首页 > 最新文献

Discourse Processes最新文献

英文 中文
The costs of multimodal metaphors: comparing ERPs to figurative expressions in verbal and verbo-pictorial formats 多模态隐喻的代价:比较ERP对语言和动词图画形式的形象表达的影响
IF 2.2 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EDUCATIONAL Pub Date : 2023-12-18 DOI: 10.1080/0163853x.2023.2282895
Valentina Bambini, Giacomo Ranieri, Luca Bischetti, Biagio Scalingi, Chiara Bertini, Irene Ricci, Walter Schaeken, Paolo Canal
Psycholinguistic research on metaphor has focused on verbal material. Yet, metaphors frequently occur in a multimodal format, blending words and pictures to convey meaning. Here we compared verbal ...
关于隐喻的心理语言学研究主要集中在语言材料上。然而,隐喻经常以多模态的形式出现,融合文字和图片来传达意义。在这里,我们比较了口头隐喻和图画隐喻。
{"title":"The costs of multimodal metaphors: comparing ERPs to figurative expressions in verbal and verbo-pictorial formats","authors":"Valentina Bambini, Giacomo Ranieri, Luca Bischetti, Biagio Scalingi, Chiara Bertini, Irene Ricci, Walter Schaeken, Paolo Canal","doi":"10.1080/0163853x.2023.2282895","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/0163853x.2023.2282895","url":null,"abstract":"Psycholinguistic research on metaphor has focused on verbal material. Yet, metaphors frequently occur in a multimodal format, blending words and pictures to convey meaning. Here we compared verbal ...","PeriodicalId":11316,"journal":{"name":"Discourse Processes","volume":"306 2 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2023-12-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138745262","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Text analysis approach to measuring text social information in children’s picture books 用文本分析法衡量儿童图画书中的文本社会信息
IF 2.2 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EDUCATIONAL Pub Date : 2023-11-26 DOI: 10.1080/0163853X.2023.2291297
M. M. Davidson
ABSTRACT Text social information includes the cognitive processes and social communication skills that support real or hypothetical human thought or interaction. The current measure of text social information is genre. However, genre is a limited measure because of poor operationalization, limited specificity, and overlap with structural and linguistic differences. The purpose of this study was to develop an automated text analysis approach to measure text social information in children’s picture books beyond genre. Studies 1 and 2 found convergent and divergent validity for several measures that captured text social information with these measures being significantly higher in children’s fiction compared to nonfiction books and not correlated with other structural and linguistic text measures. Study 3 found two components of text social information, based on a principal component analysis. These two components captured a general socialness factor (i.e., theory of mind, emotions, and social relationships) and pragmatics/conversation. Study 4 provides preliminary evidence for the predictive validity of the text social information measures. Together, this study provides an initial set of continuous measures for measuring text social information that can begin to advance the field in determining how text social information impacts comprehension and social cognition.
摘要 文本社会信息包括支持真实或假设的人类思维或互动的认知过程和社会交流技能。目前衡量文本社会信息的标准是体裁。然而,体裁是一种有限的衡量标准,因为其可操作性差、特异性有限,而且与结构和语言差异重叠。本研究旨在开发一种自动文本分析方法,以衡量儿童图画书中超越体裁的文本社会信息。研究 1 和研究 2 发现了几种捕捉文本社会信息的测量方法的收敛有效性和发散有效性,与非虚构类图书相比,这些测量方法在儿童虚构类图书中的使用率明显更高,而且与其他结构和语言文本测量方法没有关联。研究 3 通过主成分分析发现了文本社交信息的两个组成部分。这两个组成部分分别反映了一般社会性因素(即心智理论、情感和社会关系)和语用/对话。研究 4 为文本社交信息测量的预测有效性提供了初步证据。总之,本研究为测量文本社交信息提供了一套初步的连续测量方法,可以开始推动该领域确定文本社交信息如何影响理解和社交认知。
{"title":"Text analysis approach to measuring text social information in children’s picture books","authors":"M. M. Davidson","doi":"10.1080/0163853X.2023.2291297","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/0163853X.2023.2291297","url":null,"abstract":"ABSTRACT Text social information includes the cognitive processes and social communication skills that support real or hypothetical human thought or interaction. The current measure of text social information is genre. However, genre is a limited measure because of poor operationalization, limited specificity, and overlap with structural and linguistic differences. The purpose of this study was to develop an automated text analysis approach to measure text social information in children’s picture books beyond genre. Studies 1 and 2 found convergent and divergent validity for several measures that captured text social information with these measures being significantly higher in children’s fiction compared to nonfiction books and not correlated with other structural and linguistic text measures. Study 3 found two components of text social information, based on a principal component analysis. These two components captured a general socialness factor (i.e., theory of mind, emotions, and social relationships) and pragmatics/conversation. Study 4 provides preliminary evidence for the predictive validity of the text social information measures. Together, this study provides an initial set of continuous measures for measuring text social information that can begin to advance the field in determining how text social information impacts comprehension and social cognition.","PeriodicalId":11316,"journal":{"name":"Discourse Processes","volume":"28 1","pages":"695 - 721"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2023-11-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139235258","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Vocabulary size and exposure to print predict mastery of connectives in teenage years 词汇量的大小和接触印刷品的时间可以预测青少年对连接词的掌握程度
2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EDUCATIONAL Pub Date : 2023-10-16 DOI: 10.1080/0163853x.2023.2266963
Ekaterina Tskhovrebova, Sandrine Zufferey, Elena Tribushinina
Connectives such as because and but are crucial for signaling coherence relations in discourse. They contribute to a better reading comprehension and, thus, academic performance. The aim of this article is to contribute to our understanding of connective development during teenage years by studying individual differences in the performance of native Russian-speaking teenagers (N = 107, Mage = 13.93, range: 11 to 17) in a connective-cloze task. The tested connectives marked six coherence relations and were used either predominantly in speech or in the written language. In addition, we examined whether students’ performance with the connectives was modulated by their general linguistic experience, as assessed by a vocabulary test and degree of exposure to print. Our results reveal that interpersonal differences in lexicon size and level of exposure to print were the strongest predictors of appropriate usage of connectives, whereas differences in age, connective mode, and polyfunctionality played a lesser role. This finding may indicate that, starting from age 11, biological age and intrinsic properties of connectives matter less for their mastery than general linguistic experience, as measured by vocabulary level and exposure to print.
because和but等连接词是语篇中表达连贯关系的关键。它们有助于更好的阅读理解,从而提高学习成绩。本文的目的是通过研究母语为俄语的青少年(N = 107, Mage = 13.93,范围:11 - 17)在连接填空任务中的个体差异,帮助我们理解青少年时期的连接发展。测试的连接词标记了六种连贯关系,主要用于口头或书面语言。此外,我们通过词汇测试和接触印刷品的程度来评估学生使用连接词的表现是否受到他们的一般语言经验的调节。我们的研究结果表明,词汇量和印刷品接触水平的人际差异是正确使用连接词的最强预测因子,而年龄、连接方式和多功能性的差异则起着较小的作用。这一发现可能表明,从11岁开始,生物年龄和连接词的内在属性对他们掌握的影响不如一般的语言经验,这是通过词汇水平和接触印刷品来衡量的。
{"title":"Vocabulary size and exposure to print predict mastery of connectives in teenage years","authors":"Ekaterina Tskhovrebova, Sandrine Zufferey, Elena Tribushinina","doi":"10.1080/0163853x.2023.2266963","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/0163853x.2023.2266963","url":null,"abstract":"Connectives such as because and but are crucial for signaling coherence relations in discourse. They contribute to a better reading comprehension and, thus, academic performance. The aim of this article is to contribute to our understanding of connective development during teenage years by studying individual differences in the performance of native Russian-speaking teenagers (N = 107, Mage = 13.93, range: 11 to 17) in a connective-cloze task. The tested connectives marked six coherence relations and were used either predominantly in speech or in the written language. In addition, we examined whether students’ performance with the connectives was modulated by their general linguistic experience, as assessed by a vocabulary test and degree of exposure to print. Our results reveal that interpersonal differences in lexicon size and level of exposure to print were the strongest predictors of appropriate usage of connectives, whereas differences in age, connective mode, and polyfunctionality played a lesser role. This finding may indicate that, starting from age 11, biological age and intrinsic properties of connectives matter less for their mastery than general linguistic experience, as measured by vocabulary level and exposure to print.","PeriodicalId":11316,"journal":{"name":"Discourse Processes","volume":"222 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-10-16","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"136112227","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Comprehending irony in text: evidence from scanpaths 理解文本中的反讽:来自扫描路径的证据
2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EDUCATIONAL Pub Date : 2023-10-16 DOI: 10.1080/0163853x.2023.2266964
Henri Olkoniemi, Diane Mézière, Johanna K. Kaakinen
Eyetracking studies have shown that readers reread ironic phrases when resolving their meaning. Moreover, it has been shown that the timecourse of processing ironic meaning is affected by reader’s working memory capacity (WMC). Irony is a context-dependent phenomenon but using traditional eye-movement measures it is difficult to analyze processing beyond sentence-level. A promising method to study individual differences in irony processing at the paragraph-level is scanpath analysis. In the present experiment, we analyzed whether individual differences in WMC are reflected in scanpaths during reading ironic stories by combining data from two previous eye-tracking studies (N = 120). The results revealed three different reading patterns: fast-and-linear reading, selective reading, and nonselective rereading. The readers predominantly used the fast-and-linear reading pattern for ironic and literal stories. However, readers were less likely to use the nonselective rereading pattern with ironic than literal texts. The reading patterns for ironic stories were modulated by WMC. Results showed that scanpaths captured differences missed by standard measures, showing it to be a valuable tool to study individual differences in irony processing.
眼球追踪研究表明,读者在理解反讽短语的意思时,会重读它们。此外,研究还表明,反讽意义加工的时间过程受读者工作记忆容量的影响。反语是一种语境依赖现象,但传统的眼动测量方法难以分析句子层面以外的加工过程。扫描路径分析是研究段落反语加工个体差异的一种很有前途的方法。在本实验中,我们结合之前两项眼动追踪研究(N = 120)的数据,分析了阅读讽刺故事时WMC的个体差异是否反映在扫描路径上。结果显示了三种不同的阅读模式:快速线性阅读、选择性阅读和非选择性重读。读者主要使用快速和线性阅读模式来阅读讽刺和文字故事。然而,与字面文本相比,读者不太可能使用非选择性重读讽刺文本的模式。反讽故事的阅读模式受WMC的调节。结果表明,扫描路径捕捉到了标准测量所遗漏的差异,表明它是研究反语加工个体差异的一种有价值的工具。
{"title":"Comprehending irony in text: evidence from scanpaths","authors":"Henri Olkoniemi, Diane Mézière, Johanna K. Kaakinen","doi":"10.1080/0163853x.2023.2266964","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/0163853x.2023.2266964","url":null,"abstract":"Eyetracking studies have shown that readers reread ironic phrases when resolving their meaning. Moreover, it has been shown that the timecourse of processing ironic meaning is affected by reader’s working memory capacity (WMC). Irony is a context-dependent phenomenon but using traditional eye-movement measures it is difficult to analyze processing beyond sentence-level. A promising method to study individual differences in irony processing at the paragraph-level is scanpath analysis. In the present experiment, we analyzed whether individual differences in WMC are reflected in scanpaths during reading ironic stories by combining data from two previous eye-tracking studies (N = 120). The results revealed three different reading patterns: fast-and-linear reading, selective reading, and nonselective rereading. The readers predominantly used the fast-and-linear reading pattern for ironic and literal stories. However, readers were less likely to use the nonselective rereading pattern with ironic than literal texts. The reading patterns for ironic stories were modulated by WMC. Results showed that scanpaths captured differences missed by standard measures, showing it to be a valuable tool to study individual differences in irony processing.","PeriodicalId":11316,"journal":{"name":"Discourse Processes","volume":"28 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-10-16","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"136113241","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Eye-tracking evidence from attachment structures favors a serial model of discourse–sentence interactivity 来自依恋结构的眼动追踪证据支持话语-句子互动的序列模型
2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EDUCATIONAL Pub Date : 2023-10-04 DOI: 10.1080/0163853x.2023.2260246
Jack Dempsey, Anna Tsiola, Kiel Christianson
ABSTRACTMany psycholinguistic studies examine how people parse sentences in isolation; however, years of work in discourse processing have shown that sentence-level interpretations are influenced at some stage by discourse-level information. Evidence over the past 20 years remains mixed as to the temporal dynamics of such top-down interactions. In particular, dynamic accounts where readers use the discourse model to generate expectations for certain grammatical structures before and during parsing differ from serial accounts where an algorithmic first-pass processing mechanism precedes integration of sentence material into the discourse model. To test between these two theories, the current study investigates eye-movement behaviors when reading temporarily ambiguous attachment structures following discourses with biases either matching, mismatching, or neutral with respect to the attachment resolution. No evidence was found suggesting readers systematically use discourse information to generate structural expectations, in line with serial accounts of processing at the sentence–discourse interface. Scanpath analyses further highlight the confirmatory nature of rereading when participants encounter discourse continuations that do not fit with prior contexts. Disclosure statementNo potential conflict of interest was reported by the author(s).Data availability statementAll materials, data, and analyses are shared openly via OSF at https://osf.io/gfjn6/. DOI 10.17605/OSF.IO/GFJN6.Supplementary materialsSupplemental data for this article can be accessed online at https://doi.org/10.1080/0163853X.2023.2260246.
许多心理语言学研究考察人们如何孤立地分析句子;然而,多年来在语篇处理方面的研究表明,句子级解释在某些阶段会受到语篇级信息的影响。关于这种自上而下的相互作用的时间动态,过去20年的证据仍然是混杂的。特别是,读者在解析之前和过程中使用话语模型对某些语法结构产生期望的动态描述,不同于在将句子材料整合到话语模型之前采用算法先过处理机制的连续描述。为了检验这两种理论之间的差异,本研究调查了在阅读暂时模糊的依恋结构时的眼动行为,这些结构在依恋解决方面有匹配、不匹配或中立的偏见。没有证据表明读者系统地使用语篇信息来产生结构性期望,这与在句子-语篇界面处理的一系列报道相一致。扫描路径分析进一步强调,当参与者遇到不符合先前语境的话语延续时,重读的确认性。披露声明作者未报告潜在的利益冲突。数据可用性声明所有材料、数据和分析都通过OSF在https://osf.io/gfjn6/上公开共享。DOI 10.17605 / OSF.IO / GFJN6。补充材料本文的补充数据可在https://doi.org/10.1080/0163853X.2023.2260246上在线获取。
{"title":"Eye-tracking evidence from attachment structures favors a serial model of discourse–sentence interactivity","authors":"Jack Dempsey, Anna Tsiola, Kiel Christianson","doi":"10.1080/0163853x.2023.2260246","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/0163853x.2023.2260246","url":null,"abstract":"ABSTRACTMany psycholinguistic studies examine how people parse sentences in isolation; however, years of work in discourse processing have shown that sentence-level interpretations are influenced at some stage by discourse-level information. Evidence over the past 20 years remains mixed as to the temporal dynamics of such top-down interactions. In particular, dynamic accounts where readers use the discourse model to generate expectations for certain grammatical structures before and during parsing differ from serial accounts where an algorithmic first-pass processing mechanism precedes integration of sentence material into the discourse model. To test between these two theories, the current study investigates eye-movement behaviors when reading temporarily ambiguous attachment structures following discourses with biases either matching, mismatching, or neutral with respect to the attachment resolution. No evidence was found suggesting readers systematically use discourse information to generate structural expectations, in line with serial accounts of processing at the sentence–discourse interface. Scanpath analyses further highlight the confirmatory nature of rereading when participants encounter discourse continuations that do not fit with prior contexts. Disclosure statementNo potential conflict of interest was reported by the author(s).Data availability statementAll materials, data, and analyses are shared openly via OSF at https://osf.io/gfjn6/. DOI 10.17605/OSF.IO/GFJN6.Supplementary materialsSupplemental data for this article can be accessed online at https://doi.org/10.1080/0163853X.2023.2260246.","PeriodicalId":11316,"journal":{"name":"Discourse Processes","volume":"194 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-10-04","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"135591014","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Pinning down the interaction between animacy and syntactic function in the interpretation of German and Italian personal and demonstrative pronouns 德语和意大利语人称代词和指示代词解释中活力与句法功能的相互作用
2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EDUCATIONAL Pub Date : 2023-10-02 DOI: 10.1080/0163853x.2023.2252699
Markus Bader, Jacopo Torregrossa, Esther Rinke
This article investigates how animacy in interaction with the syntactic function of a referent’s antecedent determines the interpretation of different types of pronouns and demonstratives in German and Italian. The results of a sentence continuation task conducted in both languages show that Italian null pronouns and German p-pronouns have a strong tendency to refer to a preceding subject, but only if its referent is animate. With inanimate subjects, both forms tend to refer to the animate referent in object position, showing that animacy enhances a referent’s accessibility more than the syntactic function of its previous mention. Demonstratives in German and Italian generally tend to refer to object antecedents, a tendency that is also influenced by the animacy of the referent, especially in German. The cross-linguistic comparison reveals that the effect of animacy is overall stronger in German than in Italian, suggesting that across languages, different forms may show a different sensitivity to syntactic function and animacy.
本文研究了动性与指称先行词的句法功能的相互作用如何决定了德语和意大利语中不同类型代词和指示语的解释。在两种语言中进行的句子接续任务的结果表明,意大利语的空代词和德语的p代词都有很强的指向前面主语的倾向,但前提是它的指称对象是有生命的。对于无生命的主语,这两种形式都倾向于引用处于客体位置的有生命的指称物,这表明有生命性比前面提到的句法功能更能增强指称物的可及性。德语和意大利语的指示词通常倾向于指代宾语先行词,这种倾向也受到指称物的活力的影响,尤其是在德语中。跨语言比较发现,德语的动画效果总体上强于意大利语,这表明在不同的语言中,不同的形式对句法功能和动画的敏感性可能不同。
{"title":"Pinning down the interaction between animacy and syntactic function in the interpretation of German and Italian personal and demonstrative pronouns","authors":"Markus Bader, Jacopo Torregrossa, Esther Rinke","doi":"10.1080/0163853x.2023.2252699","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/0163853x.2023.2252699","url":null,"abstract":"This article investigates how animacy in interaction with the syntactic function of a referent’s antecedent determines the interpretation of different types of pronouns and demonstratives in German and Italian. The results of a sentence continuation task conducted in both languages show that Italian null pronouns and German p-pronouns have a strong tendency to refer to a preceding subject, but only if its referent is animate. With inanimate subjects, both forms tend to refer to the animate referent in object position, showing that animacy enhances a referent’s accessibility more than the syntactic function of its previous mention. Demonstratives in German and Italian generally tend to refer to object antecedents, a tendency that is also influenced by the animacy of the referent, especially in German. The cross-linguistic comparison reveals that the effect of animacy is overall stronger in German than in Italian, suggesting that across languages, different forms may show a different sensitivity to syntactic function and animacy.","PeriodicalId":11316,"journal":{"name":"Discourse Processes","volume":"54 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-10-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"135895815","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
How literary text reading is influenced by narrative voice and focalization: evidence from eye movements 文学文本阅读如何受到叙事声音和聚焦的影响:来自眼球运动的证据
2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EDUCATIONAL Pub Date : 2023-10-02 DOI: 10.1080/0163853x.2023.2260247
Lijuan Chen, Xiaodong Xu, Hongling Lv
ABSTRACTA fictional story is always narrated from a certain narrative voice and mode of focalization. These core narrative techniques have a major impact on how readers interpret the narrative plot and connect with the characters. This study used eye-tracking to investigate how classic narrative reading is affected by narrative voice and focalization. The results showed that the third-person narrative voice was read more slowly than the first-person narrative voice, especially when the narrative was presented with internal focalization. Importantly, the transition from a first-person to a third-person narrative voice generally resulted in longer reading times, whereas a switch from a third-person to a first-person narrative voice only yielded limited benefits in terms of reduced reading time. These findings provide direct evidence to support the assumption that there is a distinction between the first-person narration and the third-person narration and demonstrate the important role of narrative voice and focalization in understanding narrative texts. AcknowledgmentsWe are grateful to Professor Jie Zhang for his invaluable support in conducting this study. We are also grateful to Tianyue Wang and Yiyi Lu for their assistance in collecting and analyzing the data. We are thankful to the reviewer for prompting us to consider this important question.Disclosure statementNo potential conflict of interest was reported by the author(s).Additional informationFundingThis study was supported by the Jiangsu Social Science Fund (Grant No. [23YYB010]), the Grand for the Social Science Foundation of the Higher Education Institutions of Jiangsu Province (Grant No. [2021SJA0086]), the High-quality Research Project on the Application of Social Science in Jiangsu Province (Grant No. [22SWB-17]), and the National Social Science Foundation of China (Key Program: Grant No. [18AYY010]; Major Program: Grant No. [21&ZD288]).
摘要小说故事总是以一定的叙事声音和聚焦方式进行叙述。这些核心叙事技巧对读者如何解读叙事情节以及与人物的联系有着重要的影响。本研究采用眼动追踪的方法研究了叙事声音和聚焦对经典叙事阅读的影响。结果表明,第三人称叙事语态的阅读速度比第一人称叙事语态慢,尤其是当叙事呈现内部聚焦时。重要的是,从第一人称到第三人称叙事的转换通常会导致更长的阅读时间,而从第三人称到第一人称叙事的转换只会在减少阅读时间方面产生有限的好处。这些发现为第一人称叙述和第三人称叙述之间存在区别的假设提供了直接证据,并证明了叙事声音和聚焦在理解叙事文本中的重要作用。我们非常感谢张杰教授在本研究中给予的宝贵支持。我们也非常感谢王天悦和陆依依对数据收集和分析的协助。我们感谢审稿人促使我们考虑这个重要的问题。披露声明作者未报告潜在的利益冲突。本研究由江苏省社会科学基金资助(批准号:No. 8226)。[23 yyb010]),大的社会科学基金会江苏省高等教育机构(批准号[2021SJA0086]),江苏省社会科学应用高质量研究项目(批准号:2018102@qq.com);[22 swb-17]),中国国家社会科学基金(主要项目:批准号(18 ayy010);重大项目:批准号:[21 &zd288])。
{"title":"How literary text reading is influenced by narrative voice and focalization: <i>evidence from eye movements</i>","authors":"Lijuan Chen, Xiaodong Xu, Hongling Lv","doi":"10.1080/0163853x.2023.2260247","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/0163853x.2023.2260247","url":null,"abstract":"ABSTRACTA fictional story is always narrated from a certain narrative voice and mode of focalization. These core narrative techniques have a major impact on how readers interpret the narrative plot and connect with the characters. This study used eye-tracking to investigate how classic narrative reading is affected by narrative voice and focalization. The results showed that the third-person narrative voice was read more slowly than the first-person narrative voice, especially when the narrative was presented with internal focalization. Importantly, the transition from a first-person to a third-person narrative voice generally resulted in longer reading times, whereas a switch from a third-person to a first-person narrative voice only yielded limited benefits in terms of reduced reading time. These findings provide direct evidence to support the assumption that there is a distinction between the first-person narration and the third-person narration and demonstrate the important role of narrative voice and focalization in understanding narrative texts. AcknowledgmentsWe are grateful to Professor Jie Zhang for his invaluable support in conducting this study. We are also grateful to Tianyue Wang and Yiyi Lu for their assistance in collecting and analyzing the data. We are thankful to the reviewer for prompting us to consider this important question.Disclosure statementNo potential conflict of interest was reported by the author(s).Additional informationFundingThis study was supported by the Jiangsu Social Science Fund (Grant No. [23YYB010]), the Grand for the Social Science Foundation of the Higher Education Institutions of Jiangsu Province (Grant No. [2021SJA0086]), the High-quality Research Project on the Application of Social Science in Jiangsu Province (Grant No. [22SWB-17]), and the National Social Science Foundation of China (Key Program: Grant No. [18AYY010]; Major Program: Grant No. [21&ZD288]).","PeriodicalId":11316,"journal":{"name":"Discourse Processes","volume":"1 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-10-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"135898308","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Epistemicity and communicative strategies 认知与交际策略
2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EDUCATIONAL Pub Date : 2023-09-26 DOI: 10.1080/0163853x.2023.2255494
Alexandra Lorson, Hannah Rohde, Chris Cummins
When communicating, interlocutors negotiate knowledge by proposing propositional content to be added to their shared common ground. The way in which speakers put forward propositional content – expressing more or less confidence in its truthfulness – may affect the way in which other interlocutors react to such content. This article examines speakers’ production choices and hearers’ interpretations of the formulations believe/know/bare assertion to test how (maximal) certainty is expressed and inferred, whether speakers adjust their production choices depending on their communicative goals and whether hearers are able to adjust their interpretations correspondingly. For this purpose, we created two scenarios – one with a cooperative interlocutor and one with a potentially uncooperative interlocutor. The results suggest that know is epistemically the most powerful formulation – stronger than the bare assertion – but that the bare assertion may still be preferred over know for expressing maximal certainty in cooperative scenarios. Our findings also suggest that believe is used to hedge the assertive strength of statements in cooperative settings. Whereas speakers and hearers agree in the relative epistemic ordering of the formulations (believe < bare assertion < know), when inferring the speakers’ degrees of belief hearers to not appear to consistently take into account that speakers’ communicative goals may shift as a function of context.
在交流时,对话者通过提出要添加到他们共同基础上的命题内容来协商知识。说话者提出命题内容的方式——或多或少地表达对其真实性的信心——可能会影响其他对话者对该内容的反应。本文考察了说话人的生产选择和听者对believe/know/bare断言的解释,以检验(最大)确定性是如何表达和推断的,说话人是否会根据他们的交际目标调整他们的生产选择,听者是否能够相应地调整他们的解释。为此,我们创建了两个场景——一个具有合作的对话者,另一个具有潜在的不合作的对话者。结果表明,“知道”在认识论上是最有力的表述——比“赤裸裸的断言”更有力——但在合作场景中,“赤裸裸的断言”可能仍然比“知道”更能表达最大的确定性。我们的研究结果还表明,在合作环境中,信念被用来对冲陈述的自信强度。虽然说话者和听者在表述的相对认知顺序上是一致的(相信<断言<知道),但在推断说话者的信念程度时,听者似乎并没有始终考虑到说话者的交际目标可能会随着语境的变化而变化。
{"title":"Epistemicity and communicative strategies","authors":"Alexandra Lorson, Hannah Rohde, Chris Cummins","doi":"10.1080/0163853x.2023.2255494","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/0163853x.2023.2255494","url":null,"abstract":"When communicating, interlocutors negotiate knowledge by proposing propositional content to be added to their shared common ground. The way in which speakers put forward propositional content – expressing more or less confidence in its truthfulness – may affect the way in which other interlocutors react to such content. This article examines speakers’ production choices and hearers’ interpretations of the formulations believe/know/bare assertion to test how (maximal) certainty is expressed and inferred, whether speakers adjust their production choices depending on their communicative goals and whether hearers are able to adjust their interpretations correspondingly. For this purpose, we created two scenarios – one with a cooperative interlocutor and one with a potentially uncooperative interlocutor. The results suggest that know is epistemically the most powerful formulation – stronger than the bare assertion – but that the bare assertion may still be preferred over know for expressing maximal certainty in cooperative scenarios. Our findings also suggest that believe is used to hedge the assertive strength of statements in cooperative settings. Whereas speakers and hearers agree in the relative epistemic ordering of the formulations (believe < bare assertion < know), when inferring the speakers’ degrees of belief hearers to not appear to consistently take into account that speakers’ communicative goals may shift as a function of context.","PeriodicalId":11316,"journal":{"name":"Discourse Processes","volume":"42 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-09-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"134958351","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Non-gendered pronoun processing: an investigation of the gender non-specific third person singular pronoun ‘TA’ in Chinese 非性别代词加工:汉语非特定性别第三人称单数代词“TA”的研究
2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EDUCATIONAL Pub Date : 2023-09-18 DOI: 10.1080/0163853x.2023.2255075
Jordan Gallant, Kerry Sluchinski
ABSTRACT This study investigated the processing of the Chinese nongendered third-person singular pronoun, “TA,” in a series of self-paced reading experiments. We begin by investigating the perceived appropriateness of TA using a novel implementation of the modified maze task. We then contrasted reading latencies for TA and male- and female-gender pronouns in reference to antecedents with varying stereotypical gender (e.g., occupation terms) and definitional gender (e.g., kinship terms). In our analysis, we assessed several means of operationalizing stereotypical gender information. Optimal model performance was achieved with a continuous measure that accounted for individual differences in gender perception, suggesting the involvement of a probabilistic component. Results for reading latencies and perceived appropriateness of TA support previous findings from discourse analysis that TA is not entirely gender-neutral but rather has nuanced contexts of use in modern Chinese written discourse.
摘要本研究考察了汉语非性别第三人称单数代词“TA”在自定节奏阅读中的加工过程。我们首先使用一种改进的迷宫任务的新实现来调查TA的感知适当性。然后,我们对比了TA、男性和女性代词在不同刻板性别(如职业术语)和定义性别(如亲属术语)先行词下的阅读潜伏期。在我们的分析中,我们评估了将刻板印象的性别信息操作化的几种方法。通过考虑性别感知的个体差异的连续测量,可以实现最佳模型性能,这表明涉及概率成分。阅读延迟和语篇恰当性的结果支持了先前语篇分析的发现,即语篇并非完全性别中立,而是在现代汉语书面语篇中有微妙的使用语境。披露声明作者未报告潜在的利益冲突。数据可用性声明本研究的参与者没有书面同意公开分享他们的数据,因此无法获得支持本研究结果的数据。然而,所有的视觉句子刺激、相应的理解问题、问卷项目和分析代码都已在开放科学框架上公开发布:https://osf.io/n65hd/Additional信息。本工作得到了加拿大社会科学与人文研究理事会(752-2021-2541和752-2022-1171)的部分资助。
{"title":"Non-gendered pronoun processing: an investigation of the gender non-specific third person singular pronoun ‘TA’ in Chinese","authors":"Jordan Gallant, Kerry Sluchinski","doi":"10.1080/0163853x.2023.2255075","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/0163853x.2023.2255075","url":null,"abstract":"ABSTRACT This study investigated the processing of the Chinese nongendered third-person singular pronoun, “TA,” in a series of self-paced reading experiments. We begin by investigating the perceived appropriateness of TA using a novel implementation of the modified maze task. We then contrasted reading latencies for TA and male- and female-gender pronouns in reference to antecedents with varying stereotypical gender (e.g., occupation terms) and definitional gender (e.g., kinship terms). In our analysis, we assessed several means of operationalizing stereotypical gender information. Optimal model performance was achieved with a continuous measure that accounted for individual differences in gender perception, suggesting the involvement of a probabilistic component. Results for reading latencies and perceived appropriateness of TA support previous findings from discourse analysis that TA is not entirely gender-neutral but rather has nuanced contexts of use in modern Chinese written discourse.","PeriodicalId":11316,"journal":{"name":"Discourse Processes","volume":"172 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-09-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"135203397","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Structural repetition in responses to indirect requests 回应间接请求时的结构重复
2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EDUCATIONAL Pub Date : 2023-09-18 DOI: 10.1080/0163853x.2023.2255508
Katherine Chia, Ashley A. Edwards, Christopher Schatschneider, Michael P. Kaschak
ABSTRACTWe report three experiments that assess whether structural priming in a question–answer dialogue context is affected by the use of direct requests, conventional indirect requests, and nonconventional indirect requests. In Experiments 1 and 2, experimenters made phone calls to businesses and asked either Can you tell me (at) what time you close? (conventional indirect request) or May I ask you (at) what time you close? (nonconventional indirect request). Structural priming was demonstrated by participants’ greater tendency to produce a preposition in their response (At 9 vs. 9 o’clock) when the question had a preposition than when it did not. The results of Experiments 1 and 2 suggest that priming is not statistically different across request types. In Experiment 3, we compared priming for the conventional indirect requests to priming for direct requests ([At] what time do you close?). Again, priming did not differ across question types. We conducted a final analysis that included data from all three experiments plus a large dataset collected using the same procedure. The larger analysis (n > 43,000) confirmed that priming did not differ across sentence types. AcknowledgmentWe thank the many research assistants who assisted with these studies: Rebecca Applebaum, Samirah Artiste, Haley Barash, Mia Carter, Alathea Fairweather, Matthew Gomes, Karina Guenin, Alex Gutowski, Victoria Kolev, Elizabeth Lacy, Mollie Londot, Jordan Madsen, Jennifer Mast, Casey Oberdick, Maria Ribeiro-Siqueira, Kayla Sizemore, Lindsey Summerlin, Kristen Tinnerman, Jeremiah Townsend, Richard Valencia, Olivia Wentworth-Buchanan, Alyssa Westmoreland, Rachel White, Jordan Wiener, and Ashlyn Young.Disclosure statementNo potential conflict of interest was reported by the author(s).Notes1. We analyzed the data with the following model: Dependent Variable ~ Question Type × Sentence + (1 + Question Type | Experimenter).2. Confidence intervals computed using the confint() function in R, based on the model specified in footnote 1.3. The designation “responded to the direct component of the indirect request” should only apply to the conventional and nonconventional indirect requests. However, there were a small number of cases where participants responded to What time do you close? (a direct request) by saying Sure! We close at 9 (or something similar). These were coded as “1” because the participant provided a yes or no answer before responding to the request for information (even though the question did not include a yes or no component).
摘要本文报道了三个实验,评估了直接请求、常规间接请求和非常规间接请求的使用是否会影响问答对话语境中的结构启动。在实验1和2中,实验者给商家打电话,问:“你能告诉我你们什么时候打烊?”(传统的间接请求)或者我可以问一下你们几点打烊吗?(非常规间接请求)。当问题有介词时,参与者比没有介词时更倾向于在他们的回答中产生介词(在9点vs. 9点),这证明了结构启动。实验1和2的结果表明,启动在不同请求类型之间没有统计学差异。在实验3中,我们比较了传统间接请求的启动和直接请求的启动(你什么时候关闭?)。同样,不同问题类型的启动效应没有差异。我们进行了最后的分析,包括所有三个实验的数据以及使用相同程序收集的大型数据集。更大的分析(n > 43,000)证实了启动在句子类型之间没有差异。我们感谢协助这些研究的许多研究助理:丽贝卡·阿普尔鲍姆、萨米拉·阿提斯特、海莉·巴拉什、米娅·卡特、阿拉希娅·费尔韦瑟、马修·戈麦斯、卡琳娜·盖宁、亚历克斯·古托斯基、维多利亚·科列夫、伊丽莎白·莱西、莫莉·朗多、乔丹·马德森、詹妮弗·马斯特、凯西·奥伯迪克、玛丽亚·里贝罗·西奎拉、凯拉·西兹莫尔、林赛·萨默林、克里斯汀·特纳曼、耶利米·汤森、理查德·瓦伦西亚、奥利维亚·温特沃斯-布坎南、阿丽莎·威斯特摩兰、雷切尔·怀特、乔丹·维纳和阿什琳·杨。披露声明作者未报告潜在的利益冲突。我们采用因变量~问题类型×句子+(1 +问题类型|实验者)的模型对数据进行分析。使用R中的contt()函数计算置信区间,基于脚注1.3中指定的模型。“对间接请求的直接组成部分作出答复”的指定应只适用于常规和非常规间接请求。然而,在少数情况下,参与者会回答“你什么时候关门?”(直接请求)说Sure!我们在9点(或类似的时间)关门。这些被编码为“1”,因为参与者在响应信息请求之前提供了一个是或否的答案(即使问题不包括是或否组件)。
{"title":"Structural repetition in responses to indirect requests","authors":"Katherine Chia, Ashley A. Edwards, Christopher Schatschneider, Michael P. Kaschak","doi":"10.1080/0163853x.2023.2255508","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/0163853x.2023.2255508","url":null,"abstract":"ABSTRACTWe report three experiments that assess whether structural priming in a question–answer dialogue context is affected by the use of direct requests, conventional indirect requests, and nonconventional indirect requests. In Experiments 1 and 2, experimenters made phone calls to businesses and asked either Can you tell me (at) what time you close? (conventional indirect request) or May I ask you (at) what time you close? (nonconventional indirect request). Structural priming was demonstrated by participants’ greater tendency to produce a preposition in their response (At 9 vs. 9 o’clock) when the question had a preposition than when it did not. The results of Experiments 1 and 2 suggest that priming is not statistically different across request types. In Experiment 3, we compared priming for the conventional indirect requests to priming for direct requests ([At] what time do you close?). Again, priming did not differ across question types. We conducted a final analysis that included data from all three experiments plus a large dataset collected using the same procedure. The larger analysis (n > 43,000) confirmed that priming did not differ across sentence types. AcknowledgmentWe thank the many research assistants who assisted with these studies: Rebecca Applebaum, Samirah Artiste, Haley Barash, Mia Carter, Alathea Fairweather, Matthew Gomes, Karina Guenin, Alex Gutowski, Victoria Kolev, Elizabeth Lacy, Mollie Londot, Jordan Madsen, Jennifer Mast, Casey Oberdick, Maria Ribeiro-Siqueira, Kayla Sizemore, Lindsey Summerlin, Kristen Tinnerman, Jeremiah Townsend, Richard Valencia, Olivia Wentworth-Buchanan, Alyssa Westmoreland, Rachel White, Jordan Wiener, and Ashlyn Young.Disclosure statementNo potential conflict of interest was reported by the author(s).Notes1. We analyzed the data with the following model: Dependent Variable ~ Question Type × Sentence + (1 + Question Type | Experimenter).2. Confidence intervals computed using the confint() function in R, based on the model specified in footnote 1.3. The designation “responded to the direct component of the indirect request” should only apply to the conventional and nonconventional indirect requests. However, there were a small number of cases where participants responded to What time do you close? (a direct request) by saying Sure! We close at 9 (or something similar). These were coded as “1” because the participant provided a yes or no answer before responding to the request for information (even though the question did not include a yes or no component).","PeriodicalId":11316,"journal":{"name":"Discourse Processes","volume":"32 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-09-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"135203604","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Discourse Processes
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1