首页 > 最新文献

Cortex最新文献

英文 中文
Cognitive control & the anterior cingulate cortex: Necessity & coherence. 认知控制与前扣带皮层:必要性与连贯性。
IF 3.2 2区 心理学 Q1 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2025-01-01 Epub Date: 2024-11-29 DOI: 10.1016/j.cortex.2024.11.010
Lisa Cipolotti, Joe Mole, James K Ruffle, Amy Nelson, Robert Gray, Parashkev Nachev

Influential theories of complex behaviour invoke the notion of cognitive control modulated by conflict between counterfactual actions. Medial frontal cortex, notably the anterior cingulate cortex, has been variously posited as critical to such conflict detection, resolution, or monitoring, largely based on correlative data from functional imaging. Examining performance on the most widely used "conflict" task-Stroop-in a large cohort of patients with focal brain injury (N = 176), we compare anatomical patterns of lesion-inferred neural substrate dependence to those derived from functional imaging, meta-analytically summarised. Our results show that whereas performance is sensitive to the integrity of left lateral frontal regions implicated by functional imaging, it does not depend on medial frontal cortex, despite sampling adequate to reveal robust medial effects in the context of phonemic fluency. We suggest that medial frontal cortex is not critically invoked by Stroop and proceed to review the conceptual grounds for rejecting the core notion of conflict-driven cognitive control.

复杂行为的有影响力的理论援引认知控制的概念,由反事实行为之间的冲突调节。内侧额叶皮层,特别是前扣带皮层,在很大程度上基于功能成像的相关数据,被认为是冲突检测、解决或监测的关键。在一大批局灶性脑损伤患者(N = 176)中,研究了最广泛使用的“冲突”任务(stroop)的表现,我们比较了病变推断的神经基质依赖的解剖模式和功能成像得出的解剖模式,并进行了荟萃分析总结。我们的研究结果表明,尽管表现对与功能成像有关的左侧外侧额叶区域的完整性敏感,但它并不依赖于内侧额叶皮层,尽管取样足够充分地揭示了音素流畅性背景下强大的内侧效应。我们认为内侧额叶皮层并没有被Stroop批判性地引用,并继续回顾拒绝冲突驱动认知控制核心概念的概念依据。
{"title":"Cognitive control & the anterior cingulate cortex: Necessity & coherence.","authors":"Lisa Cipolotti, Joe Mole, James K Ruffle, Amy Nelson, Robert Gray, Parashkev Nachev","doi":"10.1016/j.cortex.2024.11.010","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cortex.2024.11.010","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Influential theories of complex behaviour invoke the notion of cognitive control modulated by conflict between counterfactual actions. Medial frontal cortex, notably the anterior cingulate cortex, has been variously posited as critical to such conflict detection, resolution, or monitoring, largely based on correlative data from functional imaging. Examining performance on the most widely used \"conflict\" task-Stroop-in a large cohort of patients with focal brain injury (N = 176), we compare anatomical patterns of lesion-inferred neural substrate dependence to those derived from functional imaging, meta-analytically summarised. Our results show that whereas performance is sensitive to the integrity of left lateral frontal regions implicated by functional imaging, it does not depend on medial frontal cortex, despite sampling adequate to reveal robust medial effects in the context of phonemic fluency. We suggest that medial frontal cortex is not critically invoked by Stroop and proceed to review the conceptual grounds for rejecting the core notion of conflict-driven cognitive control.</p>","PeriodicalId":10758,"journal":{"name":"Cortex","volume":" ","pages":"87-99"},"PeriodicalIF":3.2,"publicationDate":"2025-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142790992","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Mirror writing of digits: Is there a difference between boys and girls? 数字的镜像书写:男孩和女孩之间有区别吗?
IF 3.2 2区 心理学 Q1 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2025-01-01 Epub Date: 2024-11-08 DOI: 10.1016/j.cortex.2024.10.014
Jean-Paul Fischer

The neurological process of mirror generalization in memory, also known as mirror symmetrization, presents a real dilemma for typically developing 5- to 6-year-olds when learning to write characters (digits and letters). Should they write the digit 3 oriented to the left, that is correctly, or to the right, which leads to its mirror image ε? It has been anecdotally suggested that boys are more prone to mirror-writing than girls, but there is no scientific evidence for this idea. The present article gathers data from 691 children in the upper section of the French école maternelle (age between 5 and 6 ½), who each wrote the digits 0 to 9 four times under dictation and not necessarily in their natural order. Both simple and complex (mixed-effects linear regression) statistical comparisons on the percentages of digit reversal, show a substantial difference: girls produce more mirror reversals than boys. And yet the reversal curves as a function of the digits are quite similar between the two sexes (r = .97). It has been proposed that mirror reversal of characters results from the left orientation of some of them (e.g., 3, 7, J, Z), that is, in an orientation contrary to the direction of writing in our Western cultures. The present investigation shows that (1) this character orientation hypothesis (choosing to write characters in the same orientation as sentence writing) better explains reversals than the counterclockwise hypothesis (children are trained to draw circles counter-clockwise to prepare for attached cursive writing); (2) the study of the stability of reversals additionally supports the explanation of mirror writing by the left orientation of the digits (1, 2, 3, 7 and, less obviously, 9); but (3) neither of the preceding findings (left-right orientation and stability) provided a convincing explanation for the aforementioned gender difference.

记忆中镜像泛化的神经学过程,也被称为镜像对称,在学习写字符(数字和字母)时给5到6岁的孩子带来了一个真正的困境。数字3应该向左写,这是正确的,还是向右写,这导致它的镜像ε?有传闻称,男孩比女孩更倾向于镜像书写,但这一观点没有科学依据。本文收集了691名法国儿童(年龄在5岁到6岁半之间)的数据,他们在口述的情况下将数字0到9写了四次,不一定是按自然顺序写的。简单和复杂(混合效应线性回归)的数字反转百分比统计比较显示出实质性差异:女孩比男孩产生更多的镜像反转。然而,两性之间的反向曲线作为手指的函数是非常相似的(r = .97)。有人提出,汉字的镜像反转是由于其中一些字符(例如,3,7,j, Z)的向左方向造成的,也就是说,与我们西方文化中的书写方向相反。本研究表明:(1)汉字朝向假设(选择与句子书写方向相同的汉字)比逆时针假设(儿童被训练逆时针画圆圈以准备附体草书书写)更能解释反转;(2)对反转稳定性的研究进一步支持了数字左向书写的解释(1、2、3、7和不太明显的9);但是(3)之前的研究结果(左右取向和稳定性)都不能令人信服地解释上述性别差异。
{"title":"Mirror writing of digits: Is there a difference between boys and girls?","authors":"Jean-Paul Fischer","doi":"10.1016/j.cortex.2024.10.014","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cortex.2024.10.014","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The neurological process of mirror generalization in memory, also known as mirror symmetrization, presents a real dilemma for typically developing 5- to 6-year-olds when learning to write characters (digits and letters). Should they write the digit 3 oriented to the left, that is correctly, or to the right, which leads to its mirror image ε? It has been anecdotally suggested that boys are more prone to mirror-writing than girls, but there is no scientific evidence for this idea. The present article gathers data from 691 children in the upper section of the French école maternelle (age between 5 and 6 ½), who each wrote the digits 0 to 9 four times under dictation and not necessarily in their natural order. Both simple and complex (mixed-effects linear regression) statistical comparisons on the percentages of digit reversal, show a substantial difference: girls produce more mirror reversals than boys. And yet the reversal curves as a function of the digits are quite similar between the two sexes (r = .97). It has been proposed that mirror reversal of characters results from the left orientation of some of them (e.g., 3, 7, J, Z), that is, in an orientation contrary to the direction of writing in our Western cultures. The present investigation shows that (1) this character orientation hypothesis (choosing to write characters in the same orientation as sentence writing) better explains reversals than the counterclockwise hypothesis (children are trained to draw circles counter-clockwise to prepare for attached cursive writing); (2) the study of the stability of reversals additionally supports the explanation of mirror writing by the left orientation of the digits (1, 2, 3, 7 and, less obviously, 9); but (3) neither of the preceding findings (left-right orientation and stability) provided a convincing explanation for the aforementioned gender difference.</p>","PeriodicalId":10758,"journal":{"name":"Cortex","volume":" ","pages":"124-134"},"PeriodicalIF":3.2,"publicationDate":"2025-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142754841","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Definition: Mirror writing. 定义镜像书写
IF 3.2 2区 心理学 Q1 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2025-01-01 Epub Date: 2024-08-22 DOI: 10.1016/j.cortex.2024.07.017
Jean-Paul Fischer, Robert D McIntosh
{"title":"Definition: Mirror writing.","authors":"Jean-Paul Fischer, Robert D McIntosh","doi":"10.1016/j.cortex.2024.07.017","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cortex.2024.07.017","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":10758,"journal":{"name":"Cortex","volume":" ","pages":"214-215"},"PeriodicalIF":3.2,"publicationDate":"2025-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142105148","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Brothers in Cortex: For Sergio. 《皮质兄弟》:献给塞尔吉奥。
IF 3.2 2区 心理学 Q1 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2025-01-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.cortex.2024.12.001
Jordan Grafman
{"title":"Brothers in Cortex: For Sergio.","authors":"Jordan Grafman","doi":"10.1016/j.cortex.2024.12.001","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2024.12.001","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":10758,"journal":{"name":"Cortex","volume":"182 ","pages":"197-198"},"PeriodicalIF":3.2,"publicationDate":"2025-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143001624","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Editorial integrity in the era of cancel culture. 取消文化时代的编辑诚信。
IF 3.2 2区 心理学 Q1 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2025-01-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.cortex.2024.11.004
Mike Anderson
{"title":"Editorial integrity in the era of cancel culture.","authors":"Mike Anderson","doi":"10.1016/j.cortex.2024.11.004","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2024.11.004","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":10758,"journal":{"name":"Cortex","volume":"182 ","pages":"203-207"},"PeriodicalIF":3.2,"publicationDate":"2025-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143001627","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Unveiling spatial and non-spatial aspects of neglect in everyday behavior. 揭示日常行为中忽视的空间和非空间方面。
IF 3.2 2区 心理学 Q1 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2025-01-01 Epub Date: 2024-10-22 DOI: 10.1016/j.cortex.2024.10.002
Brigitte C Kaufmann, Paolo Bartolomeo, Monica N Toba
{"title":"Unveiling spatial and non-spatial aspects of neglect in everyday behavior.","authors":"Brigitte C Kaufmann, Paolo Bartolomeo, Monica N Toba","doi":"10.1016/j.cortex.2024.10.002","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cortex.2024.10.002","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":10758,"journal":{"name":"Cortex","volume":" ","pages":"208-211"},"PeriodicalIF":3.2,"publicationDate":"2025-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142557344","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The curious case of Cortex covers. 皮质的奇特案例。
IF 3.2 2区 心理学 Q1 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2025-01-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.cortex.2024.11.003
Anjan Chatterjee
{"title":"The curious case of Cortex covers.","authors":"Anjan Chatterjee","doi":"10.1016/j.cortex.2024.11.003","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2024.11.003","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":10758,"journal":{"name":"Cortex","volume":"182 ","pages":"199-202"},"PeriodicalIF":3.2,"publicationDate":"2025-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143001631","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Visual short-term memory binding and attentional processes during object-scene integration are preserved in mild cognitive impairment. 在轻度认知障碍中,客体-场景整合过程中的视觉短期记忆结合和注意过程得以保留。
IF 3.2 2区 心理学 Q1 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2025-01-01 Epub Date: 2024-12-19 DOI: 10.1016/j.cortex.2024.12.002
Elena Allegretti, Marika Mauti, Moreno I Coco

Binding, a critical cognitive process likely mediated by attention, is essential for creating coherent object representations within a scene. This process is vulnerable in individuals with dementia, who exhibit deficits in visual working memory (VWM) binding, primarily tested using abstract arrays of standalone objects. To explore how binding operates in more realistic settings across the lifespan, we examined the impact of object saliency and semantic consistency on VWM binding and the role of overt attention. Using an eye-tracking change detection task, we compared younger adults, healthy older adults, and individuals with Mild Cognitive Impairment (MCI). Participants were presented with naturalistic scenes and asked to detect changes in the identity and/or location of objects that were either semantically consistent or inconsistent with their scene context. Across all age groups, semantically inconsistent objects were prioritised during encoding, leading to better change detection than consistent objects. Highly salient objects decreased the inconsistency advantage while being detrimental to detection accuracy when inspected at longer latencies to the first fixation. Longer fixation durations on the critical object were beneficial for recognition. In contrast, delayed initial inspection or frequent subsequent fixations on other objects were detrimental to detection, regardless of age or cognitive impairment. These findings challenge the notion of generalised semantic memory impairment in the prodromal stages of dementia and highlight the importance of efficient attentional control in supporting VWM binding, even in the face of cognitive decline. Overall, preserved low-level and high-level mechanisms of object-scene integration can compensate for age-related cognitive decline, enabling successful binding in naturalistic contexts.

绑定是一个关键的认知过程,可能由注意力介导,对于在场景中创建连贯的对象表征至关重要。这一过程在痴呆症患者中是脆弱的,他们表现出视觉工作记忆(VWM)结合的缺陷,主要使用独立对象的抽象数组进行测试。为了探索在整个生命周期中绑定如何在更现实的环境中运作,我们研究了对象显著性和语义一致性对VWM绑定的影响以及显性注意的作用。使用眼动追踪变化检测任务,我们比较了年轻人、健康老年人和轻度认知障碍(MCI)患者。参与者被呈现在自然场景中,并被要求检测物体的身份和/或位置的变化,这些物体在语义上与场景上下文一致或不一致。在所有年龄组中,语义不一致的对象在编码过程中被优先考虑,导致比一致的对象更好的变化检测。高度突出的物体减少了不一致的优势,但当在第一次固定的较长时间内检查时,对检测精度有害。长时间注视关键物体有利于识别。相比之下,无论年龄或认知障碍如何,延迟初始检查或随后频繁盯着其他物体都不利于检测。这些发现挑战了痴呆症前驱阶段普遍语义记忆障碍的概念,并强调了有效的注意力控制在支持VWM结合方面的重要性,即使在面对认知衰退时也是如此。总的来说,保留的低水平和高水平的对象-场景整合机制可以弥补与年龄相关的认知衰退,使其在自然环境中成功结合。
{"title":"Visual short-term memory binding and attentional processes during object-scene integration are preserved in mild cognitive impairment.","authors":"Elena Allegretti, Marika Mauti, Moreno I Coco","doi":"10.1016/j.cortex.2024.12.002","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cortex.2024.12.002","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Binding, a critical cognitive process likely mediated by attention, is essential for creating coherent object representations within a scene. This process is vulnerable in individuals with dementia, who exhibit deficits in visual working memory (VWM) binding, primarily tested using abstract arrays of standalone objects. To explore how binding operates in more realistic settings across the lifespan, we examined the impact of object saliency and semantic consistency on VWM binding and the role of overt attention. Using an eye-tracking change detection task, we compared younger adults, healthy older adults, and individuals with Mild Cognitive Impairment (MCI). Participants were presented with naturalistic scenes and asked to detect changes in the identity and/or location of objects that were either semantically consistent or inconsistent with their scene context. Across all age groups, semantically inconsistent objects were prioritised during encoding, leading to better change detection than consistent objects. Highly salient objects decreased the inconsistency advantage while being detrimental to detection accuracy when inspected at longer latencies to the first fixation. Longer fixation durations on the critical object were beneficial for recognition. In contrast, delayed initial inspection or frequent subsequent fixations on other objects were detrimental to detection, regardless of age or cognitive impairment. These findings challenge the notion of generalised semantic memory impairment in the prodromal stages of dementia and highlight the importance of efficient attentional control in supporting VWM binding, even in the face of cognitive decline. Overall, preserved low-level and high-level mechanisms of object-scene integration can compensate for age-related cognitive decline, enabling successful binding in naturalistic contexts.</p>","PeriodicalId":10758,"journal":{"name":"Cortex","volume":" ","pages":"53-70"},"PeriodicalIF":3.2,"publicationDate":"2025-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142913840","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
A 20-year tale on closing-in behavior in graphic copying tasks: Revisiting Della Sala's findings in new samples of patients with dementia and stroke. 一个关于图形复制任务中封闭行为的20年故事:重新审视德拉·萨拉在痴呆症和中风患者新样本中的发现。
IF 3.2 2区 心理学 Q1 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2025-01-01 Epub Date: 2024-11-19 DOI: 10.1016/j.cortex.2024.10.018
N De Lucia, H B Coslett, E Ambron

Closing-in behavior (CIB) is characterized by the placement of the graphic copy near (Near CIB) or even on the top of (Overlap CIB) the stimulus to be reproduced. Although CIB has received little attention in the literature, Sergio Della Sala and colleagues made important contributions to the understanding of the phenomenon. They noted that CIB is often observed in Alzheimer's Disease but is also present in other forms of dementia and mild cognitive impairment and stroke; they argued that CIB may reflect a deficit in executive function, rather than working memory, and that the phenomenon occurs more frequently in dual task conditions. Importantly, they demonstrated that CIB may not be specific to copying but may instead reflect a general deficit in decoupling movement location from the focus of attention. In the present study, we explored these observations in a mixed sample of 106 participants (AD n = 37, frontal stroke n = 25, other forms of dementia n = 24, and normal controls n = 20). First, we confirmed that CIB is equally common in AD, other forms of dementia and frontal stroke. Second, we confirmed the association between CIB and executive function deficits. Third, we showed that individuals with CIB are more likely to exhibit the phenomenon in dual task situations, in which line-drawing is associated with an unrelated secondary task (tapping, counting, or counting backward). The present work supports and extends the contributions of Della Sala and colleagues demonstrating that CIB is enhanced when the general attentional load of the task increases.

闭合行为(CIB)的特征是将图形副本放置在待复制刺激的附近(near CIB)甚至顶部(Overlap CIB)。尽管CIB在文献中很少受到关注,但塞尔吉奥·德拉·萨拉及其同事对理解这一现象做出了重要贡献。他们指出,CIB经常在阿尔茨海默病中观察到,但也存在于其他形式的痴呆、轻度认知障碍和中风中;他们认为CIB可能反映了执行功能的缺陷,而不是工作记忆的缺陷,而且这种现象在双重任务条件下更频繁发生。重要的是,他们证明了CIB可能不是特定于复制的,而是反映了运动位置与注意力焦点解耦的普遍缺陷。在本研究中,我们在106名参与者的混合样本中探讨了这些观察结果(AD n = 37,额叶中风n = 25,其他形式的痴呆n = 24,正常对照n = 20)。首先,我们证实CIB在阿尔茨海默病、其他形式的痴呆和额叶中风中同样常见。其次,我们证实了CIB与执行功能缺陷之间的联系。第三,我们发现CIB患者在双重任务情境下更有可能表现出这种现象,在双重任务情境下,画线与一个不相关的次要任务(敲击、计数或向后计数)有关。本研究支持并扩展了Della Sala及其同事的研究成果,即当任务的一般注意负荷增加时,CIB会增强。
{"title":"A 20-year tale on closing-in behavior in graphic copying tasks: Revisiting Della Sala's findings in new samples of patients with dementia and stroke.","authors":"N De Lucia, H B Coslett, E Ambron","doi":"10.1016/j.cortex.2024.10.018","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cortex.2024.10.018","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Closing-in behavior (CIB) is characterized by the placement of the graphic copy near (Near CIB) or even on the top of (Overlap CIB) the stimulus to be reproduced. Although CIB has received little attention in the literature, Sergio Della Sala and colleagues made important contributions to the understanding of the phenomenon. They noted that CIB is often observed in Alzheimer's Disease but is also present in other forms of dementia and mild cognitive impairment and stroke; they argued that CIB may reflect a deficit in executive function, rather than working memory, and that the phenomenon occurs more frequently in dual task conditions. Importantly, they demonstrated that CIB may not be specific to copying but may instead reflect a general deficit in decoupling movement location from the focus of attention. In the present study, we explored these observations in a mixed sample of 106 participants (AD n = 37, frontal stroke n = 25, other forms of dementia n = 24, and normal controls n = 20). First, we confirmed that CIB is equally common in AD, other forms of dementia and frontal stroke. Second, we confirmed the association between CIB and executive function deficits. Third, we showed that individuals with CIB are more likely to exhibit the phenomenon in dual task situations, in which line-drawing is associated with an unrelated secondary task (tapping, counting, or counting backward). The present work supports and extends the contributions of Della Sala and colleagues demonstrating that CIB is enhanced when the general attentional load of the task increases.</p>","PeriodicalId":10758,"journal":{"name":"Cortex","volume":" ","pages":"42-52"},"PeriodicalIF":3.2,"publicationDate":"2025-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142749664","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Bimodal aphasia and dysgraphia: Phonological output buffer aphasia and orthographic output buffer dysgraphia in spoken and sign language. 双峰性失语症和书写困难:语音输出缓冲失语症和正字法输出缓冲书写困难在口语和手语。
IF 3.2 2区 心理学 Q1 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2025-01-01 Epub Date: 2024-11-09 DOI: 10.1016/j.cortex.2024.10.013
Neta Haluts, Doron Levy, Naama Friedmann

We report a case of crossmodal bilingual aphasia-aphasia in two modalities, spoken and sign language-and dysgraphia in both writing and fingerspelling. The patient, Sunny, was a 42 year-old woman after a left temporo-parietal stroke, a speaker of Hebrew, Romanian, and English and an adult learner, daily user of Israeli Sign language (ISL). We assessed Sunny's spoken and sign languages using a comprehensive test battery of naming, reading, and repetition tasks, and also analysed her spontaneous-speech and sign. Her writing and fingerspelling were assessed using tasks of dictation, naming, and delayed copying. In spoken language production, Sunny showed a classical phonological output buffer (POB) impairment in naming, reading, repetition, and spontaneous production, with phonological errors (transpositions, substitutions, insertions, and omissions) in words and pseudo-words, and whole-unit errors in morphological affixes, function-words, and number-words, with a length effect. Importantly, her error pattern in ISL was remarkably similar in the parallel tasks, with phonological errors in signs and pseudo-signs, affecting all the phonological parameters of the sign (movement, handshape, location, and orientation), and whole-unit errors in morphemes, function-signs, and number-signs. Sunny's impairment was selective to the POB, without phonological input, semantic-conceptual, or syntactic deficits. This shows for the first time how POB impairment, a kind of conduction aphasia, manifests itself in a sign language, and indicates that the POB for sign-language has the same cognitive architecture as the one for spoken language. It may also indicate similar neural underpinnings for spoken and sign languages. In writing, Sunny forms the first case of a selective type of dysgraphia in fingerspelling, orthographic (graphemic) output buffer dysgraphia. In both writing and fingerspelling, she made letter errors (letter transpositions, substitutions, insertions, and omissions), as well as morphological errors and errors in function words, and showed length effect. Sunny's impairment was selective to the orthographic output buffer, whereas her reading, including orthographic input processing, was intact. This suggests that the orthographic output buffer is shared for writing and fingerspelling, at least in a late learner of sign language. The results shed further light on the architecture of phonological and orthographic production.

我们报告一例跨模态双语失语症,口语和手语两种形式的失语症,以及书写和拼写的困难。患者Sunny是一名左颞顶叶中风后的42岁女性,会说希伯来语、罗马尼亚语和英语,是一名成人学习者,日常使用以色列手语(ISL)。我们通过命名、阅读和重复任务的综合测试来评估Sunny的口语和手语,并分析了她的自发语言和手语。她的写作和拼写是通过听写、命名和延迟抄写来评估的。在口语生产中,Sunny在命名、阅读、重复和自发生产方面表现出典型的语音输出缓冲(POB)障碍,在单词和假词中存在语音错误(换位、替换、插入和省略),在形态词缀、功能词和数字词中存在整体单位错误,并具有长度效应。重要的是,她在ISL中的错误模式在平行任务中非常相似,符号和伪符号的语音错误,影响了符号的所有语音参数(运动、手形、位置和方向),以及语素、功能符号和数字符号的整体单位错误。Sunny的缺陷是选择性的POB,没有语音输入、语义概念或句法缺陷。这首次揭示了作为一种传导性失语症的POB障碍是如何在手语中表现出来的,并表明手语的POB与口语的POB具有相同的认知结构。这也可能表明口语和手语的神经基础是相似的。在写作方面,Sunny形成了第一例选择性拼写、正字法输出缓冲书写困难的案例。无论是书写还是拼字,她都出现了字母错误(字母调换、替换、插入、遗漏),以及形态错误和虚词错误,并表现出长度效应。Sunny的正字法输出缓冲是选择性的,而她的阅读,包括正字法输入处理,是完整的。这表明正字法输出缓冲区是用于书写和拼写的,至少在手语的晚期学习者中是这样。研究结果进一步阐明了语音和正字法生产的结构。
{"title":"Bimodal aphasia and dysgraphia: Phonological output buffer aphasia and orthographic output buffer dysgraphia in spoken and sign language.","authors":"Neta Haluts, Doron Levy, Naama Friedmann","doi":"10.1016/j.cortex.2024.10.013","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cortex.2024.10.013","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>We report a case of crossmodal bilingual aphasia-aphasia in two modalities, spoken and sign language-and dysgraphia in both writing and fingerspelling. The patient, Sunny, was a 42 year-old woman after a left temporo-parietal stroke, a speaker of Hebrew, Romanian, and English and an adult learner, daily user of Israeli Sign language (ISL). We assessed Sunny's spoken and sign languages using a comprehensive test battery of naming, reading, and repetition tasks, and also analysed her spontaneous-speech and sign. Her writing and fingerspelling were assessed using tasks of dictation, naming, and delayed copying. In spoken language production, Sunny showed a classical phonological output buffer (POB) impairment in naming, reading, repetition, and spontaneous production, with phonological errors (transpositions, substitutions, insertions, and omissions) in words and pseudo-words, and whole-unit errors in morphological affixes, function-words, and number-words, with a length effect. Importantly, her error pattern in ISL was remarkably similar in the parallel tasks, with phonological errors in signs and pseudo-signs, affecting all the phonological parameters of the sign (movement, handshape, location, and orientation), and whole-unit errors in morphemes, function-signs, and number-signs. Sunny's impairment was selective to the POB, without phonological input, semantic-conceptual, or syntactic deficits. This shows for the first time how POB impairment, a kind of conduction aphasia, manifests itself in a sign language, and indicates that the POB for sign-language has the same cognitive architecture as the one for spoken language. It may also indicate similar neural underpinnings for spoken and sign languages. In writing, Sunny forms the first case of a selective type of dysgraphia in fingerspelling, orthographic (graphemic) output buffer dysgraphia. In both writing and fingerspelling, she made letter errors (letter transpositions, substitutions, insertions, and omissions), as well as morphological errors and errors in function words, and showed length effect. Sunny's impairment was selective to the orthographic output buffer, whereas her reading, including orthographic input processing, was intact. This suggests that the orthographic output buffer is shared for writing and fingerspelling, at least in a late learner of sign language. The results shed further light on the architecture of phonological and orthographic production.</p>","PeriodicalId":10758,"journal":{"name":"Cortex","volume":" ","pages":"147-180"},"PeriodicalIF":3.2,"publicationDate":"2025-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142821902","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Cortex
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1