Pub Date : 2023-01-01Epub Date: 2023-03-27DOI: 10.1080/0163853x.2023.2185408
Maverick E Smith, Christopher A Kurby, Heather R Bailey
We segment what we read into meaningful events, each separated by a discrete boundary. How does event segmentation during encoding relate to the structure of story information in long-term memory? To evaluate this question, participants read stories of fictional historical events and then engaged in a post-reading verb arrangement task. In this task, participants saw verbs from each of the events placed randomly on a computer screen, and then they arranged the verbs into groups onscreen based on their understanding of the story. Participants who successfully comprehended the story placed verbs from the same event closer to each other than verbs from different events, even after controlling for orthographic, text-based, semantic, and situational overlap between verbs. Thus, how people structure story information into separate events during online comprehension is associated with how that information is stored in memory. Specifically, story information within an event is bound together in memory more so than information between events.
{"title":"Events shape long-term memory for story information.","authors":"Maverick E Smith, Christopher A Kurby, Heather R Bailey","doi":"10.1080/0163853x.2023.2185408","DOIUrl":"10.1080/0163853x.2023.2185408","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>We segment what we read into meaningful events, each separated by a discrete boundary. How does event segmentation during encoding relate to the structure of story information in long-term memory? To evaluate this question, participants read stories of fictional historical events and then engaged in a post-reading verb arrangement task. In this task, participants saw verbs from each of the events placed randomly on a computer screen, and then they arranged the verbs into groups onscreen based on their understanding of the story. Participants who successfully comprehended the story placed verbs from the same event closer to each other than verbs from different events, even after controlling for orthographic, text-based, semantic, and situational overlap between verbs. Thus, how people structure story information into separate events during online comprehension is associated with how that information is stored in memory. Specifically, story information within an event is bound together in memory more so than information between events.</p>","PeriodicalId":11316,"journal":{"name":"Discourse Processes","volume":"60 2","pages":"141-161"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2023-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10343716/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9833709","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 2021-01-01Epub Date: 2020-03-26DOI: 10.1080/0163853X.2020.1734416
Mercedes Spencer, Laurie E Cutting
In the current investigation, we used structural equation mediation modeling to examine the relations between executive function (indexed by measures of working memory, shifting, and inhibition), decoding ability, and reading comprehension in a sample of 298 6- to 8-year-old children (N =132 and 166 for boys and girls, respectively). Results for the full sample indicated that executive function was mediated by decoding ability. When sex was examined as a moderator of these associations, there was evidence for a trend suggesting that direct relations between executive function and reading comprehension were stronger for girls compared to boys; no significant differences were found for other direct and indirect relations. Taken together, these findings highlight the importance of executive function in supporting underlying integrative processes associated with reading comprehension and emphasize the need to further consider the role of executive function in relation to reading.
{"title":"Relations Among Executive Function, Decoding, and Reading Comprehension: An Investigation of Sex Differences.","authors":"Mercedes Spencer, Laurie E Cutting","doi":"10.1080/0163853X.2020.1734416","DOIUrl":"10.1080/0163853X.2020.1734416","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>In the current investigation, we used structural equation mediation modeling to examine the relations between executive function (indexed by measures of working memory, shifting, and inhibition), decoding ability, and reading comprehension in a sample of 298 6- to 8-year-old children (<i>N</i> =132 and 166 for boys and girls, respectively). Results for the full sample indicated that executive function was mediated by decoding ability. When sex was examined as a moderator of these associations, there was evidence for a trend suggesting that direct relations between executive function and reading comprehension were stronger for girls compared to boys; no significant differences were found for other direct and indirect relations. Taken together, these findings highlight the importance of executive function in supporting underlying integrative processes associated with reading comprehension and emphasize the need to further consider the role of executive function in relation to reading.</p>","PeriodicalId":11316,"journal":{"name":"Discourse Processes","volume":"58 1","pages":"42-59"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2021-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC7954233/pdf/nihms-1579172.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"25477449","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 2014-01-01DOI: 10.1080/0163853X.2014.916174
Morton Ann Gernsbacher
Google the question, "How is the Internet changing the way we communicate?," and you will find no shortage of opinions, or fears, about the Internet altering the way we communicate. Although the Internet is not necessarily making communication briefer (neither is the Internet making communication less formal), the Internet is manifesting our preference for writing over speaking. I propose that our preference for communicating through Internet-based text derives from a fundamental feature of writing: In contrast to speech, which is most often synchronous, text is most often asynchronous.
{"title":"Internet-Based Communication.","authors":"Morton Ann Gernsbacher","doi":"10.1080/0163853X.2014.916174","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/0163853X.2014.916174","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Google the question, \"How is the Internet changing the way we communicate?,\" and you will find no shortage of opinions, or fears, about the Internet altering the way we communicate. Although the Internet is not necessarily making communication briefer (neither is the Internet making communication less formal), the Internet is manifesting our preference for writing over speaking. I propose that our preference for communicating through Internet-based text derives from a fundamental feature of writing: In contrast to speech, which is most often synchronous, text is most often asynchronous.</p>","PeriodicalId":11316,"journal":{"name":"Discourse Processes","volume":"51 5-6","pages":"359-373"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2014-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1080/0163853X.2014.916174","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"33970322","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 2013-01-01DOI: 10.1080/0163853X.2013.850604
Charles Clifton, Lyn Frazier
Plural phrases are open to many interpretations in English, where cumulative interpretations of noun and verb phrases are possible without any disambiguating morphology. A sentence like Every week, the high school kids went to the movies or the ballgame might involve quantifying over multiple occurrences of a single scenario, in which subsets of the kids do different things, or it might involve quantifying over distinct scenarios, in which all of the kids do one thing or all of them do the other. In the present work and related earlier work (Harris et al., 2013), we pursue the No Extra Times principle that favors interpretations where a phrase is construed as describing a single event taking place during a given time period. In two written interpretation studies, we found that participants more often interpret indeterminate sentences with disjunctive predicates by partitioning the set of individuals rather than partitioning the predicate to denote distinct scenarios or times. We conclude by offering some speculations about why partitioning the eventuality denoted by the verb phrase into multiple times is more costly than partitioning the entities denoted by its subject noun phrase into multiple sets.
复数短语在英语中有多种解释,其中名词和动词短语的累积解释可能没有任何消除歧义的形态。像这样的句子,每个星期,高中生去看电影或球赛可能涉及到对一个场景的多次出现进行量化,在这个场景中,孩子们的子集做不同的事情,或者它可能涉及到对不同的场景进行量化,在这个场景中,所有的孩子都做一件事,或者所有的孩子都做另一件事。在目前的工作和相关的早期工作中(Harris et al., 2013),我们追求无额外时间原则,该原则支持将短语解释为描述在给定时间段内发生的单个事件的解释。在两项书面解释研究中,我们发现参与者更多地通过划分个体集来解释带有析取谓词的不确定句子,而不是通过划分谓词来表示不同的场景或时间。最后,我们提出了一些推测,为什么将动词短语表示的可能性划分为多个次数比将其主语短语表示的实体划分为多个集合更昂贵。
{"title":"Partition if You Must: Evidence for a No Extra Times Principle.","authors":"Charles Clifton, Lyn Frazier","doi":"10.1080/0163853X.2013.850604","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/0163853X.2013.850604","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Plural phrases are open to many interpretations in English, where cumulative interpretations of noun and verb phrases are possible without any disambiguating morphology. A sentence like <i>Every week, the high school kids went to the movies or the ballgame</i> might involve quantifying over multiple occurrences of a single scenario, in which subsets of the kids do different things, or it might involve quantifying over distinct scenarios, in which all of the kids do one thing or all of them do the other. In the present work and related earlier work (Harris et al., 2013), we pursue the No Extra Times principle that favors interpretations where a phrase is construed as describing a single event taking place during a given time period. In two written interpretation studies, we found that participants more often interpret indeterminate sentences with disjunctive predicates by partitioning the set of individuals rather than partitioning the predicate to denote distinct scenarios or times. We conclude by offering some speculations about why partitioning the eventuality denoted by the verb phrase into multiple times is more costly than partitioning the entities denoted by its subject noun phrase into multiple sets.</p>","PeriodicalId":11316,"journal":{"name":"Discourse Processes","volume":"50 8","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2013-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1080/0163853X.2013.850604","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"32021202","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 2013-01-01DOI: 10.1080/0163853X.2013.778167
Katy Carlson
Three self-paced reading experiments explored the processing of "only" and its interaction with context. In isolated sentences, the focus particle "only" predicts an upcoming contrast. Ambiguous replacive sentences (e.g., "The curator embarrassed the gallery owner in public, not the artist") with "only" on the subject or object showed faster reading of the contrast phrase ("not the artist") than without it. The position of "only" also influenced the phrase's meaning; despite a bias toward object contrasts, subject "only" increased subject interpretations. If preceding context provides another reason for the focus particle, it no longer predicts an upcoming contrast. In biasing contexts including indirect questions, there was no facilitation when "only" marked the argument which answered the question, while "only" on the other argument slowed processing. Both "only" and context influenced interpretation. The results show that focus particles and questions can each influence processing of an upcoming contrast on- and off-line.
三个自定节奏阅读实验探讨了“only”的加工及其与语境的相互作用。在孤立的句子中,焦点词“only”预示着即将到来的对比。用主题或对象上的“only”代替模棱两可的句子(例如,“策展人在公共场合使画廊老板难堪,而不是艺术家”),比不加“only”的对比短语(“not The artist”)的阅读速度更快。“only”的位置也影响了这个短语的意思;尽管偏向于客体对比,但主体“只”增加了主体的解释。如果前面的上下文为焦点粒子提供了另一个原因,它就不再预测即将到来的对比。在有偏见的语境中,包括间接问题,当“only”标记回答问题的论点时,没有促进作用,而“only”标记另一个论点时,则会减慢处理速度。“only”和语境都会影响解释。结果表明,焦点粒子和问题都可以影响即将到来的对比处理。
{"title":"The Role of <i>Only</i> in Contrasts in and out of Context.","authors":"Katy Carlson","doi":"10.1080/0163853X.2013.778167","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/0163853X.2013.778167","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Three self-paced reading experiments explored the processing of \"only\" and its interaction with context. In isolated sentences, the focus particle \"only\" predicts an upcoming contrast. Ambiguous replacive sentences (e.g., \"The curator embarrassed the gallery owner in public, not the artist\") with \"only\" on the subject or object showed faster reading of the contrast phrase (\"not the artist\") than without it. The position of \"only\" also influenced the phrase's meaning; despite a bias toward object contrasts, subject \"only\" increased subject interpretations. If preceding context provides another reason for the focus particle, it no longer predicts an upcoming contrast. In biasing contexts including indirect questions, there was no facilitation when \"only\" marked the argument which answered the question, while \"only\" on the other argument slowed processing. Both \"only\" and context influenced interpretation. The results show that focus particles and questions can each influence processing of an upcoming contrast on- and off-line.</p>","PeriodicalId":11316,"journal":{"name":"Discourse Processes","volume":"50 4","pages":"249-275"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2013-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1080/0163853X.2013.778167","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"32679629","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 2013-01-01Epub Date: 2013-02-07DOI: 10.1080/0163853X.2012.742001
Stephen T Hamilton, Erin M Freed, Debra L Long
The goal of this study was to examine predictions derived from the Lexical Quality Hypothesis (Perfetti & Hart, 2002; Perfetti, 2007) regarding relations among word-decoding, working-memory capacity, and the ability to integrate new concepts into a developing discourse representation. Hierarchical Linear Modeling was used to quantify the effects of two text properties (length and number of new concepts) on reading times of focal and spillover sentences, with variance in those effects estimated as a function of individual difference factors (decoding, vocabulary, print exposure, and working-memory capacity). The analysis revealed complex, cross-level interactions that complement the Lexical Quality Hypothesis.
{"title":"Modeling Reader- and Text- Interactions During Narrative Comprehension: A Test of the Lexical Quality Hypothesis.","authors":"Stephen T Hamilton, Erin M Freed, Debra L Long","doi":"10.1080/0163853X.2012.742001","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/0163853X.2012.742001","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The goal of this study was to examine predictions derived from the Lexical Quality Hypothesis (Perfetti & Hart, 2002; Perfetti, 2007) regarding relations among word-decoding, working-memory capacity, and the ability to integrate new concepts into a developing discourse representation. Hierarchical Linear Modeling was used to quantify the effects of two text properties (length and number of new concepts) on reading times of focal and spillover sentences, with variance in those effects estimated as a function of individual difference factors (decoding, vocabulary, print exposure, and working-memory capacity). The analysis revealed complex, cross-level interactions that complement the Lexical Quality Hypothesis.</p>","PeriodicalId":11316,"journal":{"name":"Discourse Processes","volume":"50 2","pages":"139-163"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2013-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1080/0163853X.2012.742001","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"31426172","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 2012-01-01Epub Date: 2012-05-23DOI: 10.1080/0163853X.2012.654761
Cecilia E Ford, Sandra A Thompson, Veronika Drake
This paper considers points in turn construction where conversation researchers have shown that talk routinely continues beyond possible turn completion, but where we find bodily-visual behavior doing such turn extension work. The bodily-visual behaviors we examine share many features with verbal turn extensions, but we argue that embodied movements have distinct properties that make them well-suited for specific kinds of social action, including stance display and by-play in sequences framed as subsidiary to a simultaneous and related verbal exchange. Our study is in line with a research agenda taking seriously the point made by Goodwin (2000a, b, 2003), Hayashi (2003, 2005), Iwasaki (2009), and others that scholars seeking to account for practices in language and social interaction do themselves a disservice if they privilege the verbal dimension; rather, as suggested in Stivers/Sidnell (2005), each semiotic system/modality, while coordinated with others, has its own organization. With the current exploration of bodily-visual turn extensions, we hope to contribute to a growing understanding of how these different modes of organization are managed concurrently and in concert by interactants in carrying out their everyday social actions.
{"title":"Bodily-visual practices and turn continuation.","authors":"Cecilia E Ford, Sandra A Thompson, Veronika Drake","doi":"10.1080/0163853X.2012.654761","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/0163853X.2012.654761","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>This paper considers points in turn construction where conversation researchers have shown that talk routinely continues beyond possible turn completion, but where we find bodily-visual behavior doing such turn extension work. The bodily-visual behaviors we examine share many features with verbal turn extensions, but we argue that embodied movements have distinct properties that make them well-suited for specific kinds of social action, including stance display and by-play in sequences framed as subsidiary to a simultaneous and related verbal exchange. Our study is in line with a research agenda taking seriously the point made by Goodwin (2000a, b, 2003), Hayashi (2003, 2005), Iwasaki (2009), and others that scholars seeking to account for practices in language and social interaction do themselves a disservice if they privilege the verbal dimension; rather, as suggested in Stivers/Sidnell (2005), each semiotic system/modality, while coordinated with others, has its own organization. With the current exploration of bodily-visual turn extensions, we hope to contribute to a growing understanding of how these different modes of organization are managed concurrently and in concert by interactants in carrying out their everyday social actions.</p>","PeriodicalId":11316,"journal":{"name":"Discourse Processes","volume":"49 3-4","pages":"192-212"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2012-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1080/0163853X.2012.654761","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"31426171","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 2006-01-01DOI: 10.1207/s15326950dp4202_6
Kiel Christianson, Carrick C Williams, Rose T Zacks, Fernanda Ferreira
We report 3 experiments that examined younger and older adults' reliance on "good-enough" interpretations for garden-path sentences (e.g., "While Anna dressed the baby played in the crib") as indicated by their responding "Yes" to questions probing the initial, syntactically unlicensed interpretation (e.g., "Did Anna dress the baby?"). The manipulation of several factors expected to influence the probability of generating or maintaining the unlicensed interpretation resulted in 2 major age differences: Older adults were generally more likely to endorse the incorrect interpretation for sentences containing optionally transitive verbs (e.g., hunted, paid), and they showed decreased availability of the correct interpretation of subordinate clauses containing reflexive absolute transitive verbs (e.g., dress, bathe). These age differences may in part be linked to older adults' increased reliance on heuristic-like good-enough processing to compensate for age-related deficits in working memory capacity. The results support previous studies suggesting that syntactic reanalysis may not be an all-or-nothing process and might not be completed unless questions probing unresolved aspects of the sentence structure challenge the resultant interpretation.
{"title":"Younger and Older Adults' \"Good-Enough\" Interpretations of Garden-Path Sentences.","authors":"Kiel Christianson, Carrick C Williams, Rose T Zacks, Fernanda Ferreira","doi":"10.1207/s15326950dp4202_6","DOIUrl":"10.1207/s15326950dp4202_6","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>We report 3 experiments that examined younger and older adults' reliance on \"good-enough\" interpretations for garden-path sentences (e.g., \"While Anna dressed the baby played in the crib\") as indicated by their responding \"Yes\" to questions probing the initial, syntactically unlicensed interpretation (e.g., \"Did Anna dress the baby?\"). The manipulation of several factors expected to influence the probability of generating or maintaining the unlicensed interpretation resulted in 2 major age differences: Older adults were generally more likely to endorse the incorrect interpretation for sentences containing optionally transitive verbs (e.g., hunted, paid), and they showed decreased availability of the correct interpretation of subordinate clauses containing reflexive absolute transitive verbs (e.g., dress, bathe). These age differences may in part be linked to older adults' increased reliance on heuristic-like good-enough processing to compensate for age-related deficits in working memory capacity. The results support previous studies suggesting that syntactic reanalysis may not be an all-or-nothing process and might not be completed unless questions probing unresolved aspects of the sentence structure challenge the resultant interpretation.</p>","PeriodicalId":11316,"journal":{"name":"Discourse Processes","volume":"42 2","pages":"205-238"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2006-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC1761649/pdf/nihms-13133.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"26531239","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 2004-04-01DOI: 10.1207/s15326950dp3702_1
Ted J M Sanders, Morton Ann Gernsbacher
Accessibility is one of the most important challenges at the intersection of linguistic and psycholinguistic studies of text and discourse processing. Linguists have shown how linguistic indicators of referential coherence show a systematic pattern: Longer linguistic forms (like full lexical NPs) tend to be used when referents are relatively low accessible, shorter forms (pronouns and zero anaphora) are used when referents are highly accessible. This linguistic theory fits in nicely with a dynamic view of text and discourse processing: When a reader proceeds through a text, the activation of concepts as part of the reader's representation fluctuates constantly. Hypotheses considering activation patterns can be tested with on-line research methods like reading time or eye-movement recording. The articles in this special issue show how accessibility phenomena need to be studied from a linguistic and a psycholinguistic angle, and in the latter case from interpretation as well as production.
{"title":"Accessibility in Text and Discourse Processing.","authors":"Ted J M Sanders, Morton Ann Gernsbacher","doi":"10.1207/s15326950dp3702_1","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1207/s15326950dp3702_1","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Accessibility is one of the most important challenges at the intersection of linguistic and psycholinguistic studies of text and discourse processing. Linguists have shown how linguistic indicators of referential coherence show a systematic pattern: Longer linguistic forms (like full lexical NPs) tend to be used when referents are relatively low accessible, shorter forms (pronouns and zero anaphora) are used when referents are highly accessible. This linguistic theory fits in nicely with a dynamic view of text and discourse processing: When a reader proceeds through a text, the activation of concepts as part of the reader's representation fluctuates constantly. Hypotheses considering activation patterns can be tested with on-line research methods like reading time or eye-movement recording. The articles in this special issue show how accessibility phenomena need to be studied from a linguistic and a psycholinguistic angle, and in the latter case from interpretation as well as production.</p>","PeriodicalId":11316,"journal":{"name":"Discourse Processes","volume":"37 2","pages":"79-89"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2004-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1207/s15326950dp3702_1","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"32917134","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 2004-04-01DOI: 10.1207/s15326950dp3702_4
Morton Ann Gernsbacher, Rachel R W Robertson, Paola Palladino, Necia K Werner
Three experiments investigated how readers manage their mental representations during narrative comprehension. The first experiment investigated whether readers' access to their mental representations of the main character in a narrative becomes enhanced (producing a "benefit") when the character is rementioned; the first experiment also investigated whether readers' access to the main character in a narrative becomes weakened or interfered with (producing a "cost") when a new character is introduced. The purpose of the second experiment was to ensure that there was nothing unusually salient about the accessibility of names; thus, we assessed readers' access to an object associated with the main character rather than the character's name. Again, readers demonstrated increased accessibility to the main character when it was rementioned in the narrative, and readers demonstrated reduced accessibility to the main character when a new character was introduced. A third experiment compared more-skilled and less-skilled readers' abilities to manage these mental representations during narrative comprehension. Findings were consistent with research suggesting that more-skilled readers are more skilled at attenuating interfering information (i.e., suppression). Data from all 3 experiments suggest that successful narrative comprehension involves managing mental representations of salient and often times interfering characters.
{"title":"Managing Mental Representations During Narrative Comprehension.","authors":"Morton Ann Gernsbacher, Rachel R W Robertson, Paola Palladino, Necia K Werner","doi":"10.1207/s15326950dp3702_4","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1207/s15326950dp3702_4","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Three experiments investigated how readers manage their mental representations during narrative comprehension. The first experiment investigated whether readers' access to their mental representations of the main character in a narrative becomes enhanced (producing a \"benefit\") when the character is rementioned; the first experiment also investigated whether readers' access to the main character in a narrative becomes weakened or interfered with (producing a \"cost\") when a new character is introduced. The purpose of the second experiment was to ensure that there was nothing unusually salient about the accessibility of names; thus, we assessed readers' access to an object associated with the main character rather than the character's name. Again, readers demonstrated increased accessibility to the main character when it was rementioned in the narrative, and readers demonstrated reduced accessibility to the main character when a new character was introduced. A third experiment compared more-skilled and less-skilled readers' abilities to manage these mental representations during narrative comprehension. Findings were consistent with research suggesting that more-skilled readers are more skilled at attenuating interfering information (i.e., suppression). Data from all 3 experiments suggest that successful narrative comprehension involves managing mental representations of salient and often times interfering characters.</p>","PeriodicalId":11316,"journal":{"name":"Discourse Processes","volume":"37 2","pages":"145-164"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2004-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1207/s15326950dp3702_4","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"32917135","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}