首页 > 最新文献

Discourse Processes最新文献

英文 中文
Anaphoric reference to mereological entities 语义实体的回指
IF 2.2 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EDUCATIONAL Pub Date : 2023-03-16 DOI: 10.1080/0163853X.2023.2197682
DERYA ÇOKAL, R. Filik, P. Sturt, Massimo Poesio
ABSTRACT Corpus evidence suggests that in contexts in which the presence of multiple antecedents might favor plural reference, the disadvantage observed for singular reference may disappear if the potential antecedents are combined in a group-like plural entity. We examined the relative salience of antecedents in conditions where the context either made a group interpretation available (i.e., mereological entity) (e.g., The engineer hooked up the engine to the boxcar …, where group = train), or not (e.g., The engineer detached the engine from the boxcar …). Results from three experiments in which participants were asked to identify referents for singular versus plural pronouns (Experiment 1), to confirm the referents of pronouns in a sentence completion task (Experiment 2), and to provide paraphrases for given texts (Experiment 3), collectively provided evidence that the creation of a group makes that entity (i) a possible referent for singular anaphoric reference and (ii) more salient than its constituents.
摘要语料库证据表明,在存在多个前因可能有利于复数指称的情况下,如果潜在的前因组合成一个类似群体的复数实体,则单数指称的缺点可能会消失。我们研究了在上下文提供了群体解释(即表面实体)(例如,工程师将发动机连接到棚车上…,其中群体=火车)或没有(例如,工程人员将发动机从棚车上拆下…)的情况下,前因的相对显著性。来自三个实验的结果,在这三个实验中,参与者被要求识别单数代词和复数代词的指代(实验1),在句子完成任务中确认代词的指涉(实验2),并为给定的文本提供转述(实验3),集体提供的证据表明,一个群体的创建使该实体(i)成为单数回指指称的可能指称对象,(ii)比其组成部分更突出。
{"title":"Anaphoric reference to mereological entities","authors":"DERYA ÇOKAL, R. Filik, P. Sturt, Massimo Poesio","doi":"10.1080/0163853X.2023.2197682","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/0163853X.2023.2197682","url":null,"abstract":"ABSTRACT Corpus evidence suggests that in contexts in which the presence of multiple antecedents might favor plural reference, the disadvantage observed for singular reference may disappear if the potential antecedents are combined in a group-like plural entity. We examined the relative salience of antecedents in conditions where the context either made a group interpretation available (i.e., mereological entity) (e.g., The engineer hooked up the engine to the boxcar …, where group = train), or not (e.g., The engineer detached the engine from the boxcar …). Results from three experiments in which participants were asked to identify referents for singular versus plural pronouns (Experiment 1), to confirm the referents of pronouns in a sentence completion task (Experiment 2), and to provide paraphrases for given texts (Experiment 3), collectively provided evidence that the creation of a group makes that entity (i) a possible referent for singular anaphoric reference and (ii) more salient than its constituents.","PeriodicalId":11316,"journal":{"name":"Discourse Processes","volume":"60 1","pages":"202 - 223"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2023-03-16","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"43522233","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Representation of predictive inferences when multiple alternatives are available 当有多个备选方案可用时,预测推断的表示
IF 2.2 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EDUCATIONAL Pub Date : 2023-03-16 DOI: 10.1080/0163853X.2023.2196915
Edward A. Cranford, Jarrod Moss
ABSTRACT When a situation could lead to multiple mutually exclusive consequences, recent research shows that people automatically generate multiple predictive inferences in memory. Several theoretical mechanisms have been proposed to account for the generation of predictive inferences. One hypothesis is that inferences are minimally encoded, represented only by a set of semantic features related to the inferences or by a more general concept that covers the consequences of both inferences. A second hypothesis is that activation of the inferences is delayed because it is initially weak and requires time to build. In three experiments designed to examine these two hypotheses, participants read narrative passages that supported mutually exclusive consequences. Predictive inferences were not detected in working memory using a word-naming task, even at longer delays, but were detected using a short-term contradiction paradigm and, therefore, available as part of the discourse representation. The combined results indicate that predictive inferences are minimally encoded rather than their activation being delayed.
摘要最近的研究表明,当一种情况可能导致多种相互排斥的后果时,人们会在记忆中自动产生多种预测推断。已经提出了几种理论机制来解释预测推断的产生。一种假设是,推理是最低限度编码的,仅由与推理相关的一组语义特征表示,或由涵盖两种推理结果的更一般的概念表示。第二个假设是,推断的激活被延迟,因为它最初很弱,需要时间来构建。在三个旨在检验这两个假设的实验中,参与者阅读了支持互斥结果的叙事段落。使用单词命名任务在工作记忆中没有检测到预测推断,即使是在较长的延迟下,但使用短期矛盾范式检测到了预测推断,因此可以作为话语表征的一部分。组合结果表明,预测推断是最低限度地编码的,而不是它们的激活被延迟。
{"title":"Representation of predictive inferences when multiple alternatives are available","authors":"Edward A. Cranford, Jarrod Moss","doi":"10.1080/0163853X.2023.2196915","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/0163853X.2023.2196915","url":null,"abstract":"ABSTRACT When a situation could lead to multiple mutually exclusive consequences, recent research shows that people automatically generate multiple predictive inferences in memory. Several theoretical mechanisms have been proposed to account for the generation of predictive inferences. One hypothesis is that inferences are minimally encoded, represented only by a set of semantic features related to the inferences or by a more general concept that covers the consequences of both inferences. A second hypothesis is that activation of the inferences is delayed because it is initially weak and requires time to build. In three experiments designed to examine these two hypotheses, participants read narrative passages that supported mutually exclusive consequences. Predictive inferences were not detected in working memory using a word-naming task, even at longer delays, but were detected using a short-term contradiction paradigm and, therefore, available as part of the discourse representation. The combined results indicate that predictive inferences are minimally encoded rather than their activation being delayed.","PeriodicalId":11316,"journal":{"name":"Discourse Processes","volume":"60 1","pages":"181 - 201"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2023-03-16","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"42212526","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Comprehension of explicit and implicit information in prereaders: the role of maternal education, receptive vocabulary, executive functions, and theory of mind 前读者对外显和内隐信息的理解:母亲教育、接受性词汇、执行功能和心理理论的作用
IF 2.2 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EDUCATIONAL Pub Date : 2023-03-16 DOI: 10.1080/0163853X.2023.2185406
Raffaele Dicataldo, Ughetta Moscardino, I. Mammarella, Maja Roch
ABSTRACT Listening narrative comprehension is a complex process that requires the processing of explicit (i.e., information presented in the text) and implicit information (i.e., information inferable from the text) and involves several linguistic and cognitive skills. However, the specific role of these skills in children’s comprehension remains unclear. This study investigated the contribution of maternal education, receptive vocabulary, executive functions (working memory, inhibitory control and cognitive flexibility), and Theory of Mind to the comprehension of explicit and implicit information during a listening comprehension task among 100 Italian native speakers attending first grade (M age = 6.5 years, SD = 3.7). Hierarchical linear regression analyses indicated that maternal education and children’s verbal skills were positively associated with comprehension of explicit information, whereas cognitive flexibility and Theory of Mind provided an independent contribution to the comprehension of implicit information over and above maternal education and verbal skills. Prereaders not only process different types of information during a listening comprehension task and engage in integrative processes to go beyond the information presented in the text but also rely on different linguistic and cognitive skills in the comprehension of both explicit and implicit information.
听力叙事理解是一个复杂的过程,需要对显性信息(即语篇所呈现的信息)和隐性信息(即从语篇推断出的信息)进行加工,涉及多种语言和认知技能。然而,这些技能在儿童理解中的具体作用尚不清楚。本研究考察了母亲教育、接受性词汇、执行功能(工作记忆、抑制控制和认知灵活性)和心理理论对100名一年级意大利语母语者(M年龄= 6.5岁,SD = 3.7)听力理解任务中外显信息和内隐信息理解的贡献。层次线性回归分析表明,母亲教育程度和言语能力对儿童外显信息的理解有显著的正向影响,而认知灵活性和心理理论对儿童内隐信息的理解有独立的影响,而母亲教育程度和言语能力对儿童内隐信息的理解有独立的影响。预读者在听力理解任务中不仅处理不同类型的信息,参与超越文本所呈现信息的整合过程,而且在理解显性和隐性信息时依赖不同的语言和认知技能。
{"title":"Comprehension of explicit and implicit information in prereaders: the role of maternal education, receptive vocabulary, executive functions, and theory of mind","authors":"Raffaele Dicataldo, Ughetta Moscardino, I. Mammarella, Maja Roch","doi":"10.1080/0163853X.2023.2185406","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/0163853X.2023.2185406","url":null,"abstract":"ABSTRACT Listening narrative comprehension is a complex process that requires the processing of explicit (i.e., information presented in the text) and implicit information (i.e., information inferable from the text) and involves several linguistic and cognitive skills. However, the specific role of these skills in children’s comprehension remains unclear. This study investigated the contribution of maternal education, receptive vocabulary, executive functions (working memory, inhibitory control and cognitive flexibility), and Theory of Mind to the comprehension of explicit and implicit information during a listening comprehension task among 100 Italian native speakers attending first grade (M age = 6.5 years, SD = 3.7). Hierarchical linear regression analyses indicated that maternal education and children’s verbal skills were positively associated with comprehension of explicit information, whereas cognitive flexibility and Theory of Mind provided an independent contribution to the comprehension of implicit information over and above maternal education and verbal skills. Prereaders not only process different types of information during a listening comprehension task and engage in integrative processes to go beyond the information presented in the text but also rely on different linguistic and cognitive skills in the comprehension of both explicit and implicit information.","PeriodicalId":11316,"journal":{"name":"Discourse Processes","volume":"60 1","pages":"163 - 180"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2023-03-16","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"43887913","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Is viewing a painting like reading a story?: Trans-symbolic comprehension processes and aesthetic responses across two media 看一幅画就像读一个故事吗?跨媒介的跨符号理解过程与审美反应
IF 2.2 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EDUCATIONAL Pub Date : 2023-02-07 DOI: 10.1080/0163853X.2023.2172299
Christian C. Steciuch, Keith K. Millis, Ryan D. Kopatich
ABSTRACT A large body of research has outlined how mental models are formed by comprehending texts, yet relatively less work has been conducted in the field of comprehending artworks. Trans-symbolic comprehension (TSC) processes have been theorized to partially account for how mental models are formed across media. The current study tested whether participants use these processes similarly across reading stories and viewing paintings. The current study also tested whether the frequencies of TSC processes predict the aesthetic responses of understanding, interest, and pleasure. Participants typed out their thoughts while viewing paintings and reading texts. The think-aloud protocols were then parsed and coded for TSC processes. Results indicated similarities in TSC processes across both texts and paintings; however, the association between the TSC processes and aesthetic responses were greater for the texts than for the paintings. Overall, the results provide support for the TSC framework.
摘要大量的研究已经概述了如何通过理解文本来形成心理模型,但在理解艺术品领域进行的工作相对较少。跨符号理解(TSC)过程已被理论化,部分解释了心理模型是如何在媒体上形成的。目前的研究测试了参与者在阅读故事和观看绘画时是否类似地使用这些过程。目前的研究还测试了TSC过程的频率是否可以预测理解、兴趣和愉悦的审美反应。参与者在观看绘画和阅读文本时,把自己的想法打出来。然后,为TSC进程解析并编码了“大声思考”协议。结果表明,文本和绘画的TSC过程相似;然而,文本的TSC过程和美学反应之间的联系大于绘画。总体而言,结果为TSC框架提供了支持。
{"title":"Is viewing a painting like reading a story?: Trans-symbolic comprehension processes and aesthetic responses across two media","authors":"Christian C. Steciuch, Keith K. Millis, Ryan D. Kopatich","doi":"10.1080/0163853X.2023.2172299","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/0163853X.2023.2172299","url":null,"abstract":"ABSTRACT A large body of research has outlined how mental models are formed by comprehending texts, yet relatively less work has been conducted in the field of comprehending artworks. Trans-symbolic comprehension (TSC) processes have been theorized to partially account for how mental models are formed across media. The current study tested whether participants use these processes similarly across reading stories and viewing paintings. The current study also tested whether the frequencies of TSC processes predict the aesthetic responses of understanding, interest, and pleasure. Participants typed out their thoughts while viewing paintings and reading texts. The think-aloud protocols were then parsed and coded for TSC processes. Results indicated similarities in TSC processes across both texts and paintings; however, the association between the TSC processes and aesthetic responses were greater for the texts than for the paintings. Overall, the results provide support for the TSC framework.","PeriodicalId":11316,"journal":{"name":"Discourse Processes","volume":"60 1","pages":"97 - 118"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2023-02-07","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"48912832","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Discourse Processes Adopts the Transparency and Openness Promotion Guidelines 话语过程采用透明和开放促进准则
IF 2.2 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EDUCATIONAL Pub Date : 2023-02-07 DOI: 10.1080/0163853x.2023.2185400
Johanna K. Kaakinen, Catherine M. Bohn-Gettler
ABSTRACT The Discourse Processes editorial team has decided to adopt the Transparency and Openness Promotion guidelines. The purpose of the guidelines is to help researchers, journal editors, and reviewers adopt practices that support transparency, openness, and reproducibility of research. The guidelines include eight transparency standards regarding (1) citations of data and materials, (2) data sharing, (3) analytic methods, (4) research materials, (5) design and analysis methods, (6) preregistration of the study, (7) preregistration of the analysis plan, and (8) replication studies. We describe each standard, how they are implemented, and what it means from the perspective of an author who wishes to publish their work in Discourse Processes.
摘要《话语过程》编辑团队决定采用《透明度和开放性促进指南》。该指南的目的是帮助研究人员、期刊编辑和审稿人采用支持研究透明度、公开性和可重复性的做法。该指南包括八项透明度标准,涉及(1)数据和材料的引用,(2)数据共享,(3)分析方法,(4)研究材料,(5)设计和分析方法,以及(6)研究的预注册,(7)分析计划的预注册和(8)复制研究。我们从希望在话语过程中发表作品的作者的角度描述了每一个标准,它们是如何实施的,以及它意味着什么。
{"title":"Discourse Processes Adopts the Transparency and Openness Promotion Guidelines","authors":"Johanna K. Kaakinen, Catherine M. Bohn-Gettler","doi":"10.1080/0163853x.2023.2185400","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/0163853x.2023.2185400","url":null,"abstract":"ABSTRACT The Discourse Processes editorial team has decided to adopt the Transparency and Openness Promotion guidelines. The purpose of the guidelines is to help researchers, journal editors, and reviewers adopt practices that support transparency, openness, and reproducibility of research. The guidelines include eight transparency standards regarding (1) citations of data and materials, (2) data sharing, (3) analytic methods, (4) research materials, (5) design and analysis methods, (6) preregistration of the study, (7) preregistration of the analysis plan, and (8) replication studies. We describe each standard, how they are implemented, and what it means from the perspective of an author who wishes to publish their work in Discourse Processes.","PeriodicalId":11316,"journal":{"name":"Discourse Processes","volume":"60 1","pages":"93 - 96"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2023-02-07","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"44249764","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Reading perspectives moderate text-belief consistency effects in eye movements and comprehension 阅读视角在眼动和理解中调节文本信念一致性效应
IF 2.2 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EDUCATIONAL Pub Date : 2023-02-06 DOI: 10.1080/0163853X.2023.2172300
Johanna Abendroth, Tobias Richter
ABSTRACT Readers often prioritize processing and comprehension of information perceived as relevant to a particular intention. Using a repeated-measurement study, we investigated how readers’ prior beliefs and external reading perspectives influence processing and comprehension of belief-relevant texts on two socioscientific controversies. University students read belief-relevant texts from a belief-consistent perspective in one experimental session and from a belief-inconsistent reading perspective in another. Eye tracking was used to measure immediate and delayed processing and a sentence verification task was used to measure comprehension. Results revealed longer first-pass reading times for belief-inconsistent claims compared to belief-consistent claims, especially in the belief-inconsistent reading perspective. Longer lookbacks on belief-consistent claims were found in the belief-consistent reading perspective but similar lookback times for both types of claims in the belief-inconsistent reading perspective. We further found better comprehension for belief-consistent information in the belief-consistent reading perspective but balanced comprehension levels in the belief-inconsistent reading perspective.
读者通常优先处理和理解与特定意图相关的信息。通过重复测量研究,我们调查了读者的先验信念和外部阅读视角如何影响两个社会科学争议的信念相关文本的加工和理解。大学生在一个实验阶段从信念一致的角度阅读与信念相关的文本,在另一个实验阶段从信念不一致的角度阅读与信念相关的文本。用眼动追踪来衡量即时和延迟处理,用句子验证任务来衡量理解。结果显示,与信念一致的说法相比,信念不一致的说法的第一次阅读时间更长,尤其是在信念不一致的阅读角度。在信念一致的阅读视角下,对信念一致的观点的回顾时间更长,而在信念不一致的阅读视角下,两种观点的回顾时间相似。我们进一步发现,在信念一致的阅读视角下,学生对信念一致的信息有更好的理解,而在信念不一致的阅读视角下,学生的理解水平保持平衡。
{"title":"Reading perspectives moderate text-belief consistency effects in eye movements and comprehension","authors":"Johanna Abendroth, Tobias Richter","doi":"10.1080/0163853X.2023.2172300","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/0163853X.2023.2172300","url":null,"abstract":"ABSTRACT Readers often prioritize processing and comprehension of information perceived as relevant to a particular intention. Using a repeated-measurement study, we investigated how readers’ prior beliefs and external reading perspectives influence processing and comprehension of belief-relevant texts on two socioscientific controversies. University students read belief-relevant texts from a belief-consistent perspective in one experimental session and from a belief-inconsistent reading perspective in another. Eye tracking was used to measure immediate and delayed processing and a sentence verification task was used to measure comprehension. Results revealed longer first-pass reading times for belief-inconsistent claims compared to belief-consistent claims, especially in the belief-inconsistent reading perspective. Longer lookbacks on belief-consistent claims were found in the belief-consistent reading perspective but similar lookback times for both types of claims in the belief-inconsistent reading perspective. We further found better comprehension for belief-consistent information in the belief-consistent reading perspective but balanced comprehension levels in the belief-inconsistent reading perspective.","PeriodicalId":11316,"journal":{"name":"Discourse Processes","volume":"60 1","pages":"119 - 140"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2023-02-06","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"47379413","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Sounding others’ sensations in interaction 在互动中倾听他人的感受
IF 2.2 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EDUCATIONAL Pub Date : 2023-01-02 DOI: 10.1080/0163853X.2023.2165027
L. Keevallik, E. Hofstetter, A. Weatherall, S. Wiggins
ABSTRACT This study investigates the practice of “sounding for others,” wherein one person vocalizes to enact someone else’s putatively ongoing bodily sensation. We argue that it constitutes a collaborative way of performing sensorial experiences. Examples include producing cries with others’ strain or pain and parents sounding an mmm of gustatory pleasure on their infant’s behalf. Vocal sounds, their loudness, and duration are specifically deployed for instructing bodily experiences during novices’ real-time performance of various activities, such as tasting food for the first time or straining during a Pilates exercise. Vocalizations that are indexically tied to the body provide immediate displays of understanding and empathy that may be explicated further through lexicon. The existence of this practice challenges the conceptualization of communication as a transfer of information from an individual agent – even regarding assumedly individual body sensations – instead providing evidence of the joint nature of action and supporting dialogic theories of communication, including when language-marginal vocalizations are used.
摘要本研究调查了“为他人发声”的实践,即一个人发声以产生他人假定的持续身体感觉。我们认为,它构成了一种进行感官体验的合作方式。例如,伴随着他人的紧张或痛苦而哭泣,父母代表婴儿发出阵阵味觉上的快乐。人声、音量和持续时间专门用于指导新手实时进行各种活动时的身体体验,例如第一次品尝食物或在普拉提运动中紧张。与身体指数相关的发音提供了理解和同理心的即时表现,可以通过词典进一步解释。这种实践的存在挑战了将沟通概念化为来自个体主体的信息传递——即使是关于假定的个体身体感觉——而不是提供行动的共同性质的证据,并支持沟通的对话理论,包括当使用语言边缘发声时。
{"title":"Sounding others’ sensations in interaction","authors":"L. Keevallik, E. Hofstetter, A. Weatherall, S. Wiggins","doi":"10.1080/0163853X.2023.2165027","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/0163853X.2023.2165027","url":null,"abstract":"ABSTRACT This study investigates the practice of “sounding for others,” wherein one person vocalizes to enact someone else’s putatively ongoing bodily sensation. We argue that it constitutes a collaborative way of performing sensorial experiences. Examples include producing cries with others’ strain or pain and parents sounding an mmm of gustatory pleasure on their infant’s behalf. Vocal sounds, their loudness, and duration are specifically deployed for instructing bodily experiences during novices’ real-time performance of various activities, such as tasting food for the first time or straining during a Pilates exercise. Vocalizations that are indexically tied to the body provide immediate displays of understanding and empathy that may be explicated further through lexicon. The existence of this practice challenges the conceptualization of communication as a transfer of information from an individual agent – even regarding assumedly individual body sensations – instead providing evidence of the joint nature of action and supporting dialogic theories of communication, including when language-marginal vocalizations are used.","PeriodicalId":11316,"journal":{"name":"Discourse Processes","volume":"60 1","pages":"73 - 91"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2023-01-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"46861718","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 3
The role of relevance determinations in multiple text reading and writing: an investigation of the MD-TRACE 相关性决定在多语篇阅读和写作中的作用——对MD-TRACE的调查
IF 2.2 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EDUCATIONAL Pub Date : 2023-01-02 DOI: 10.1080/0163853X.2022.2159741
Hye Yeon Lee, Alexandra List
ABSTRACT This study examined the role of relevance determinations within the context of undergraduates’ multiple text reading and writing. In this study, undergraduate students were randomly assigned to one of two experimental conditions (i.e., to compose a research report about either the causes of or the solutions to the urban housing crisis), using a library of 12 digital texts (six more relevant and six less relevant to students’ assigned task condition). Guided by the Multiple Documents Task-Based Relevance Assessment and Content Extraction (MD-TRACE) model, we identified the key features that students included in their task models, used log data to profile students’ text selection justifications and navigation, and categorized students’ writing as task-relevant or not. As such, we found students’ relevance determinations to play a key role in forming task models prior to text access, selecting and navigating texts during multiple text use, and composing a task product after accessing multiple texts. While we did not find students’ initial task models to be associated with their patterns of text selection justifications or navigation nor with writing performance, we did find differences in writing performance across students belonging to different text selection justification and navigation profiles. Implications for theory and research on learning from multiple texts are discussed.
摘要本研究考察了关联决定在大学生多文本阅读和写作中的作用。在这项研究中,本科生被随机分配到两个实验条件中的一个(即,撰写一份关于城市住房危机的原因或解决方案的研究报告),使用一个由12篇数字文本组成的图书馆(与学生分配的任务条件相关6篇,不相关6篇)。在基于多文档任务的相关性评估和内容提取(MD-TRACE)模型的指导下,我们确定了学生在任务模型中包含的关键特征,使用日志数据来描述学生的文本选择理由和导航,并将学生的写作分为任务相关或非任务相关。因此,我们发现学生的相关性决定在文本访问前形成任务模型、在多个文本使用过程中选择和导航文本以及在访问多个文本后编写任务产品方面发挥着关键作用。虽然我们没有发现学生的初始任务模型与他们的文本选择理由或导航模式有关,也没有发现与写作表现有关,但我们确实发现,属于不同文本选择理由和导航档案的学生在写作表现上存在差异。讨论了从多个文本中学习对理论和研究的启示。
{"title":"The role of relevance determinations in multiple text reading and writing: an investigation of the MD-TRACE","authors":"Hye Yeon Lee, Alexandra List","doi":"10.1080/0163853X.2022.2159741","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/0163853X.2022.2159741","url":null,"abstract":"ABSTRACT This study examined the role of relevance determinations within the context of undergraduates’ multiple text reading and writing. In this study, undergraduate students were randomly assigned to one of two experimental conditions (i.e., to compose a research report about either the causes of or the solutions to the urban housing crisis), using a library of 12 digital texts (six more relevant and six less relevant to students’ assigned task condition). Guided by the Multiple Documents Task-Based Relevance Assessment and Content Extraction (MD-TRACE) model, we identified the key features that students included in their task models, used log data to profile students’ text selection justifications and navigation, and categorized students’ writing as task-relevant or not. As such, we found students’ relevance determinations to play a key role in forming task models prior to text access, selecting and navigating texts during multiple text use, and composing a task product after accessing multiple texts. While we did not find students’ initial task models to be associated with their patterns of text selection justifications or navigation nor with writing performance, we did find differences in writing performance across students belonging to different text selection justification and navigation profiles. Implications for theory and research on learning from multiple texts are discussed.","PeriodicalId":11316,"journal":{"name":"Discourse Processes","volume":"112 23","pages":"42 - 72"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2023-01-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41264712","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Events shape long-term memory for story information. 事件会形成对故事信息的长期记忆。
IF 2.2 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EDUCATIONAL Pub Date : 2023-01-01 Epub Date: 2023-03-27 DOI: 10.1080/0163853x.2023.2185408
Maverick E Smith, Christopher A Kurby, Heather R Bailey

We segment what we read into meaningful events, each separated by a discrete boundary. How does event segmentation during encoding relate to the structure of story information in long-term memory? To evaluate this question, participants read stories of fictional historical events and then engaged in a post-reading verb arrangement task. In this task, participants saw verbs from each of the events placed randomly on a computer screen, and then they arranged the verbs into groups onscreen based on their understanding of the story. Participants who successfully comprehended the story placed verbs from the same event closer to each other than verbs from different events, even after controlling for orthographic, text-based, semantic, and situational overlap between verbs. Thus, how people structure story information into separate events during online comprehension is associated with how that information is stored in memory. Specifically, story information within an event is bound together in memory more so than information between events.

我们将阅读内容分割成有意义的事件,每个事件之间都有一个离散的边界。编码过程中的事件分割与长时记忆中的故事信息结构有何关系?为了评估这个问题,参与者阅读了虚构的历史事件故事,然后进行了读后动词排列任务。在这项任务中,受试者看到每个事件中的动词被随机放置在电脑屏幕上,然后他们根据自己对故事的理解将这些动词在屏幕上排列成组。即使在控制了动词之间的正字法、文本、语义和情景重叠之后,成功理解故事的参与者也会将同一事件中的动词排列得比不同事件中的动词更靠近。因此,人们在在线理解过程中如何将故事信息结构化为独立的事件,与如何将这些信息存储在记忆中有关。具体来说,与事件之间的信息相比,事件内部的故事信息在记忆中的结合程度更高。
{"title":"Events shape long-term memory for story information.","authors":"Maverick E Smith, Christopher A Kurby, Heather R Bailey","doi":"10.1080/0163853x.2023.2185408","DOIUrl":"10.1080/0163853x.2023.2185408","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>We segment what we read into meaningful events, each separated by a discrete boundary. How does event segmentation during encoding relate to the structure of story information in long-term memory? To evaluate this question, participants read stories of fictional historical events and then engaged in a post-reading verb arrangement task. In this task, participants saw verbs from each of the events placed randomly on a computer screen, and then they arranged the verbs into groups onscreen based on their understanding of the story. Participants who successfully comprehended the story placed verbs from the same event closer to each other than verbs from different events, even after controlling for orthographic, text-based, semantic, and situational overlap between verbs. Thus, how people structure story information into separate events during online comprehension is associated with how that information is stored in memory. Specifically, story information within an event is bound together in memory more so than information between events.</p>","PeriodicalId":11316,"journal":{"name":"Discourse Processes","volume":"60 2","pages":"141-161"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2023-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10343716/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9833709","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
In Pursuit of a Good Conversation: How Contribution Balance, Common Ground, and Conversational Closings Influence Conversation Assessment and Conversational Memory 追求良好的会话:贡献平衡、共同点和会话结束如何影响会话评估和会话记忆
IF 2.2 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EDUCATIONAL Pub Date : 2022-12-12 DOI: 10.1080/0163853X.2022.2152552
Andrew J. Guydish, J. E. Fox Tree
ABSTRACT How do people determine whether a conversation is good or bad? Do conversational phenomena (reaching common ground, striving to contribute equally, successful conversational closings) influence judgments of conversation quality and recall of conversations? We tested whether individuals reading previously transcribed conversations considered psycholinguistic characteristics in their assessments of whether the conversations were good or bad. Additionally, we tested whether these assessments influenced how the conversations were remembered. Well-formed interactions (balanced, grounded, or with well-structured closings) were rated as better than ill-formed counterparts (not balanced, not well grounded, or with poorly structured closings). When recalling the best interaction they saw, participants chose a well-formed conversation about 80% of the time. When recalling the worst interaction they saw, they chose an ill-formed conversation about 90% of the time. Balance information was important to both judgments. Participants recognized well-balanced conversations more accurately and were also faster to recognize well-balanced conversations. In contrast, participants recognized ill-formed grounding better, although it took more time to do so. Well-formed and ill-formed closings were recognized to a similar degree, but improperly structured closings were recognized more quickly. These findings support the hypothesis that common ground, contribution balance, and conversational closings influence both perception of conversational quality and memory for previously transcribed conversations.
人们如何判断一段对话是好是坏?会话现象(达成共识、努力平等地贡献、成功地结束会话)会影响对会话质量的判断和对会话的回忆吗?我们测试了阅读先前记录对话的个体在评估对话是好是坏时是否考虑了心理语言特征。此外,我们还测试了这些评估是否会影响对话的记忆方式。格式良好的交互(平衡的、基础的或结构良好的结尾)被评为比格式不良的交互(不平衡的、基础的或结构不良的结尾)更好。当回忆他们看到的最好的互动时,80%的参与者选择了一个形式良好的对话。当回忆他们看到的最糟糕的互动时,90%的人选择了形式不佳的对话。平衡信息对两种判断都很重要。参与者更准确地识别出平衡良好的对话,也更快地识别出平衡良好的对话。相比之下,参与者更能识别出错误的接地,尽管需要更多的时间。格式良好和格式不良的闭包被识别的程度相似,但结构不正确的闭包被识别得更快。这些发现支持了一个假设,即共同点、贡献平衡和会话结束都会影响对会话质量的感知和对先前转录的会话的记忆。
{"title":"In Pursuit of a Good Conversation: How Contribution Balance, Common Ground, and Conversational Closings Influence Conversation Assessment and Conversational Memory","authors":"Andrew J. Guydish, J. E. Fox Tree","doi":"10.1080/0163853X.2022.2152552","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/0163853X.2022.2152552","url":null,"abstract":"ABSTRACT How do people determine whether a conversation is good or bad? Do conversational phenomena (reaching common ground, striving to contribute equally, successful conversational closings) influence judgments of conversation quality and recall of conversations? We tested whether individuals reading previously transcribed conversations considered psycholinguistic characteristics in their assessments of whether the conversations were good or bad. Additionally, we tested whether these assessments influenced how the conversations were remembered. Well-formed interactions (balanced, grounded, or with well-structured closings) were rated as better than ill-formed counterparts (not balanced, not well grounded, or with poorly structured closings). When recalling the best interaction they saw, participants chose a well-formed conversation about 80% of the time. When recalling the worst interaction they saw, they chose an ill-formed conversation about 90% of the time. Balance information was important to both judgments. Participants recognized well-balanced conversations more accurately and were also faster to recognize well-balanced conversations. In contrast, participants recognized ill-formed grounding better, although it took more time to do so. Well-formed and ill-formed closings were recognized to a similar degree, but improperly structured closings were recognized more quickly. These findings support the hypothesis that common ground, contribution balance, and conversational closings influence both perception of conversational quality and memory for previously transcribed conversations.","PeriodicalId":11316,"journal":{"name":"Discourse Processes","volume":"60 1","pages":"18 - 41"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2022-12-12","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"44665032","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
期刊
Discourse Processes
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1