首页 > 最新文献

Discourse Processes最新文献

英文 中文
Demo “but”-prefaced responses to inquiry in Japanese 用日语示范用“但是”开头回答问题
2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EDUCATIONAL Pub Date : 2023-09-18 DOI: 10.1080/0163853x.2023.2255501
Yuki Arita
ABSTRACTThis conversation analytic study investigates the use of the Japanese contrastive marker demo “but” as a preface to responses to polar questions. Demo-prefaced responses are one type of nonconforming answers, that is, responses that provide (dis)affirmation to preceding questions without yes/no-tokens. This study explores how question recipients treat the preceding questions with their demo-prefaced responses. The data analysis is twofold: this research first examines the turn-initial demo to scrutinize basic properties of demo-prefacing in responses to polar questions; then the study explores how the basic properties are in effect with a + demo-prefacing (i.e., demo-prefacing preceded by the turn-initial particle a), the largest set of combined turn-beginning elements involving demo in the database. This study reveals that demo-prefacing fundamentally serves to proffer qualification upon implied affirmation. When used with a, demo registers question recipients’ implied acknowledgment of the legitimacy of the question’s presupposition while projecting disaffirmation of the inquired proposition. Disclosure statementNo potential conflict of interest was reported by the author(s).Appendix 1Transcript symbolsTableDownload CSVDisplay TableAbbreviationsTableDownload CSVDisplay TableNotes1. The English translation for demo is provided in the title of this article for the benefit of readers with no Japanese language background. It is not the author’s intention to suggest an absolute correspondence between demo and but.2. A similar function has been reported for the English but as well. Schiffrin (Citation1987) identifies but as a point-making device. She observes that speakers use but to return to a prior concern that had been interrupted, misunderstood, and/or challenged during activities of storytelling or argument building. See also Choe and Reddington (Citation2018), who examine the use of but-prefacing as a practice of refocusing on the main course of action in public discourse.3. As discussed later, however, certain properties of demo-prefacing to disagreeing turns which Mori (Citation1999) documents are also observed in demo-prefaced question responses in this study.4. Iya is one of the Japanese disaffirming tokens. Kushida (Citation2005) points out that while iya can be translated into “no,” iya cannot be used as a free-standing particle and thus has functions broader than those of the English “no.” Kushida further shows that iya can preface both conforming and nonconforming responses to polar questions. In the present study, iya + demo-prefacing always introduces nonconforming responses (n = 7). The possible interactional correlation of demo-prefacing with iya-prefacing should be investigated with more examples.5. The particle n (or un, nn) is an informal form of an affirmation particle hai “yes.” The affirmation particle n is typically pronounced with a falling pitch. On the contrary, the particle n placed before demo is elongated and p
摘要本会话分析研究考察了日语对比标记“但是”作为极性问题回答的开场白。演示开头的回答是一种非一致性回答,也就是说,对前面的问题提供(非)肯定的回答,而不使用是/否标记。本研究探讨了问题接受者如何用他们的示范开场白来对待前面的问题。数据分析是双重的:本研究首先考察了转向-初始演示,以考察极性问题回答中演示开头的基本性质;然后,研究探讨了在数据库中涉及demo的最大组合turn- starting元素集a + demo-prefacing(即,demo-prefacing前面有turn-initial particle a)下,基本属性是如何生效的。本研究揭示了演示序语的基本作用是对默示肯定提供限定条件。当与a连用时,演示表达了问题接受者对问题预设的合法性的隐含承认,同时投射出对被询问命题的否定。披露声明作者未报告潜在的利益冲突。附录1Transcript symbolsTableDownload CSVDisplay table缩写stabledownload CSVDisplay TableNotes1。为了方便没有日语背景的读者,本文的标题中提供了demo的英文翻译。作者无意建议demo和but之间有绝对的对应关系。据报道,英语中也有类似的功能。Schiffrin (Citation1987)认为but是一种表达观点的手段。她观察到,讲话者在讲故事或建立论点的过程中使用but来回到先前被打断、误解和/或挑战的问题。另见Choe和Reddington (Citation2018),他们研究了使用但前缀作为一种重新关注公共话语中的主要行动过程的实践。然而,正如稍后讨论的那样,Mori (Citation1999)文献中提到的,在本研究中也可以在演示开头的问题回答中观察到不同意转折的演示开头的某些特性。Iya是日本人否定的象征之一。Kushida (Citation2005)指出,虽然iya可以翻译成“不”,但iya不能作为一个独立的粒子使用,因此它的功能比英语中的“不”更广泛。Kushida进一步表明,对于极性问题,iya可以为符合和不符合的回答做序。在本研究中,iya + demo-prefacing总是会引入不符合的响应(n = 7), demo-prefacing与iya-prefacing之间可能存在的交互关系有待更多的实例研究。助词n(或un, nn)是肯定助词hai“yes”的非正式形式。肯定助词n的发音通常是降调的。相反,放置在demo之前的粒子n被拉长,产生一个平坦的音高,它通常用作占位符而不是肯定令牌。虽然不同情况下,伸长率n:的长度会有轻微的变化,但在本表中总结为n:。此数据集仅包括a紧跟着demo而中间没有停顿的匝数。换句话说,a和demo是作为一个回合结构单元和由同一扬声器产生的后续回合组件一起生产的。因此,焦点a +演示序可以被视为“词汇化预制件”之一,即“由多个单词组成的半固定形式,但作为一个韵律整体传递”(Thompson et al., Citation2015, p. 80)。在英语中,“不”是用来对否定疑问句做出肯定的回答(例如:A:你不会说日语吗?B:没有。)。8。摘要(1)中以demo开头的转折也是用kedo设计的,它被放置在转折的最后(即,demo mukashi hodo dewa nai kedo ne“demo they aren’t as much as they were before”在第6行)。Ono等人(Citation2012)指出,转折的最后粒子kedo让接受者理解其对比语义的含义,而这些含义本来是在主句中呈现的。在Rie的演示开头的转折中,转折结尾的kedo(随后是话语结尾的助词ne)似乎也有助于表明她对先前问题的隐含肯定。Mori (Citation1999)也观察到在不同意的回合中使用turn-final kedo,认为它有助于减轻分歧或暗示部分同意。本研究选择在涉及demo的其他回合开始元素中检查+ demo前置,主要有两个原因。
{"title":"<i>Demo</i> “but”-prefaced responses to inquiry in Japanese","authors":"Yuki Arita","doi":"10.1080/0163853x.2023.2255501","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/0163853x.2023.2255501","url":null,"abstract":"ABSTRACTThis conversation analytic study investigates the use of the Japanese contrastive marker demo “but” as a preface to responses to polar questions. Demo-prefaced responses are one type of nonconforming answers, that is, responses that provide (dis)affirmation to preceding questions without yes/no-tokens. This study explores how question recipients treat the preceding questions with their demo-prefaced responses. The data analysis is twofold: this research first examines the turn-initial demo to scrutinize basic properties of demo-prefacing in responses to polar questions; then the study explores how the basic properties are in effect with a + demo-prefacing (i.e., demo-prefacing preceded by the turn-initial particle a), the largest set of combined turn-beginning elements involving demo in the database. This study reveals that demo-prefacing fundamentally serves to proffer qualification upon implied affirmation. When used with a, demo registers question recipients’ implied acknowledgment of the legitimacy of the question’s presupposition while projecting disaffirmation of the inquired proposition. Disclosure statementNo potential conflict of interest was reported by the author(s).Appendix 1Transcript symbolsTableDownload CSVDisplay TableAbbreviationsTableDownload CSVDisplay TableNotes1. The English translation for demo is provided in the title of this article for the benefit of readers with no Japanese language background. It is not the author’s intention to suggest an absolute correspondence between demo and but.2. A similar function has been reported for the English but as well. Schiffrin (Citation1987) identifies but as a point-making device. She observes that speakers use but to return to a prior concern that had been interrupted, misunderstood, and/or challenged during activities of storytelling or argument building. See also Choe and Reddington (Citation2018), who examine the use of but-prefacing as a practice of refocusing on the main course of action in public discourse.3. As discussed later, however, certain properties of demo-prefacing to disagreeing turns which Mori (Citation1999) documents are also observed in demo-prefaced question responses in this study.4. Iya is one of the Japanese disaffirming tokens. Kushida (Citation2005) points out that while iya can be translated into “no,” iya cannot be used as a free-standing particle and thus has functions broader than those of the English “no.” Kushida further shows that iya can preface both conforming and nonconforming responses to polar questions. In the present study, iya + demo-prefacing always introduces nonconforming responses (n = 7). The possible interactional correlation of demo-prefacing with iya-prefacing should be investigated with more examples.5. The particle n (or un, nn) is an informal form of an affirmation particle hai “yes.” The affirmation particle n is typically pronounced with a falling pitch. On the contrary, the particle n placed before demo is elongated and p","PeriodicalId":11316,"journal":{"name":"Discourse Processes","volume":"32 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-09-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"135203609","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The role of social status in sarcasm interpretation: evidence from the United Kingdom and China 社会地位在讽刺语解释中的作用:来自英国和中国的证据
2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EDUCATIONAL Pub Date : 2023-09-12 DOI: 10.1080/0163853x.2023.2252695
Ning Zhu, Ruth Filik
{"title":"The role of social status in sarcasm interpretation: evidence from the United Kingdom and China","authors":"Ning Zhu, Ruth Filik","doi":"10.1080/0163853x.2023.2252695","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/0163853x.2023.2252695","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":11316,"journal":{"name":"Discourse Processes","volume":"61 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-09-12","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"135877983","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Exploring the spatial gradient effect in narratives 探索叙事中的空间梯度效应
IF 2.2 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EDUCATIONAL Pub Date : 2023-08-09 DOI: 10.1080/0163853X.2023.2245313
Emily R. Smith, R. B. Lea, E. J. O'Brien
ABSTRACT The current set of experiments was designed to explore the processing of spatial information during reading, specifically the spatial-shift effect and the spatial-gradient effect. Experiments 1 and 2 demonstrated that when participants were presented with text alone (i.e., without prior map memorization, virtual environments, task demands, or prior knowledge of the spatial information), the spatial-shift effect does emerge. Using the time to name a spatial location as a measure of spatial activation, participant naming times were faster for the original spatial location when the protagonist had moved a smaller distance relative to when the protagonist had moved a larger distance. Further, Experiment 3 provided evidence to support that a spatial gradient of activation is evident; this was done by adding an additional point of spatial distance in which the protagonist did not move from the initial location. The results are discussed within the context of the memory-based view of text comprehension.
本实验旨在探讨阅读过程中空间信息的加工,特别是空间位移效应和空间梯度效应。实验1和2表明,当参与者单独面对文本时(即,没有事先的地图记忆、虚拟环境、任务要求或对空间信息的先验知识),空间转移效应确实出现了。使用命名空间位置的时间作为空间激活的衡量标准,当主角移动的距离较小时,参与者命名原始空间位置的时间要比主角移动的距离较大时快。此外,实验3提供了证据支持空间梯度的激活是明显的;这是通过添加一个额外的空间距离点来实现的,在这个空间距离点中主角不会从初始位置移动。研究结果将在基于记忆的文本理解观点的背景下进行讨论。
{"title":"Exploring the spatial gradient effect in narratives","authors":"Emily R. Smith, R. B. Lea, E. J. O'Brien","doi":"10.1080/0163853X.2023.2245313","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/0163853X.2023.2245313","url":null,"abstract":"ABSTRACT The current set of experiments was designed to explore the processing of spatial information during reading, specifically the spatial-shift effect and the spatial-gradient effect. Experiments 1 and 2 demonstrated that when participants were presented with text alone (i.e., without prior map memorization, virtual environments, task demands, or prior knowledge of the spatial information), the spatial-shift effect does emerge. Using the time to name a spatial location as a measure of spatial activation, participant naming times were faster for the original spatial location when the protagonist had moved a smaller distance relative to when the protagonist had moved a larger distance. Further, Experiment 3 provided evidence to support that a spatial gradient of activation is evident; this was done by adding an additional point of spatial distance in which the protagonist did not move from the initial location. The results are discussed within the context of the memory-based view of text comprehension.","PeriodicalId":11316,"journal":{"name":"Discourse Processes","volume":"60 1","pages":"522 - 533"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2023-08-09","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"45352440","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Emotional shifts, event-congruent emotions, and transportation in narrative persuasion 叙事说服中的情感转移、事件一致性情感和传递
2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EDUCATIONAL Pub Date : 2023-08-09 DOI: 10.1080/0163853x.2023.2252696
Marie-Luise C. R. Schmidt, Julia R. Winkler, Markus Appel, Tobias Richter
Emotional shifts in stories are assumed to contribute to narrative persuasion by enhancing engagement with the story. This effect might depend on the congruency of audiences’ emotional experience to the emotions implied by the story. In two experiments with wellcontrolled story manipulations, we compared the persuasive effect of stories with shifts in valence (from positive to negative to positive) to continuously positive story versions and examined moderating influences of event-congruent emotions and narrative transportation. The positive story versions were consistently more persuasive than the versions with emotional shifts. Transportation increased the persuasive effect of the stories, but only in audiences that listened to the shifting stories. In both emotional story trajectories, event-congruent emotional experience enhanced persuasion. We discuss our findings in terms of boundary conditions of the effect of emotional shifts in narrative persuasion.
故事中的情感转变被认为有助于通过增强故事的吸引力来说服叙事。这种效果可能取决于观众的情感体验与故事所暗示的情感的一致性。在两个控制良好的故事操作实验中,我们比较了效价(从积极到消极再到积极)转变到持续积极故事版本的故事的说服效果,并考察了事件一致情绪和叙事转移的调节作用。积极的故事版本始终比情绪变化的版本更有说服力。交通工具增加了故事的说服力,但只对那些听了变化故事的观众有效。在两种情感故事轨迹中,事件一致的情感体验增强了说服力。我们从叙事说服中情感转移效应的边界条件的角度讨论了我们的发现。
{"title":"Emotional shifts, event-congruent emotions, and transportation in narrative persuasion","authors":"Marie-Luise C. R. Schmidt, Julia R. Winkler, Markus Appel, Tobias Richter","doi":"10.1080/0163853x.2023.2252696","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/0163853x.2023.2252696","url":null,"abstract":"Emotional shifts in stories are assumed to contribute to narrative persuasion by enhancing engagement with the story. This effect might depend on the congruency of audiences’ emotional experience to the emotions implied by the story. In two experiments with wellcontrolled story manipulations, we compared the persuasive effect of stories with shifts in valence (from positive to negative to positive) to continuously positive story versions and examined moderating influences of event-congruent emotions and narrative transportation. The positive story versions were consistently more persuasive than the versions with emotional shifts. Transportation increased the persuasive effect of the stories, but only in audiences that listened to the shifting stories. In both emotional story trajectories, event-congruent emotional experience enhanced persuasion. We discuss our findings in terms of boundary conditions of the effect of emotional shifts in narrative persuasion.","PeriodicalId":11316,"journal":{"name":"Discourse Processes","volume":"101 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-08-09","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"135745924","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Politeness and the communication of uncertainty when breaking bad news 礼貌与突发坏消息时的不确定性沟通
IF 2.2 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EDUCATIONAL Pub Date : 2023-08-09 DOI: 10.1080/0163853X.2023.2245310
Harry T. Clelland, M. Haigh
ABSTRACT Uncertain language can be used to express genuine uncertainty but can also be used to manage face (e.g., by softening bad news). These conflicting motivations can create ambiguity in health communication. In this preregistered two-part experiment, participants assumed the position of a health specialist and wrote a letter communicating either a certain or an uncertain medical diagnosis. This was addressed to either a patient (high face threat) or the patient’s family doctor (low face threat). Letters written under high face threat contained more words and more dispreferred markers (e.g., sorry, unfortunately) than those written under low face threat. The number of explicit hedges (e.g., possibly, maybe) did not differ as a function of face threat. Time taken to write the letters was elevated only in the condition where face threat was high and the diagnosis was uncertain, suggesting that the joint pressures of communicating uncertain information in a tactful way increased the task demands. Our data demonstrate that participants spontaneously produced dispreferred markers (but not explicit hedges) to manage face and that face management is more taxing under uncertainty. Ratings from a second set of participants indicate that face management strategies did not affect the perceived meaning or manner of the message. For open materials, data, and code, see https://doi.org/10.17605/OSF.IO/ZU2AN.
摘要不确定语言可以用来表达真正的不确定性,但也可以用来管理面子(例如,软化坏消息)。这些相互冲突的动机可能会在健康沟通中产生歧义。在这个预先注册的由两部分组成的实验中,参与者扮演了健康专家的角色,并写了一封信,传达了某种或不确定的医学诊断。这是针对患者(高脸威胁)或患者的家庭医生(低脸威胁)。在高脸威胁下写的信比在低脸威胁下的信包含更多的单词和更多的反驳标记(例如,对不起,不幸的是)。明确对冲的数量(例如,可能,可能)与面部威胁的函数没有差异。只有在面部威胁较高且诊断不确定的情况下,写信所需的时间才会增加,这表明以委婉的方式传达不确定信息的共同压力增加了任务需求。我们的数据表明,参与者自发地产生了反驳标记(但不是明确的对冲)来管理面部,并且面部管理在不确定性下更为繁重。第二组参与者的评分表明,面部管理策略不会影响信息的感知意义或方式。有关打开的材质、数据和代码,请参见https://doi.org/10.17605/OSF.IO/ZU2AN.
{"title":"Politeness and the communication of uncertainty when breaking bad news","authors":"Harry T. Clelland, M. Haigh","doi":"10.1080/0163853X.2023.2245310","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/0163853X.2023.2245310","url":null,"abstract":"ABSTRACT Uncertain language can be used to express genuine uncertainty but can also be used to manage face (e.g., by softening bad news). These conflicting motivations can create ambiguity in health communication. In this preregistered two-part experiment, participants assumed the position of a health specialist and wrote a letter communicating either a certain or an uncertain medical diagnosis. This was addressed to either a patient (high face threat) or the patient’s family doctor (low face threat). Letters written under high face threat contained more words and more dispreferred markers (e.g., sorry, unfortunately) than those written under low face threat. The number of explicit hedges (e.g., possibly, maybe) did not differ as a function of face threat. Time taken to write the letters was elevated only in the condition where face threat was high and the diagnosis was uncertain, suggesting that the joint pressures of communicating uncertain information in a tactful way increased the task demands. Our data demonstrate that participants spontaneously produced dispreferred markers (but not explicit hedges) to manage face and that face management is more taxing under uncertainty. Ratings from a second set of participants indicate that face management strategies did not affect the perceived meaning or manner of the message. For open materials, data, and code, see https://doi.org/10.17605/OSF.IO/ZU2AN.","PeriodicalId":11316,"journal":{"name":"Discourse Processes","volume":"60 1","pages":"479 - 501"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2023-08-09","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"49502914","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Families’ discursive practices in data discussions about migration histories 家庭在移民史数据讨论中的话语实践
IF 2.2 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EDUCATIONAL Pub Date : 2023-07-03 DOI: 10.1080/0163853X.2023.2225981
Daryl Axelrod, J. Kahn
ABSTRACT Large-scale data and data visualizations are ubiquitous now in the stories that shape our society. In particular, these stories influence youth and families’ communication and understanding of scientific, social, and personal issues. Consequently, we need to better understand how youth and families can engage and learn with the tools that generate such narratives. This study reports on a qualitative analysis of 13 data discussions with families that used data visualization tools to explore georeferenced data connected to their family migration histories. We developed the Co-Construct, Situate, Advance a Theory (CSAT) model, from family cases, which details the discursive and interactional processes by which families tell personal stories grounded in data displays. Extending earlier studies of storytelling as theory-building in family conversations, our model provides an understanding of families’ discursive practices around data exploration. We discuss implications for family learning with data technologies.
摘要大规模数据和数据可视化现在在塑造我们社会的故事中无处不在。特别是,这些故事影响了青年和家庭对科学、社会和个人问题的沟通和理解。因此,我们需要更好地了解青年和家庭如何利用产生这种叙事的工具进行参与和学习。本研究报告了对13个家庭数据讨论的定性分析,这些家庭使用数据可视化工具来探索与其家庭迁移历史相关的地理参考数据。我们根据家庭案例开发了共建、情境、推进理论(CSAT)模型,该模型详细描述了家庭根据数据显示讲述个人故事的话语和互动过程。我们的模型扩展了早期关于讲故事作为家庭对话理论构建的研究,提供了对家庭围绕数据探索的话语实践的理解。我们讨论了数据技术对家庭学习的影响。
{"title":"Families’ discursive practices in data discussions about migration histories","authors":"Daryl Axelrod, J. Kahn","doi":"10.1080/0163853X.2023.2225981","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/0163853X.2023.2225981","url":null,"abstract":"ABSTRACT Large-scale data and data visualizations are ubiquitous now in the stories that shape our society. In particular, these stories influence youth and families’ communication and understanding of scientific, social, and personal issues. Consequently, we need to better understand how youth and families can engage and learn with the tools that generate such narratives. This study reports on a qualitative analysis of 13 data discussions with families that used data visualization tools to explore georeferenced data connected to their family migration histories. We developed the Co-Construct, Situate, Advance a Theory (CSAT) model, from family cases, which details the discursive and interactional processes by which families tell personal stories grounded in data displays. Extending earlier studies of storytelling as theory-building in family conversations, our model provides an understanding of families’ discursive practices around data exploration. We discuss implications for family learning with data technologies.","PeriodicalId":11316,"journal":{"name":"Discourse Processes","volume":"60 1","pages":"457 - 477"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2023-07-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"47978476","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Correction 校正
IF 2.2 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EDUCATIONAL Pub Date : 2023-07-03 DOI: 10.1080/0163853x.2023.2250644
D. Axelrod, J. Kahn
{"title":"Correction","authors":"D. Axelrod, J. Kahn","doi":"10.1080/0163853x.2023.2250644","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/0163853x.2023.2250644","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":11316,"journal":{"name":"Discourse Processes","volume":"60 1","pages":"I - I"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2023-07-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"43907568","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Developmental differences in children’s generation of knowledge-based inferences 儿童知识推理生成的发展差异
IF 2.2 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EDUCATIONAL Pub Date : 2023-07-03 DOI: 10.1080/0163853X.2023.2225980
Nicola K. Currie, K. Cain
ABSTRACT We examined knowledge-based inference in 6-, 8- and 10-year-olds. Participants listened to texts where the number of clues for an inference was manipulated and then judged whether single-word probes (target inference, competing inference, literal word from the text and an unrelated concept) were related to the story. Accuracy and response times were analyzed with mixed-effects models. Inference accuracy was higher with three clues, and older children responded to the inference more accurately and quickly than younger children. The inference and literal probes were compared: 10-year-olds responded more accurately and quickly to the inference; however, 6-year-olds were more accurate for the literal. We propose that by 10-years of age, children are able to activate knowledge-based inferences while listening to text. All age groups had some inhibition difficulty, with less accurate and slower responses for the competing inference than the unrelated concept. Inference instruction should be sensitive to developmental differences in inferential processing.
摘要:我们研究了6岁、8岁和10岁儿童的基于知识的推理。参与者听文本,其中推理的线索数量被操纵,然后判断单个单词的探针(目标推理、竞争推理、文本中的字面单词和一个不相关的概念)是否与故事有关。使用混合效应模型分析准确度和响应时间。三条线索的推理准确率更高,年龄较大的儿童对推理的反应比年龄较小的儿童更准确、更快。对推理和字面调查进行了比较:10岁的人对推理的反应更准确、更快;然而,6岁的孩子在字面上更准确。我们建议,到10岁时,孩子们能够在听课文时激活基于知识的推理。所有年龄组都有一些抑制困难,对竞争推理的反应不如对无关概念的反应准确和缓慢。推理教学应该对推理过程中的发展差异保持敏感。
{"title":"Developmental differences in children’s generation of knowledge-based inferences","authors":"Nicola K. Currie, K. Cain","doi":"10.1080/0163853X.2023.2225980","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/0163853X.2023.2225980","url":null,"abstract":"ABSTRACT We examined knowledge-based inference in 6-, 8- and 10-year-olds. Participants listened to texts where the number of clues for an inference was manipulated and then judged whether single-word probes (target inference, competing inference, literal word from the text and an unrelated concept) were related to the story. Accuracy and response times were analyzed with mixed-effects models. Inference accuracy was higher with three clues, and older children responded to the inference more accurately and quickly than younger children. The inference and literal probes were compared: 10-year-olds responded more accurately and quickly to the inference; however, 6-year-olds were more accurate for the literal. We propose that by 10-years of age, children are able to activate knowledge-based inferences while listening to text. All age groups had some inhibition difficulty, with less accurate and slower responses for the competing inference than the unrelated concept. Inference instruction should be sensitive to developmental differences in inferential processing.","PeriodicalId":11316,"journal":{"name":"Discourse Processes","volume":"60 1","pages":"440 - 456"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2023-07-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"45614116","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Anchoring your bridge: the importance of paraphrasing to inference making in self-explanations 锚定你的桥梁:自我解释中转述对推理的重要性
IF 2.2 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EDUCATIONAL Pub Date : 2023-05-28 DOI: 10.1080/0163853X.2023.2225757
D. McNamara, Natalie Newton, Katerina Christhilf, Kathryn S. McCarthy, Joseph P. Magliano, L. Allen
ABSTRACT Analyzing constructed responses, such as think-alouds or self-explanations, can reveal valuable information about readers’ comprehension strategies. The current study expands on the extant work by (1) investigating combinations and patterns of comprehension strategies that readers use and (2) examining the extent to which these patterns relate to individual differences and comprehension outcomes. We leveraged archival data from three datasets (n = 472) to examine how comprehension strategy use varied across datasets, texts, and populations (high school, undergraduate). Students’ self-explanations were coded for strategy use and then further analyzed in terms of combinations and patterns of strategies. Our analyses revealed that almost all readers primarily engaged in paraphrasing and/or the combination of paraphrasing and bridging, with few instances of elaboration. Further, the combination of paraphrasing and bridging was the best predictor of performance on a comprehension test. In terms of patterns, switching between strategies was not correlated to reading comprehension and was negatively correlated with the combination of paraphrasing and bridging. Understanding which strategy combinations and patterns are optimal can be used to inform adaptive instruction and feedback that can aid in more individualized support for readers.
摘要分析构建的回答,如思考或自我解释,可以揭示读者理解策略的宝贵信息。目前的研究通过(1)调查读者使用的理解策略的组合和模式,以及(2)研究这些模式与个体差异和理解结果的关系,对现有工作进行了扩展。我们利用三个数据集(n=472)的档案数据来研究理解策略的使用在数据集、文本和人群(高中生、本科生)中的变化。学生的自我解释被编码为策略使用,然后根据策略的组合和模式进行进一步分析。我们的分析表明,几乎所有读者都主要从事转述和/或转述和桥接的结合,很少有详细阐述的例子。此外,转述和衔接的结合是理解测试表现的最佳预测因素。就模式而言,策略之间的转换与阅读理解无关,与转述和衔接的组合呈负相关。了解哪些策略组合和模式是最佳的,可以用来提供自适应指导和反馈,从而有助于为读者提供更个性化的支持。
{"title":"Anchoring your bridge: the importance of paraphrasing to inference making in self-explanations","authors":"D. McNamara, Natalie Newton, Katerina Christhilf, Kathryn S. McCarthy, Joseph P. Magliano, L. Allen","doi":"10.1080/0163853X.2023.2225757","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/0163853X.2023.2225757","url":null,"abstract":"ABSTRACT Analyzing constructed responses, such as think-alouds or self-explanations, can reveal valuable information about readers’ comprehension strategies. The current study expands on the extant work by (1) investigating combinations and patterns of comprehension strategies that readers use and (2) examining the extent to which these patterns relate to individual differences and comprehension outcomes. We leveraged archival data from three datasets (n = 472) to examine how comprehension strategy use varied across datasets, texts, and populations (high school, undergraduate). Students’ self-explanations were coded for strategy use and then further analyzed in terms of combinations and patterns of strategies. Our analyses revealed that almost all readers primarily engaged in paraphrasing and/or the combination of paraphrasing and bridging, with few instances of elaboration. Further, the combination of paraphrasing and bridging was the best predictor of performance on a comprehension test. In terms of patterns, switching between strategies was not correlated to reading comprehension and was negatively correlated with the combination of paraphrasing and bridging. Understanding which strategy combinations and patterns are optimal can be used to inform adaptive instruction and feedback that can aid in more individualized support for readers.","PeriodicalId":11316,"journal":{"name":"Discourse Processes","volume":"60 1","pages":"337 - 362"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2023-05-28","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41646725","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Introduction to special issue 特刊简介
IF 2.2 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EDUCATIONAL Pub Date : 2023-05-28 DOI: 10.1080/0163853x.2023.2200506
D. Greenberg, Jason L. G. Braasch, Elizabeth L. Tighe
{"title":"Introduction to special issue","authors":"D. Greenberg, Jason L. G. Braasch, Elizabeth L. Tighe","doi":"10.1080/0163853x.2023.2200506","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/0163853x.2023.2200506","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":11316,"journal":{"name":"Discourse Processes","volume":"60 1","pages":"225 - 225"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2023-05-28","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"49097719","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Discourse Processes
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1