首页 > 最新文献

Discourse Processes最新文献

英文 中文
Events shape long-term memory for story information. 事件会形成对故事信息的长期记忆。
IF 2.2 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EDUCATIONAL Pub Date : 2023-01-01 Epub Date: 2023-03-27 DOI: 10.1080/0163853x.2023.2185408
Maverick E Smith, Christopher A Kurby, Heather R Bailey

We segment what we read into meaningful events, each separated by a discrete boundary. How does event segmentation during encoding relate to the structure of story information in long-term memory? To evaluate this question, participants read stories of fictional historical events and then engaged in a post-reading verb arrangement task. In this task, participants saw verbs from each of the events placed randomly on a computer screen, and then they arranged the verbs into groups onscreen based on their understanding of the story. Participants who successfully comprehended the story placed verbs from the same event closer to each other than verbs from different events, even after controlling for orthographic, text-based, semantic, and situational overlap between verbs. Thus, how people structure story information into separate events during online comprehension is associated with how that information is stored in memory. Specifically, story information within an event is bound together in memory more so than information between events.

我们将阅读内容分割成有意义的事件,每个事件之间都有一个离散的边界。编码过程中的事件分割与长时记忆中的故事信息结构有何关系?为了评估这个问题,参与者阅读了虚构的历史事件故事,然后进行了读后动词排列任务。在这项任务中,受试者看到每个事件中的动词被随机放置在电脑屏幕上,然后他们根据自己对故事的理解将这些动词在屏幕上排列成组。即使在控制了动词之间的正字法、文本、语义和情景重叠之后,成功理解故事的参与者也会将同一事件中的动词排列得比不同事件中的动词更靠近。因此,人们在在线理解过程中如何将故事信息结构化为独立的事件,与如何将这些信息存储在记忆中有关。具体来说,与事件之间的信息相比,事件内部的故事信息在记忆中的结合程度更高。
{"title":"Events shape long-term memory for story information.","authors":"Maverick E Smith, Christopher A Kurby, Heather R Bailey","doi":"10.1080/0163853x.2023.2185408","DOIUrl":"10.1080/0163853x.2023.2185408","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>We segment what we read into meaningful events, each separated by a discrete boundary. How does event segmentation during encoding relate to the structure of story information in long-term memory? To evaluate this question, participants read stories of fictional historical events and then engaged in a post-reading verb arrangement task. In this task, participants saw verbs from each of the events placed randomly on a computer screen, and then they arranged the verbs into groups onscreen based on their understanding of the story. Participants who successfully comprehended the story placed verbs from the same event closer to each other than verbs from different events, even after controlling for orthographic, text-based, semantic, and situational overlap between verbs. Thus, how people structure story information into separate events during online comprehension is associated with how that information is stored in memory. Specifically, story information within an event is bound together in memory more so than information between events.</p>","PeriodicalId":11316,"journal":{"name":"Discourse Processes","volume":"60 2","pages":"141-161"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2023-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10343716/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9833709","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Relations Among Executive Function, Decoding, and Reading Comprehension: An Investigation of Sex Differences. 执行功能、解码和阅读理解之间的关系:性别差异调查
IF 2.2 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EDUCATIONAL Pub Date : 2021-01-01 Epub Date: 2020-03-26 DOI: 10.1080/0163853X.2020.1734416
Mercedes Spencer, Laurie E Cutting

In the current investigation, we used structural equation mediation modeling to examine the relations between executive function (indexed by measures of working memory, shifting, and inhibition), decoding ability, and reading comprehension in a sample of 298 6- to 8-year-old children (N =132 and 166 for boys and girls, respectively). Results for the full sample indicated that executive function was mediated by decoding ability. When sex was examined as a moderator of these associations, there was evidence for a trend suggesting that direct relations between executive function and reading comprehension were stronger for girls compared to boys; no significant differences were found for other direct and indirect relations. Taken together, these findings highlight the importance of executive function in supporting underlying integrative processes associated with reading comprehension and emphasize the need to further consider the role of executive function in relation to reading.

在本次调查中,我们使用了结构方程中介模型,以 298 名 6 至 8 岁儿童为样本(男童和女童的样本数分别为 132 和 166),研究了执行功能(以工作记忆、移位和抑制测量为指标)、解码能力和阅读理解之间的关系。全部样本的结果表明,执行功能受解码能力的影响。当把性别作为这些关联的调节因素进行研究时,有证据表明,与男孩相比,女孩的执行功能与阅读理解能力之间的直接关系更强;在其他直接和间接关系方面,没有发现显著差异。综上所述,这些研究结果突出了执行功能在支持与阅读理解相关的基本综合过程中的重要性,并强调了进一步考虑执行功能在阅读中的作用的必要性。
{"title":"Relations Among Executive Function, Decoding, and Reading Comprehension: An Investigation of Sex Differences.","authors":"Mercedes Spencer, Laurie E Cutting","doi":"10.1080/0163853X.2020.1734416","DOIUrl":"10.1080/0163853X.2020.1734416","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>In the current investigation, we used structural equation mediation modeling to examine the relations between executive function (indexed by measures of working memory, shifting, and inhibition), decoding ability, and reading comprehension in a sample of 298 6- to 8-year-old children (<i>N</i> =132 and 166 for boys and girls, respectively). Results for the full sample indicated that executive function was mediated by decoding ability. When sex was examined as a moderator of these associations, there was evidence for a trend suggesting that direct relations between executive function and reading comprehension were stronger for girls compared to boys; no significant differences were found for other direct and indirect relations. Taken together, these findings highlight the importance of executive function in supporting underlying integrative processes associated with reading comprehension and emphasize the need to further consider the role of executive function in relation to reading.</p>","PeriodicalId":11316,"journal":{"name":"Discourse Processes","volume":"58 1","pages":"42-59"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2021-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC7954233/pdf/nihms-1579172.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"25477449","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Internet-Based Communication. 基于互联网的通信。
IF 2.2 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EDUCATIONAL Pub Date : 2014-01-01 DOI: 10.1080/0163853X.2014.916174
Morton Ann Gernsbacher

Google the question, "How is the Internet changing the way we communicate?," and you will find no shortage of opinions, or fears, about the Internet altering the way we communicate. Although the Internet is not necessarily making communication briefer (neither is the Internet making communication less formal), the Internet is manifesting our preference for writing over speaking. I propose that our preference for communicating through Internet-based text derives from a fundamental feature of writing: In contrast to speech, which is most often synchronous, text is most often asynchronous.

谷歌这个问题:“互联网是如何改变我们的交流方式的?”,你会发现很多关于互联网改变我们交流方式的意见或恐惧。虽然互联网不一定使交流更简短(互联网也没有使交流更不正式),但互联网表明了我们对书写的偏好超过了说话。我认为,我们对通过基于互联网的文本进行交流的偏好源于写作的一个基本特征:与通常是同步的语音相比,文本通常是异步的。
{"title":"Internet-Based Communication.","authors":"Morton Ann Gernsbacher","doi":"10.1080/0163853X.2014.916174","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/0163853X.2014.916174","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Google the question, \"How is the Internet changing the way we communicate?,\" and you will find no shortage of opinions, or fears, about the Internet altering the way we communicate. Although the Internet is not necessarily making communication briefer (neither is the Internet making communication less formal), the Internet is manifesting our preference for writing over speaking. I propose that our preference for communicating through Internet-based text derives from a fundamental feature of writing: In contrast to speech, which is most often synchronous, text is most often asynchronous.</p>","PeriodicalId":11316,"journal":{"name":"Discourse Processes","volume":"51 5-6","pages":"359-373"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2014-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1080/0163853X.2014.916174","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"33970322","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 28
Partition if You Must: Evidence for a No Extra Times Principle. 如果你必须分割:无额外时间原则的证据。
IF 2.2 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EDUCATIONAL Pub Date : 2013-01-01 DOI: 10.1080/0163853X.2013.850604
Charles Clifton, Lyn Frazier

Plural phrases are open to many interpretations in English, where cumulative interpretations of noun and verb phrases are possible without any disambiguating morphology. A sentence like Every week, the high school kids went to the movies or the ballgame might involve quantifying over multiple occurrences of a single scenario, in which subsets of the kids do different things, or it might involve quantifying over distinct scenarios, in which all of the kids do one thing or all of them do the other. In the present work and related earlier work (Harris et al., 2013), we pursue the No Extra Times principle that favors interpretations where a phrase is construed as describing a single event taking place during a given time period. In two written interpretation studies, we found that participants more often interpret indeterminate sentences with disjunctive predicates by partitioning the set of individuals rather than partitioning the predicate to denote distinct scenarios or times. We conclude by offering some speculations about why partitioning the eventuality denoted by the verb phrase into multiple times is more costly than partitioning the entities denoted by its subject noun phrase into multiple sets.

复数短语在英语中有多种解释,其中名词和动词短语的累积解释可能没有任何消除歧义的形态。像这样的句子,每个星期,高中生去看电影或球赛可能涉及到对一个场景的多次出现进行量化,在这个场景中,孩子们的子集做不同的事情,或者它可能涉及到对不同的场景进行量化,在这个场景中,所有的孩子都做一件事,或者所有的孩子都做另一件事。在目前的工作和相关的早期工作中(Harris et al., 2013),我们追求无额外时间原则,该原则支持将短语解释为描述在给定时间段内发生的单个事件的解释。在两项书面解释研究中,我们发现参与者更多地通过划分个体集来解释带有析取谓词的不确定句子,而不是通过划分谓词来表示不同的场景或时间。最后,我们提出了一些推测,为什么将动词短语表示的可能性划分为多个次数比将其主语短语表示的实体划分为多个集合更昂贵。
{"title":"Partition if You Must: Evidence for a No Extra Times Principle.","authors":"Charles Clifton,&nbsp;Lyn Frazier","doi":"10.1080/0163853X.2013.850604","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/0163853X.2013.850604","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Plural phrases are open to many interpretations in English, where cumulative interpretations of noun and verb phrases are possible without any disambiguating morphology. A sentence like <i>Every week, the high school kids went to the movies or the ballgame</i> might involve quantifying over multiple occurrences of a single scenario, in which subsets of the kids do different things, or it might involve quantifying over distinct scenarios, in which all of the kids do one thing or all of them do the other. In the present work and related earlier work (Harris et al., 2013), we pursue the No Extra Times principle that favors interpretations where a phrase is construed as describing a single event taking place during a given time period. In two written interpretation studies, we found that participants more often interpret indeterminate sentences with disjunctive predicates by partitioning the set of individuals rather than partitioning the predicate to denote distinct scenarios or times. We conclude by offering some speculations about why partitioning the eventuality denoted by the verb phrase into multiple times is more costly than partitioning the entities denoted by its subject noun phrase into multiple sets.</p>","PeriodicalId":11316,"journal":{"name":"Discourse Processes","volume":"50 8","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2013-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1080/0163853X.2013.850604","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"32021202","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
The Role of Only in Contrasts in and out of Context. Only在语境内外对比中的作用。
IF 2.2 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EDUCATIONAL Pub Date : 2013-01-01 DOI: 10.1080/0163853X.2013.778167
Katy Carlson

Three self-paced reading experiments explored the processing of "only" and its interaction with context. In isolated sentences, the focus particle "only" predicts an upcoming contrast. Ambiguous replacive sentences (e.g., "The curator embarrassed the gallery owner in public, not the artist") with "only" on the subject or object showed faster reading of the contrast phrase ("not the artist") than without it. The position of "only" also influenced the phrase's meaning; despite a bias toward object contrasts, subject "only" increased subject interpretations. If preceding context provides another reason for the focus particle, it no longer predicts an upcoming contrast. In biasing contexts including indirect questions, there was no facilitation when "only" marked the argument which answered the question, while "only" on the other argument slowed processing. Both "only" and context influenced interpretation. The results show that focus particles and questions can each influence processing of an upcoming contrast on- and off-line.

三个自定节奏阅读实验探讨了“only”的加工及其与语境的相互作用。在孤立的句子中,焦点词“only”预示着即将到来的对比。用主题或对象上的“only”代替模棱两可的句子(例如,“策展人在公共场合使画廊老板难堪,而不是艺术家”),比不加“only”的对比短语(“not The artist”)的阅读速度更快。“only”的位置也影响了这个短语的意思;尽管偏向于客体对比,但主体“只”增加了主体的解释。如果前面的上下文为焦点粒子提供了另一个原因,它就不再预测即将到来的对比。在有偏见的语境中,包括间接问题,当“only”标记回答问题的论点时,没有促进作用,而“only”标记另一个论点时,则会减慢处理速度。“only”和语境都会影响解释。结果表明,焦点粒子和问题都可以影响即将到来的对比处理。
{"title":"The Role of <i>Only</i> in Contrasts in and out of Context.","authors":"Katy Carlson","doi":"10.1080/0163853X.2013.778167","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/0163853X.2013.778167","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Three self-paced reading experiments explored the processing of \"only\" and its interaction with context. In isolated sentences, the focus particle \"only\" predicts an upcoming contrast. Ambiguous replacive sentences (e.g., \"The curator embarrassed the gallery owner in public, not the artist\") with \"only\" on the subject or object showed faster reading of the contrast phrase (\"not the artist\") than without it. The position of \"only\" also influenced the phrase's meaning; despite a bias toward object contrasts, subject \"only\" increased subject interpretations. If preceding context provides another reason for the focus particle, it no longer predicts an upcoming contrast. In biasing contexts including indirect questions, there was no facilitation when \"only\" marked the argument which answered the question, while \"only\" on the other argument slowed processing. Both \"only\" and context influenced interpretation. The results show that focus particles and questions can each influence processing of an upcoming contrast on- and off-line.</p>","PeriodicalId":11316,"journal":{"name":"Discourse Processes","volume":"50 4","pages":"249-275"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2013-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1080/0163853X.2013.778167","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"32679629","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 11
Modeling Reader- and Text- Interactions During Narrative Comprehension: A Test of the Lexical Quality Hypothesis. 叙事理解过程中读者与文本互动的建模:词汇质量假说的检验。
IF 2.2 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EDUCATIONAL Pub Date : 2013-01-01 Epub Date: 2013-02-07 DOI: 10.1080/0163853X.2012.742001
Stephen T Hamilton, Erin M Freed, Debra L Long

The goal of this study was to examine predictions derived from the Lexical Quality Hypothesis (Perfetti & Hart, 2002; Perfetti, 2007) regarding relations among word-decoding, working-memory capacity, and the ability to integrate new concepts into a developing discourse representation. Hierarchical Linear Modeling was used to quantify the effects of two text properties (length and number of new concepts) on reading times of focal and spillover sentences, with variance in those effects estimated as a function of individual difference factors (decoding, vocabulary, print exposure, and working-memory capacity). The analysis revealed complex, cross-level interactions that complement the Lexical Quality Hypothesis.

本研究的目的是检验来自词汇质量假说的预测(Perfetti & Hart, 2002;Perfetti, 2007)关于单词解码,工作记忆容量和将新概念整合到发展中的话语表征中的能力之间的关系。分层线性模型用于量化两种文本属性(长度和新概念的数量)对焦点句和溢出句阅读时间的影响,并将这些影响的方差估计为个体差异因素(解码、词汇量、印刷曝光和工作记忆容量)的函数。分析揭示了复杂的、跨层次的相互作用,这补充了词汇质量假说。
{"title":"Modeling Reader- and Text- Interactions During Narrative Comprehension: A Test of the Lexical Quality Hypothesis.","authors":"Stephen T Hamilton,&nbsp;Erin M Freed,&nbsp;Debra L Long","doi":"10.1080/0163853X.2012.742001","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/0163853X.2012.742001","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The goal of this study was to examine predictions derived from the Lexical Quality Hypothesis (Perfetti & Hart, 2002; Perfetti, 2007) regarding relations among word-decoding, working-memory capacity, and the ability to integrate new concepts into a developing discourse representation. Hierarchical Linear Modeling was used to quantify the effects of two text properties (length and number of new concepts) on reading times of focal and spillover sentences, with variance in those effects estimated as a function of individual difference factors (decoding, vocabulary, print exposure, and working-memory capacity). The analysis revealed complex, cross-level interactions that complement the Lexical Quality Hypothesis.</p>","PeriodicalId":11316,"journal":{"name":"Discourse Processes","volume":"50 2","pages":"139-163"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2013-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1080/0163853X.2012.742001","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"31426172","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 19
Bodily-visual practices and turn continuation. 身体视觉的练习和转身的延续。
IF 2.2 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EDUCATIONAL Pub Date : 2012-01-01 Epub Date: 2012-05-23 DOI: 10.1080/0163853X.2012.654761
Cecilia E Ford, Sandra A Thompson, Veronika Drake

This paper considers points in turn construction where conversation researchers have shown that talk routinely continues beyond possible turn completion, but where we find bodily-visual behavior doing such turn extension work. The bodily-visual behaviors we examine share many features with verbal turn extensions, but we argue that embodied movements have distinct properties that make them well-suited for specific kinds of social action, including stance display and by-play in sequences framed as subsidiary to a simultaneous and related verbal exchange. Our study is in line with a research agenda taking seriously the point made by Goodwin (2000a, b, 2003), Hayashi (2003, 2005), Iwasaki (2009), and others that scholars seeking to account for practices in language and social interaction do themselves a disservice if they privilege the verbal dimension; rather, as suggested in Stivers/Sidnell (2005), each semiotic system/modality, while coordinated with others, has its own organization. With the current exploration of bodily-visual turn extensions, we hope to contribute to a growing understanding of how these different modes of organization are managed concurrently and in concert by interactants in carrying out their everyday social actions.

本文考虑了对话研究人员已经表明,谈话通常会在可能的回合完成后继续进行的回合构建点,但我们发现身体视觉行为在进行这种回合扩展工作。我们研究的身体视觉行为与言语转向扩展有许多共同的特征,但我们认为具身动作具有独特的特性,使它们非常适合于特定类型的社会行为,包括作为同时和相关的言语交换的附属序列的姿态展示和表演。我们的研究与古德温(2000年a、b、2003年)、Hayashi(2003年、2005年)、岩崎(2009年)等人提出的一个研究议程是一致的,即学者们在寻求解释语言和社会互动的实践时,如果他们优先考虑语言维度,那将是一种伤害;相反,正如Stivers/Sidnell(2005)所建议的那样,每个符号系统/形态虽然与其他符号系统/形态相互协调,但都有自己的组织。随着目前对身体-视觉转向延伸的探索,我们希望对这些不同的组织模式如何在进行日常社会活动时同时管理和协调做出贡献。
{"title":"Bodily-visual practices and turn continuation.","authors":"Cecilia E Ford,&nbsp;Sandra A Thompson,&nbsp;Veronika Drake","doi":"10.1080/0163853X.2012.654761","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/0163853X.2012.654761","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>This paper considers points in turn construction where conversation researchers have shown that talk routinely continues beyond possible turn completion, but where we find bodily-visual behavior doing such turn extension work. The bodily-visual behaviors we examine share many features with verbal turn extensions, but we argue that embodied movements have distinct properties that make them well-suited for specific kinds of social action, including stance display and by-play in sequences framed as subsidiary to a simultaneous and related verbal exchange. Our study is in line with a research agenda taking seriously the point made by Goodwin (2000a, b, 2003), Hayashi (2003, 2005), Iwasaki (2009), and others that scholars seeking to account for practices in language and social interaction do themselves a disservice if they privilege the verbal dimension; rather, as suggested in Stivers/Sidnell (2005), each semiotic system/modality, while coordinated with others, has its own organization. With the current exploration of bodily-visual turn extensions, we hope to contribute to a growing understanding of how these different modes of organization are managed concurrently and in concert by interactants in carrying out their everyday social actions.</p>","PeriodicalId":11316,"journal":{"name":"Discourse Processes","volume":"49 3-4","pages":"192-212"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2012-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1080/0163853X.2012.654761","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"31426171","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 56
Younger and Older Adults' "Good-Enough" Interpretations of Garden-Path Sentences. 年轻人和老年人对花园路径句子的 "足够好 "解释。
IF 2.2 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EDUCATIONAL Pub Date : 2006-01-01 DOI: 10.1207/s15326950dp4202_6
Kiel Christianson, Carrick C Williams, Rose T Zacks, Fernanda Ferreira

We report 3 experiments that examined younger and older adults' reliance on "good-enough" interpretations for garden-path sentences (e.g., "While Anna dressed the baby played in the crib") as indicated by their responding "Yes" to questions probing the initial, syntactically unlicensed interpretation (e.g., "Did Anna dress the baby?"). The manipulation of several factors expected to influence the probability of generating or maintaining the unlicensed interpretation resulted in 2 major age differences: Older adults were generally more likely to endorse the incorrect interpretation for sentences containing optionally transitive verbs (e.g., hunted, paid), and they showed decreased availability of the correct interpretation of subordinate clauses containing reflexive absolute transitive verbs (e.g., dress, bathe). These age differences may in part be linked to older adults' increased reliance on heuristic-like good-enough processing to compensate for age-related deficits in working memory capacity. The results support previous studies suggesting that syntactic reanalysis may not be an all-or-nothing process and might not be completed unless questions probing unresolved aspects of the sentence structure challenge the resultant interpretation.

我们报告了 3 个实验,分别考察了年轻人和老年人对花园路径句子(如 "安娜穿衣服的时候,婴儿在摇篮里玩耍")的 "足够好 "解释的依赖性,具体表现为他们对句法上未授权的初始解释(如 "安娜给婴儿穿衣服了吗?")的提问回答 "是"。对预期会影响产生或维持无许可解释的概率的几个因素进行处理后,发现两个主要的年龄差异:对于含有选择及物动词的句子(如 "打猎"、"付钱"),老年人一般更倾向于赞同不正确的解释,而对于含有反身绝对及物动词的从句(如 "穿衣服"、"洗澡"),老年人则表现出正确解释的可用性降低。这些年龄差异的部分原因可能是老年人更依赖于启发式的 "足够好 "处理来弥补与年龄有关的工作记忆能力的不足。这些结果支持了之前的研究,即句法再分析可能不是一个全有或全无的过程,除非有问题对句子结构中尚未解决的方面提出质疑,否则句法再分析可能不会完成。
{"title":"Younger and Older Adults' \"Good-Enough\" Interpretations of Garden-Path Sentences.","authors":"Kiel Christianson, Carrick C Williams, Rose T Zacks, Fernanda Ferreira","doi":"10.1207/s15326950dp4202_6","DOIUrl":"10.1207/s15326950dp4202_6","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>We report 3 experiments that examined younger and older adults' reliance on \"good-enough\" interpretations for garden-path sentences (e.g., \"While Anna dressed the baby played in the crib\") as indicated by their responding \"Yes\" to questions probing the initial, syntactically unlicensed interpretation (e.g., \"Did Anna dress the baby?\"). The manipulation of several factors expected to influence the probability of generating or maintaining the unlicensed interpretation resulted in 2 major age differences: Older adults were generally more likely to endorse the incorrect interpretation for sentences containing optionally transitive verbs (e.g., hunted, paid), and they showed decreased availability of the correct interpretation of subordinate clauses containing reflexive absolute transitive verbs (e.g., dress, bathe). These age differences may in part be linked to older adults' increased reliance on heuristic-like good-enough processing to compensate for age-related deficits in working memory capacity. The results support previous studies suggesting that syntactic reanalysis may not be an all-or-nothing process and might not be completed unless questions probing unresolved aspects of the sentence structure challenge the resultant interpretation.</p>","PeriodicalId":11316,"journal":{"name":"Discourse Processes","volume":"42 2","pages":"205-238"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2006-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC1761649/pdf/nihms-13133.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"26531239","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Accessibility in Text and Discourse Processing. 文本和语篇处理中的可及性。
IF 2.2 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EDUCATIONAL Pub Date : 2004-04-01 DOI: 10.1207/s15326950dp3702_1
Ted J M Sanders, Morton Ann Gernsbacher

Accessibility is one of the most important challenges at the intersection of linguistic and psycholinguistic studies of text and discourse processing. Linguists have shown how linguistic indicators of referential coherence show a systematic pattern: Longer linguistic forms (like full lexical NPs) tend to be used when referents are relatively low accessible, shorter forms (pronouns and zero anaphora) are used when referents are highly accessible. This linguistic theory fits in nicely with a dynamic view of text and discourse processing: When a reader proceeds through a text, the activation of concepts as part of the reader's representation fluctuates constantly. Hypotheses considering activation patterns can be tested with on-line research methods like reading time or eye-movement recording. The articles in this special issue show how accessibility phenomena need to be studied from a linguistic and a psycholinguistic angle, and in the latter case from interpretation as well as production.

可及性是文本和话语处理的语言学和心理语言学交叉研究中最重要的挑战之一。语言学家已经证明了指称连贯性的语言指标是如何显示出一种系统模式的:当指称物相对较难获得时,往往使用较长的语言形式(如完整的词汇NPs),当指称物高度可获得时,则使用较短的语言形式(代词和零回指)。这一语言学理论与文本和话语处理的动态观点非常吻合:当读者继续阅读文本时,作为读者表征的一部分,概念的激活会不断波动。考虑激活模式的假设可以通过在线研究方法进行测试,比如阅读时间或眼球运动记录。本期特刊的文章展示了如何从语言学和心理语言学的角度来研究可达性现象,在后一种情况下,从解释和生产的角度来研究。
{"title":"Accessibility in Text and Discourse Processing.","authors":"Ted J M Sanders,&nbsp;Morton Ann Gernsbacher","doi":"10.1207/s15326950dp3702_1","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1207/s15326950dp3702_1","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Accessibility is one of the most important challenges at the intersection of linguistic and psycholinguistic studies of text and discourse processing. Linguists have shown how linguistic indicators of referential coherence show a systematic pattern: Longer linguistic forms (like full lexical NPs) tend to be used when referents are relatively low accessible, shorter forms (pronouns and zero anaphora) are used when referents are highly accessible. This linguistic theory fits in nicely with a dynamic view of text and discourse processing: When a reader proceeds through a text, the activation of concepts as part of the reader's representation fluctuates constantly. Hypotheses considering activation patterns can be tested with on-line research methods like reading time or eye-movement recording. The articles in this special issue show how accessibility phenomena need to be studied from a linguistic and a psycholinguistic angle, and in the latter case from interpretation as well as production.</p>","PeriodicalId":11316,"journal":{"name":"Discourse Processes","volume":"37 2","pages":"79-89"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2004-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1207/s15326950dp3702_1","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"32917134","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 38
Managing Mental Representations During Narrative Comprehension. 叙事理解过程中的心理表征管理。
IF 2.2 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EDUCATIONAL Pub Date : 2004-04-01 DOI: 10.1207/s15326950dp3702_4
Morton Ann Gernsbacher, Rachel R W Robertson, Paola Palladino, Necia K Werner

Three experiments investigated how readers manage their mental representations during narrative comprehension. The first experiment investigated whether readers' access to their mental representations of the main character in a narrative becomes enhanced (producing a "benefit") when the character is rementioned; the first experiment also investigated whether readers' access to the main character in a narrative becomes weakened or interfered with (producing a "cost") when a new character is introduced. The purpose of the second experiment was to ensure that there was nothing unusually salient about the accessibility of names; thus, we assessed readers' access to an object associated with the main character rather than the character's name. Again, readers demonstrated increased accessibility to the main character when it was rementioned in the narrative, and readers demonstrated reduced accessibility to the main character when a new character was introduced. A third experiment compared more-skilled and less-skilled readers' abilities to manage these mental representations during narrative comprehension. Findings were consistent with research suggesting that more-skilled readers are more skilled at attenuating interfering information (i.e., suppression). Data from all 3 experiments suggest that successful narrative comprehension involves managing mental representations of salient and often times interfering characters.

三个实验研究了读者在叙事理解过程中如何管理他们的心理表征。第一个实验调查了当角色被提及时,读者对叙事中主角的心理表征是否会增强(产生“好处”);第一个实验还调查了当一个新角色出现时,读者对故事中主要角色的接触是否会减弱或受到干扰(产生“成本”)。第二个实验的目的是确保名字的可及性没有什么特别突出的地方;因此,我们评估的是读者对与主角有关的物品的接触程度,而不是人物的名字。同样,当主角在叙述中被提及时,读者表现出对主角的亲和力,而当新角色出现时,读者表现出对主角的亲和力。第三个实验比较了熟练读者和不熟练读者在叙事理解过程中处理这些心理表征的能力。这一发现与先前的研究一致,即越熟练的读者越擅长衰减干扰信息(即抑制)。所有三个实验的数据都表明,成功的叙事理解涉及到对重要人物的心理表征的管理,而这些人物往往是干扰性的。
{"title":"Managing Mental Representations During Narrative Comprehension.","authors":"Morton Ann Gernsbacher,&nbsp;Rachel R W Robertson,&nbsp;Paola Palladino,&nbsp;Necia K Werner","doi":"10.1207/s15326950dp3702_4","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1207/s15326950dp3702_4","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Three experiments investigated how readers manage their mental representations during narrative comprehension. The first experiment investigated whether readers' access to their mental representations of the main character in a narrative becomes enhanced (producing a \"benefit\") when the character is rementioned; the first experiment also investigated whether readers' access to the main character in a narrative becomes weakened or interfered with (producing a \"cost\") when a new character is introduced. The purpose of the second experiment was to ensure that there was nothing unusually salient about the accessibility of names; thus, we assessed readers' access to an object associated with the main character rather than the character's name. Again, readers demonstrated increased accessibility to the main character when it was rementioned in the narrative, and readers demonstrated reduced accessibility to the main character when a new character was introduced. A third experiment compared more-skilled and less-skilled readers' abilities to manage these mental representations during narrative comprehension. Findings were consistent with research suggesting that more-skilled readers are more skilled at attenuating interfering information (i.e., suppression). Data from all 3 experiments suggest that successful narrative comprehension involves managing mental representations of salient and often times interfering characters.</p>","PeriodicalId":11316,"journal":{"name":"Discourse Processes","volume":"37 2","pages":"145-164"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2004-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1207/s15326950dp3702_4","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"32917135","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 40
期刊
Discourse Processes
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1