼群成员的吸引力和游泳速度。我们的研究结果表明,河豚群中集体运动的变化可能不会受到端脑相对大小变化的强烈影响。我们的研究表明,集体运动动物的群体动力学很可能不是由高级认知功能驱动的,而是由邻近个体之间相对简单的规则所产生的基本认知过程驱动的。
{"title":"Relative telencephalon size does not affect collective motion in the guppy (<i>Poecilia reticulata</i>).","authors":"Annika Boussard, Mikaela Ahlkvist, Alberto Corral-López, Stephanie Fong, John Fitzpatrick, Niclas Kolm","doi":"10.1093/beheco/arae033","DOIUrl":"10.1093/beheco/arae033","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Collective motion is common across all animal taxa, from swarming insects to schools of fish. The collective motion requires intricate behavioral integration among individuals, yet little is known about how evolutionary changes in brain morphology influence the ability for individuals to coordinate behavior in groups. In this study, we utilized guppies that were selectively bred for relative telencephalon size, an aspect of brain morphology that is normally associated with advanced cognitive functions, to examine its role in collective motion using an open-field assay. We analyzed high-resolution tracking data of same-sex shoals consisting of 8 individuals to assess different aspects of collective motion, such as alignment, attraction to nearby shoal members, and swimming speed. Our findings indicate that variation in collective motion in guppy shoals might not be strongly affected by variation in relative telencephalon size. Our study suggests that group dynamics in collectively moving animals are likely not driven by advanced cognitive functions but rather by fundamental cognitive processes stemming from relatively simple rules among neighboring individuals.</p>","PeriodicalId":8840,"journal":{"name":"Behavioral Ecology","volume":"35 4","pages":"arae033"},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2024-05-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11110457/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141080437","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"环境科学与生态学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Does losing reduce the tendency to engage with rivals to reach mates? An experimental test 输掉比赛会降低与对手交配的倾向吗?实验测试
IF 2.4 3区 环境科学与生态学 Q2 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-05-01 DOI: 10.1093/beheco/arae037
Chenke Zang, Meng-Han Joseph Chung, Teresa Neeman, Lauren Harrison, Ivan M Vinogradov, Michael D Jennions
Male-male contests for access to females or breeding resources is critical in determining male reproductive success. Larger males and those with more effective weaponry are more likely to win fights. However, even after controlling for such predictors of fighting ability, studies have reported a winner-loser effect: previous winners are more likely to win subsequent contests, while losers often suffer repeated defeats. While the effect of winning-losing is well-documented for the outcome of future fights, its effect on other behaviors (e.g., mating) remains poorly investigated. Here, we test whether a winning versus losing experience influenced subsequent behaviors of male mosquitofish (Gambusia holbrooki) towards rivals and potential mates. We housed focal males with either a smaller or larger opponent for 24 hours to manipulate their fighting experience to become winners or losers, respectively. The focal males then underwent tests that required them to enter and swim through a narrow corridor to reach females, bypassing a cylinder that contained either a larger rival male (competitive scenario), a juvenile or was empty (non-competitive scenarios). The tests were repeated after one week. Winners were more likely to leave the start area and to reach the females, but only when a larger rival was presented, indicating higher levels of risk-taking behavior in aggressive interactions. This winner-loser effect persisted for at least one week. We suggest that male mosquitofish adjust their assessment of their own and/or their rival’s fighting ability following contests in ways whose detection by researchers depends on the social context.
雄性与雄性之间为获得雌性或繁殖资源而进行的争斗是决定雄性繁殖成功与否的关键。体型较大的雄性和拥有更有效武器的雄性更有可能在争斗中获胜。然而,即使在控制了这些预测战斗能力的因素后,仍有研究报告称存在输赢效应:以前的赢家更有可能在以后的竞争中获胜,而输家往往屡战屡败。虽然胜负效应对未来战斗结果的影响已得到充分证实,但其对其他行为(如交配)的影响仍鲜有研究。在这里,我们测试了输赢经历是否会影响雄性蚊子鱼(Gambusia holbrooki)对竞争对手和潜在配偶的后续行为。我们将焦点雄性蚊鱼与体型较小或较大的对手饲养24小时,操纵它们的搏斗经历,使其分别成为赢家或输家。然后,这些雄性病灶鱼接受测试,要求它们进入并游过一条狭窄的走廊,绕过一个装有较大的雄性对手(竞争情景)、幼鱼或空筒(非竞争情景)的圆筒,到达雌鱼身边。一周后重复测试。获胜者更有可能离开起始区并到达雌性身边,但只有当出现较大的对手时才会如此,这表明在攻击性互动中,获胜者的冒险行为水平更高。这种输赢效应至少持续了一周。我们认为,雄性蚊子鱼在比赛后会调整对自己和/或对手战斗能力的评估,而研究人员能否发现这种调整取决于社会环境。
{"title":"Does losing reduce the tendency to engage with rivals to reach mates? An experimental test","authors":"Chenke Zang, Meng-Han Joseph Chung, Teresa Neeman, Lauren Harrison, Ivan M Vinogradov, Michael D Jennions","doi":"10.1093/beheco/arae037","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/arae037","url":null,"abstract":"Male-male contests for access to females or breeding resources is critical in determining male reproductive success. Larger males and those with more effective weaponry are more likely to win fights. However, even after controlling for such predictors of fighting ability, studies have reported a winner-loser effect: previous winners are more likely to win subsequent contests, while losers often suffer repeated defeats. While the effect of winning-losing is well-documented for the outcome of future fights, its effect on other behaviors (e.g., mating) remains poorly investigated. Here, we test whether a winning versus losing experience influenced subsequent behaviors of male mosquitofish (Gambusia holbrooki) towards rivals and potential mates. We housed focal males with either a smaller or larger opponent for 24 hours to manipulate their fighting experience to become winners or losers, respectively. The focal males then underwent tests that required them to enter and swim through a narrow corridor to reach females, bypassing a cylinder that contained either a larger rival male (competitive scenario), a juvenile or was empty (non-competitive scenarios). The tests were repeated after one week. Winners were more likely to leave the start area and to reach the females, but only when a larger rival was presented, indicating higher levels of risk-taking behavior in aggressive interactions. This winner-loser effect persisted for at least one week. We suggest that male mosquitofish adjust their assessment of their own and/or their rival’s fighting ability following contests in ways whose detection by researchers depends on the social context.","PeriodicalId":8840,"journal":{"name":"Behavioral Ecology","volume":"90 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.4,"publicationDate":"2024-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140827724","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"环境科学与生态学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Short-term heat waves have long-term consequences for parents and offspring in stickleback 短期热浪对竹节虫的亲代和子代具有长期影响
IF 2.4 3区 环境科学与生态学 Q2 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-04-27 DOI: 10.1093/beheco/arae036
Rachel Barrett, Laura R Stein
Extreme temperature events, such as heat waves, can have lasting effects on the behavior, physiology, and reproductive success of organisms. Here we examine the impact of short-term exposure to a simulated heat wave on condition, parental care, and reproductive success in a population of threespine stickleback (Gasterosteus aculeatus), a small fish with exclusive paternal care, currently experiencing regular heat waves. Males were either exposed to a simulated heat wave (23°C) for five days or held at ideal temperature (18°C). Following this five-day treatment, all males were transferred to 18°C where they completed a full parenting cycle. Offspring were raised at 18°C. We found that while mass and body condition were unaffected in males exposed to a heat wave, cortisol responses were dampened across the nesting cycle compared to control males. In addition, heat wave males had longer latency for eggs to hatch, lower hatching success, and showed lower levels of parental care behavior compared to control males. Offspring of heat wave males had lower body condition, affecting swimming performance. Altogether, our results highlight the long-term impact that even short-term events can have on reproductive success, parental behavior, and subsequent generations, providing insight into population responses to rapid environmental change.
热浪等极端温度事件会对生物的行为、生理和繁殖成功率产生持久影响。在这里,我们研究了短期暴露于模拟热浪对三刺鱼(Gasterosteus aculeatus)种群的状态、亲代照料和繁殖成功率的影响。雄鱼要么在模拟热浪(23°C)中暴露五天,要么保持在理想温度(18°C)下。经过这五天的处理后,所有雄鱼都被转移到 18°C,在那里它们完成了一个完整的育儿周期。后代在18°C的环境中长大。我们发现,虽然暴露于热浪中的雄性个体的体重和身体状况未受影响,但与对照组雄性个体相比,皮质醇反应在整个筑巢周期中受到抑制。此外,与对照组雄鸟相比,热浪雄鸟的卵孵化潜伏期更长,孵化成功率更低,并且表现出更低水平的亲鸟照料行为。热浪雄鱼的后代身体状况较差,影响了游泳性能。总之,我们的研究结果突显了即使是短期事件也会对繁殖成功率、亲代行为和后代产生长期影响,为研究种群对快速环境变化的反应提供了深入的见解。
{"title":"Short-term heat waves have long-term consequences for parents and offspring in stickleback","authors":"Rachel Barrett, Laura R Stein","doi":"10.1093/beheco/arae036","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/arae036","url":null,"abstract":"Extreme temperature events, such as heat waves, can have lasting effects on the behavior, physiology, and reproductive success of organisms. Here we examine the impact of short-term exposure to a simulated heat wave on condition, parental care, and reproductive success in a population of threespine stickleback (Gasterosteus aculeatus), a small fish with exclusive paternal care, currently experiencing regular heat waves. Males were either exposed to a simulated heat wave (23°C) for five days or held at ideal temperature (18°C). Following this five-day treatment, all males were transferred to 18°C where they completed a full parenting cycle. Offspring were raised at 18°C. We found that while mass and body condition were unaffected in males exposed to a heat wave, cortisol responses were dampened across the nesting cycle compared to control males. In addition, heat wave males had longer latency for eggs to hatch, lower hatching success, and showed lower levels of parental care behavior compared to control males. Offspring of heat wave males had lower body condition, affecting swimming performance. Altogether, our results highlight the long-term impact that even short-term events can have on reproductive success, parental behavior, and subsequent generations, providing insight into population responses to rapid environmental change.","PeriodicalId":8840,"journal":{"name":"Behavioral Ecology","volume":"13 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.4,"publicationDate":"2024-04-27","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140812302","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"环境科学与生态学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Behavioral Ecology
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1