首页 > 最新文献

Intelligence最新文献

英文 中文
The association between intelligence and face processing abilities: A conceptual and meta-analytic review 智力与面部处理能力之间的关系:概念和元分析综述
IF 3 2区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2023-01-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2022.101718
Dana L. Walker, Romina Palermo, Zoe Callis, Gilles E. Gignac

Whether there is an association between intelligence and face processing ability (i.e., face detection, face perception and face memory) is contentious, with some suggesting a moderate, positive association and others contending there is no meaningful association. The inconsistent results may be due to sample size differences, as well as variability in the quality of intelligence measures administered. The establishment of a moderate, positive correlation between face processing and intelligence would suggest it may be integrated within the Cattell-Horn-Carroll model of intelligence. Additionally, developmental prosopagnosia, a specific impairment of the recognition of facial identity, may be assessable in a manner similar to a learning disability. Consequently, we employed a psychometric meta-analytic approach to estimate the true score correlation between intelligence and face processing ability. Intelligence was positively and significantly correlated with face detection (r’ = 0.20; k = 2, N = 407), face perception (r’ = 0.42, k = 11, N = 2528), and face memory (r’ = 0.26, k = 23, N = 9062). Additionally, intelligence measurement quality moderated positively and significantly the association between intelligence and face memory (β = 0.08). On the basis of both theoretical and empirical considerations, we interpreted the results to suggest that face processing ability may be plausibly conceptualised within the Cattell-Horn-Carroll model of intelligence, in a manner similar to other relatively narrow dimensions of cognitive ability, i.e., associated positively with intelligence, but also distinct (e.g., reading comprehension). Potential clinical implications for the assessment of developmental prosopagnosia are also discussed.

智力与面部处理能力(即面部检测、面部感知和面部记忆)之间是否存在关联是有争议的,一些人认为存在适度的、积极的关联,而另一些人则认为没有有意义的关联。不一致的结果可能是由于样本大小的差异,以及智力测量质量的可变性。人脸处理和智力之间适度的正相关关系的建立表明,它可能被整合到卡特泰尔-霍恩-卡罗尔智力模型中。此外,发展性面孔失认症是一种特殊的面部识别障碍,可以用类似于学习障碍的方式来评估。因此,我们采用心理测量元分析方法来估计智力与面部处理能力之间的真实得分相关性。智力与人脸检测呈显著正相关(r′= 0.20;k = 2, N = 407),面部知觉(r = 0.42, k = 11, N = 2528)、和面部记忆(r = 0.26, k = 23, N = 9062)。此外,智力测量质量在智力与面孔记忆之间具有显著的正向调节作用(β = 0.08)。基于理论和经验的考虑,我们对结果进行了解释,表明面部处理能力可能在智力的卡特尔-霍恩-卡罗尔模型中被合理地概念化,其方式类似于其他相对狭窄的认知能力维度,即与智力呈正相关,但也不同(例如,阅读理解)。潜在的临床意义评估发展性面孔失认症也进行了讨论。
{"title":"The association between intelligence and face processing abilities: A conceptual and meta-analytic review","authors":"Dana L. Walker,&nbsp;Romina Palermo,&nbsp;Zoe Callis,&nbsp;Gilles E. Gignac","doi":"10.1016/j.intell.2022.101718","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.intell.2022.101718","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p><span>Whether there is an association between intelligence and face processing ability (i.e., face detection, face perception and face memory) is contentious, with some suggesting a moderate, positive association and others contending there is no meaningful association. The inconsistent results may be due to sample size differences, as well as variability in the quality of intelligence measures administered. The establishment of a moderate, positive correlation between face processing and intelligence would suggest it may be integrated within the Cattell-Horn-Carroll model of intelligence. Additionally, developmental prosopagnosia<span>, a specific impairment of the recognition of facial identity, may be assessable in a manner similar to a learning disability. Consequently, we employed a psychometric meta-analytic approach to estimate the true score correlation between intelligence and face processing ability. Intelligence was positively and significantly correlated with face detection (</span></span><em>r’</em> = 0.20; <em>k</em> = 2, <em>N =</em> 407), face perception (<em>r’</em> = 0.42, <em>k</em> = 11, <em>N</em> = 2528), and face memory (<em>r’</em> = 0.26, <em>k</em> = 23, <em>N</em> = 9062). Additionally, intelligence measurement quality moderated positively and significantly the association between intelligence and face memory (<em>β</em> = 0.08). On the basis of both theoretical and empirical considerations, we interpreted the results to suggest that face processing ability may be plausibly conceptualised within the Cattell-Horn-Carroll model of intelligence, in a manner similar to other relatively narrow dimensions of cognitive ability, i.e., associated positively with intelligence, but also distinct (e.g., reading comprehension). Potential clinical implications for the assessment of developmental prosopagnosia are also discussed.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":13862,"journal":{"name":"Intelligence","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.0,"publicationDate":"2023-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41529653","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
Reevaluating the Dunning-Kruger effect: A response to and replication of 邓宁-克鲁格效应的再评价:对邓宁-克鲁格效应的回应与再现
IF 3 2区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2023-01-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2022.101717
C. Dunkel, Joseph L. Nedelec, Dimitri van der Linden
{"title":"Reevaluating the Dunning-Kruger effect: A response to and replication of","authors":"C. Dunkel, Joseph L. Nedelec, Dimitri van der Linden","doi":"10.1016/j.intell.2022.101717","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2022.101717","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":13862,"journal":{"name":"Intelligence","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.0,"publicationDate":"2023-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41640090","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 3
Reevaluating the Dunning-Kruger effect: A response to and replication of Gignac and Zajenkowski (2020) 重新评估Dunning-Kruger效应:对Gignac和Zajenkowski(2020)的回应和复制
IF 3 2区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2023-01-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2022.101717
Curtis S. Dunkel , Joseph Nedelec , Dimitri van der Linden

As applied to general intelligence, the Dunning-Kruger effect (DK) is the phenomenon in which individuals at the lower end of the intellectual ability distribution are more likely to overestimate their intelligence. In a recent article in Intelligence it was suggested that the DK is primarily a statistical artifact and, indeed, the application of more appropriate analyses led to a failure to replicate a significant effect. When some of the limitations (namely sample representativeness) were addressed and the more appropriate statistical methods were used in the current study, our analyses illustrated a statistically significant DK effect. However, the magnitude of the effect was minimal; bringing its meaningfulness into question. In conclusion, it is recommended that the conditions that result in a significant DK be further explored.

邓宁-克鲁格效应(DK)适用于一般智力,是指智力分布较低端的个体更有可能高估自己的智力的现象。在《情报》杂志最近的一篇文章中,有人认为DK主要是一种统计假象,事实上,应用更合适的分析导致无法复制显著的效果。当解决了一些局限性(即样本代表性),并在当前研究中使用了更合适的统计方法时,我们的分析表明了统计学上显著的DK效应。然而,影响的程度很小;使其意义受到质疑。总之,建议进一步探索导致重大DK的条件。
{"title":"Reevaluating the Dunning-Kruger effect: A response to and replication of Gignac and Zajenkowski (2020)","authors":"Curtis S. Dunkel ,&nbsp;Joseph Nedelec ,&nbsp;Dimitri van der Linden","doi":"10.1016/j.intell.2022.101717","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2022.101717","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>As applied to general intelligence, the Dunning-Kruger effect (DK) is the phenomenon in which individuals at the lower end of the intellectual ability distribution are more likely to overestimate their intelligence. In a recent article in <em>Intelligence</em> it was suggested that the DK is primarily a statistical artifact and, indeed, the application of more appropriate analyses led to a failure to replicate a significant effect. When some of the limitations (namely sample representativeness) were addressed and the more appropriate statistical methods were used in the current study, our analyses illustrated a statistically significant DK effect. However, the magnitude of the effect was minimal; bringing its meaningfulness into question. In conclusion, it is recommended that the conditions that result in a significant DK be further explored.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":13862,"journal":{"name":"Intelligence","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.0,"publicationDate":"2023-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"49761928","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 3
Educational choice has greater effects on sex ratios of college STEM majors than has the greater male variance in general intelligence (g) 教育选择对大学STEM专业学生性别比的影响大于男性在一般智力方面的差异(g)
IF 3 2区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2023-01-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2022.101719
Dai Li , Yizhen Wang , Lantian Li

In STEM fields other than biological sciences (math-intensive STEM), there is a greater ratio of males to females (M:F ratio) than that of the general population. The Ability Distribution Hypothesis suggests that this is mainly due to greater male variance in g. Others hypothesize that this is due to sex differences in occupational interests. There has not been an empirical study to evaluate which kind of differences has greater effects on the M:F ratios in math-intensive STEM fields. To fill the gap, we examine the test scores, application for majors and final admissions in a complete dataset of college entrance. We study the M:F ratios of four math-intensive STEM majors: Economics, Engineering, Computer Science and Physical sciences and Math. In summary, we find that greater male variance exists in total test scores; greater male variance partially explains the female underrepresentation in the upper tails of total test scores; men appear to have stronger interests in Engineering and Computer Science than women, while women appear to have stronger interests in Economics and to a lesser extent Physical sciences and Math than men; compared to sex differences in test scores, sex differences in major-choosing appear to have stronger effects on the M:F ratios of math-intensive STEM majors.

在除生物科学(数学密集型STEM)以外的STEM领域,男性与女性的比例(M:F比)高于一般人群。能力分布假说认为,这主要是由于男性在g方面的差异更大。其他人假设这是由于职业兴趣的性别差异。目前还没有实证研究来评估哪种差异对数学密集型STEM领域的M:F比率有更大的影响。为了填补这一空白,我们在一个完整的大学入学数据集中检查了考试成绩、专业申请和最终录取。我们研究了四个数学密集型STEM专业的M:F比率:经济学,工程学,计算机科学和物理科学与数学。综上所述,我们发现男性在总考试成绩上存在较大的差异;较大的男性方差部分解释了女性在总测试分数上尾的代表性不足;男性似乎比女性对工程和计算机科学更感兴趣,而女性似乎对经济学更感兴趣,对物理科学和数学的兴趣程度低于男性;与考试成绩的性别差异相比,专业选择的性别差异似乎对数学密集型STEM专业的M:F比率有更大的影响。
{"title":"Educational choice has greater effects on sex ratios of college STEM majors than has the greater male variance in general intelligence (g)","authors":"Dai Li ,&nbsp;Yizhen Wang ,&nbsp;Lantian Li","doi":"10.1016/j.intell.2022.101719","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.intell.2022.101719","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>In STEM fields other than biological sciences (math-intensive STEM), there is a greater ratio of males to females (M:F ratio) than that of the general population. The <em>Ability Distribution Hypothesis</em> suggests that this is mainly due to greater male variance in <em>g</em><span>. Others hypothesize that this is due to sex differences in occupational interests. There has not been an empirical study to evaluate which kind of differences has greater effects on the M:F ratios in math-intensive STEM fields. To fill the gap, we examine the test scores, application for majors and final admissions in a complete dataset of college entrance. We study the M:F ratios of four math-intensive STEM majors: Economics, Engineering, Computer Science and Physical sciences and Math. In summary, we find that greater male variance exists in total test scores; greater male variance partially explains the female underrepresentation in the upper tails of total test scores; men appear to have stronger interests in Engineering and Computer Science than women, while women appear to have stronger interests in Economics and to a lesser extent Physical sciences and Math than men; compared to sex differences in test scores, sex differences in major-choosing appear to have stronger effects on the M:F ratios of math-intensive STEM majors.</span></p></div>","PeriodicalId":13862,"journal":{"name":"Intelligence","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.0,"publicationDate":"2023-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"47628057","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
Analytic thinking outruns fluid reasoning in explaining rejection of pseudoscience, paranormal, and conspiracist beliefs 在解释拒绝伪科学、超自然现象和阴谋论信仰时,分析思维胜过流动推理
IF 3 2区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2022-11-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2022.101705
Jan Jastrzębski, Adam Chuderski

Around one third of people across populations hold beliefs in epistemically unwarranted claims and theories. Why this effect is so strong remains elusive. In three studies (total N = 827), we clarified the relationships of fluid reasoning ability, analytic thinking style (indexed by non-intuitiveness and open-mindedness), and unwarranted beliefs in pseudoscience, paranormal phenomena, and conspiracy theories. Fluid reasoning predicted about 11% of variance in rejection of pseudoscience, but only 4% – in paranormal beliefs, and less than 2.5% – in conspiracist beliefs. By contrast, analytic thinking substantially predicted rejection of all the three kinds of beliefs, explaining 37% variance in pseudoscience and around 20% variance in paranormal and conspiracist beliefs. A novel finding indicated that fluid reasoning and analytic thinking predicted rejection of pseudoscience in an over-additive interaction. Fluid reasoning and analytic thinking explained the common variance shared by unwarranted beliefs, but not the belief-specific variance. Their relationships with unwarranted beliefs were stronger for males than for females. Overall, the three studies suggest that analytic thinking is more important than cognitive ability for adopting epistemically supported world-view.

大约三分之一的人相信在认知上毫无根据的主张和理论。为什么这种效应如此强烈仍然难以捉摸。在三项研究中(总N = 827),我们澄清了流动推理能力、分析思维方式(以非直觉性和开放性为索引)和对伪科学、超自然现象和阴谋论的毫无根据的信仰之间的关系。流动推理在拒绝伪科学方面预测了大约11%的差异,但在超自然信仰方面只有4%,在阴谋论信仰方面不到2.5%。相比之下,分析思维在很大程度上预测了对所有三种信仰的拒绝,解释了伪科学中37%的差异,以及超自然现象和阴谋论信仰中约20%的差异。一个新的发现表明,流体推理和分析思维预测拒绝伪科学在一个过度加性的相互作用。流动推理和分析思维解释了无根据信念所共有的共同差异,但不能解释信仰特有的差异。男性与无根据信念的关系比女性更强。总的来说,这三项研究表明,在采用认识论支持的世界观时,分析思维比认知能力更重要。
{"title":"Analytic thinking outruns fluid reasoning in explaining rejection of pseudoscience, paranormal, and conspiracist beliefs","authors":"Jan Jastrzębski,&nbsp;Adam Chuderski","doi":"10.1016/j.intell.2022.101705","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.intell.2022.101705","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Around one third of people across populations hold beliefs in epistemically unwarranted claims and theories. Why this effect is so strong remains elusive. In three studies (total <em>N</em> = 827), we clarified the relationships of fluid reasoning ability, analytic thinking style (indexed by non-intuitiveness and open-mindedness), and unwarranted beliefs in pseudoscience, paranormal phenomena, and conspiracy theories. Fluid reasoning predicted about 11% of variance in rejection of pseudoscience, but only 4% – in paranormal beliefs, and less than 2.5% – in conspiracist beliefs. By contrast, analytic thinking substantially predicted rejection of all the three kinds of beliefs, explaining 37% variance in pseudoscience and around 20% variance in paranormal and conspiracist beliefs. A novel finding indicated that fluid reasoning and analytic thinking predicted rejection of pseudoscience in an over-additive interaction. Fluid reasoning and analytic thinking explained the common variance shared by unwarranted beliefs, but not the belief-specific variance. Their relationships with unwarranted beliefs were stronger for males than for females. Overall, the three studies suggest that analytic thinking is more important than cognitive ability for adopting epistemically supported world-view.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":13862,"journal":{"name":"Intelligence","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.0,"publicationDate":"2022-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0160289622000861/pdfft?md5=bb495b37892b9331c1558c669ed4717e&pid=1-s2.0-S0160289622000861-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"42593189","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
g's little helpers – VOTAT and NOTAT mediate the relation between intelligence and complex problem solving g的小帮手——VOTAT和NOTAT调解了智力和解决复杂问题之间的关系
IF 3 2区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2022-11-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2022.101685
Christin Lotz , Ronny Scherer , Samuel Greiff , Jörn R. Sparfeldt

Intelligence and complex problem solving (CPS) correlate closely, but little is known about the mechanism that translates intelligence into successful CPS. Therefore, this study considered the strategic exploration behaviors VOTAT (vary-one-thing-at-a-time) and NOTAT (vary no-thing-at-a-time) as possible mediators. A sample of 495 high-school students worked on nine CPS tasks, six of which with solely direct effects and three with direct and eigendynamic effects. We expected substantial mediation effects if the applied strategic behaviors were optimal to identify the particular underlying effect types (i.e., direct effects: VOTAT; direct and eigendynamic effects: VOTAT and NOTAT). The model for tasks with only direct effects revealed VOTAT and NOTAT to be substantial mediators: Whereas VOTAT showed substantial positive relations to intelligence and CPS performance, NOTAT unexpectedly showed substantial negative relations. Both VOTAT and NOTAT resulted in significant indirect mediation effects. The model for tasks with direct and eigendynamic effects showed substantial positive relations of VOTAT and NOTAT to intelligence and CPS-performance and resulted in significant and positive indirect mediation effects. Moreover, the indirect effects differed between VOTAT and NOTAT and across the two facets of CPS performance. Overall, strategic exploration behaviors are relevant for explaining the g-CPS-relation.

智力与复杂问题解决能力(CPS)密切相关,但人们对将智力转化为成功的CPS的机制知之甚少。因此,本研究认为战略探索行为VOTAT (change -one-thing-at- time)和NOTAT (vary no-thing-at- time)可能是中介。495名高中生完成了9项CPS任务,其中6项只有直接效应,3项有直接效应和特征动力效应。如果应用的战略行为是最优的,我们预计会有实质性的中介效应,以确定特定的潜在效应类型(即直接效应:VOTAT;直接和本征动力效应:VOTAT和NOTAT)。只有直接影响的任务模型显示,VOTAT和NOTAT是实质性的中介:尽管VOTAT对智力和CPS表现表现出实质性的正相关,但NOTAT出人意料地表现出实质性的负相关。VOTAT和NOTAT均产生了显著的间接中介效应。在具有直接效应和特征动力效应的任务模型中,VOTAT和NOTAT对智力和cps绩效存在显著的正相关关系,并产生显著的正间接中介效应。此外,间接影响在VOTAT和NOTAT之间以及在CPS绩效的两个方面有所不同。总体而言,战略探索行为与解释g- cps关系相关。
{"title":"g's little helpers – VOTAT and NOTAT mediate the relation between intelligence and complex problem solving","authors":"Christin Lotz ,&nbsp;Ronny Scherer ,&nbsp;Samuel Greiff ,&nbsp;Jörn R. Sparfeldt","doi":"10.1016/j.intell.2022.101685","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.intell.2022.101685","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Intelligence and complex problem solving (CPS) correlate closely, but little is known about the mechanism that translates intelligence into successful CPS. Therefore, this study considered the strategic exploration behaviors VOTAT (vary-one-thing-at-a-time) and NOTAT (vary no-thing-at-a-time) as possible mediators. A sample of 495 high-school students worked on nine CPS tasks, six of which with solely direct effects and three with direct and eigendynamic effects. We expected substantial mediation effects if the applied strategic behaviors were optimal to identify the particular underlying effect types (i.e., direct effects: VOTAT; direct and eigendynamic effects: VOTAT and NOTAT). The model for tasks with only direct effects revealed VOTAT and NOTAT to be substantial mediators: Whereas VOTAT showed substantial positive relations to intelligence and CPS performance, NOTAT unexpectedly showed substantial negative relations. Both VOTAT and NOTAT resulted in significant indirect mediation effects. The model for tasks with direct and eigendynamic effects showed substantial positive relations of VOTAT and NOTAT to intelligence and CPS-performance and resulted in significant and positive indirect mediation effects. Moreover, the indirect effects differed between VOTAT and NOTAT and across the two facets of CPS performance. Overall, strategic exploration behaviors are relevant for explaining the <em>g</em>-CPS-relation.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":13862,"journal":{"name":"Intelligence","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.0,"publicationDate":"2022-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"46410219","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Sex differences in spatial and mechanical tilt: Support for investment theories 空间和机械倾斜的性别差异:对投资理论的支持
IF 3 2区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2022-11-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2022.101687
Thomas R. Coyle

Tilt refers to a pattern of specific abilities and is based on within subject differences in two abilities (spatial and academic), yielding relative strength in one ability (spatial) and weakness in another ability (academic). The current study examined sex differences in spatial and mechanical tilt. Tilt was measured using the Armed Services Vocational Aptitude Battery in the National Longitudinal Survey of Youth. Tilt was based on spatial and mechanical abilities contrasted with academic abilities (math or verbal), producing spatial and mechanical tilt (spatial/mechanical > academic) and academic tilt (academic > spatial/mechanical). For males and females, spatial and mechanical tilt correlated negatively with verbal and math scores on college tests (SAT, ACT, PSAT), indicating that spatial and mechanical tilt predicted low scores on the college tests. In addition, for males and females, spatial and mechanical tilt predicted jobs and college majors in science, technology, engineering, and math (STEM), with stronger effects for spatial and mechanical tilt contrasted with verbal ability. Although levels of mechanical tilt were higher for males, levels of spatial tilt showed no sex differences, a pattern that could be attributed to the spatial ability measured (visualization). In addition, no consistent sex differences in tilt relations with diverse criteria (tests, jobs, majors) were found. The results support investment theories and research on sex differences in vocational preferences for people versus things. Such theories assume that males prefer working with things and machines, boosting mechanical tilt, whereas females prefer working with people and in (non-technical) humanities fields, boosting academic tilt. Future research should consider tilt patterns in other developmental periods and examine whether tilt varies with ability level, as predicted by differentiation theories.

倾斜指的是一种特定能力的模式,它基于学科内部两种能力(空间和学术)的差异,产生一种能力(空间)的相对优势和另一种能力(学术)的相对劣势。目前的研究考察了空间倾斜和机械倾斜的性别差异。倾斜度是用全国青年纵向调查中的武装部队职业能力倾向量表来测量的。倾斜是基于空间和机械能力与学术能力(数学或语言)的对比,产生空间和机械倾斜(空间/机械>学术性)和学术性倾向(Academic >空间/机械)。对于男性和女性来说,空间倾斜和机械倾斜与大学考试(SAT, ACT, PSAT)的语言和数学成绩呈负相关,表明空间倾斜和机械倾斜预示着大学考试的低分。此外,对于男性和女性而言,空间和机械倾斜度对科学、技术、工程和数学(STEM)领域的就业和大学专业具有预测作用,且空间和机械倾斜度的影响强于语言能力。尽管男性的机械倾斜度更高,但空间倾斜度没有显示出性别差异,这种模式可以归因于所测量的空间能力(可视化)。此外,在不同标准(考试、工作、专业)的倾斜度关系中,没有发现一致的性别差异。研究结果支持了投资理论和关于职业偏好性别差异的研究。这些理论假设男性更喜欢和东西和机器一起工作,增加了机械倾向,而女性更喜欢和人一起工作,在(非技术的)人文领域,增加了学术倾向。未来的研究应考虑其他发育时期的倾斜度模式,并检验倾斜度是否如分化理论所预测的那样随能力水平而变化。
{"title":"Sex differences in spatial and mechanical tilt: Support for investment theories","authors":"Thomas R. Coyle","doi":"10.1016/j.intell.2022.101687","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.intell.2022.101687","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p><span>Tilt refers to a pattern of specific abilities and is based on within subject differences in two abilities (spatial and academic), yielding relative strength in one ability (spatial) and weakness in another ability (academic). The current study examined sex differences in spatial and mechanical tilt. Tilt was measured using the Armed Services Vocational Aptitude Battery in the National Longitudinal Survey of Youth. Tilt was based on spatial and mechanical abilities contrasted with academic abilities (math or verbal), producing spatial and mechanical tilt (spatial/mechanical &gt; academic) and academic tilt (academic &gt; spatial/mechanical). For males and females, spatial and mechanical tilt correlated </span><em>negatively</em> with verbal and math scores on college tests (SAT, ACT, PSAT), indicating that spatial and mechanical tilt predicted low scores on the college tests. In addition, for males and females, spatial and mechanical tilt predicted jobs and college majors in science, technology, engineering, and math (STEM), with stronger effects for spatial and mechanical tilt contrasted with verbal ability. Although levels of mechanical tilt were higher for males, levels of spatial tilt showed no sex differences, a pattern that could be attributed to the spatial ability measured (visualization). In addition, no consistent sex differences in tilt relations with diverse criteria (tests, jobs, majors) were found. The results support investment theories and research on sex differences in vocational preferences for people versus things. Such theories assume that males prefer working with things and machines, boosting mechanical tilt, whereas females prefer working with people and in (non-technical) humanities fields, boosting academic tilt. Future research should consider tilt patterns in other developmental periods and examine whether tilt varies with ability level, as predicted by differentiation theories.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":13862,"journal":{"name":"Intelligence","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.0,"publicationDate":"2022-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"43451923","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 7
(No) Trade-off between numeracy and verbal reasoning development: PISA evidence from Italy's academic tracking (否)计算能力和语言推理发展之间的权衡:来自意大利学术跟踪的PISA证据
IF 3 2区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2022-11-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2022.101703
Matteo Zullo

The study uses PISA data to evaluate cognitive development trade-offs between numeracy and literacy skills. The compendious literature validating the educational and financial gains from technical education fails to address the potential underdevelopment of verbal skills. Exploiting academic tracking in Italy’s high school education with distinctive Liberal Arts (n = 841) and STEM (n = 1968) pathways, the study rules out any cognitive trade-off and estimates the STEM premium on the reading section at about one-fifth of an international standard deviation (20 PISA points). Decomposition of the education production function reveals that the technical track outperforms Liberal Arts due to greater educational production efficiency overcompensating for worse educational production inputs. Further regression analysis links the STEM advantage to the four additional instructional units in math and physics. Robustness checks conducted using TIMSS and PIRLS test scores exclude that effects are secondary to differences in preexisting levels of student skills.1

该研究使用PISA数据来评估计算能力和读写能力之间的认知发展权衡。简明扼要的文献验证了技术教育的教育和经济收益,但未能解决语言技能潜在的发展不足。该研究利用意大利高中教育中独特的文科(n = 841)和STEM (n = 1968)途径的学术跟踪,排除了任何认知上的权衡,并估计STEM在阅读部分的优势约为国际标准偏差(PISA 20分)的五分之一。对教育生产函数的分解表明,由于更高的教育生产效率过度补偿了更差的教育生产投入,技术轨道优于文科。进一步的回归分析将STEM的优势与数学和物理的四个额外教学单元联系起来。使用TIMSS和PIRLS测试分数进行的稳健性检查排除了影响是继发于先前存在的学生技能水平差异
{"title":"(No) Trade-off between numeracy and verbal reasoning development: PISA evidence from Italy's academic tracking","authors":"Matteo Zullo","doi":"10.1016/j.intell.2022.101703","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.intell.2022.101703","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>The study uses PISA data to evaluate cognitive development trade-offs between numeracy and literacy skills. The compendious literature validating the educational and financial gains from technical education fails to address the potential underdevelopment of verbal skills. Exploiting academic tracking in Italy’s high school education with distinctive Liberal Arts (<em>n</em> = 841) and STEM (<em>n</em><span> = 1968) pathways, the study rules out any cognitive trade-off and estimates the STEM premium on the reading section at about one-fifth of an international standard deviation (20 PISA points). Decomposition of the education production function reveals that the technical track outperforms Liberal Arts due to greater educational production efficiency overcompensating for worse educational production inputs. Further regression analysis links the STEM advantage to the four additional instructional units in math and physics. Robustness checks conducted using TIMSS and PIRLS test scores exclude that effects are secondary to differences in preexisting levels of student skills.</span><span><sup>1</sup></span></p></div>","PeriodicalId":13862,"journal":{"name":"Intelligence","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.0,"publicationDate":"2022-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"46279805","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Domain-specificity of Flynn effects in the CHC-model: Stratum II test score changes in Germanophone samples (1996–2018) chc模型中Flynn效应的域特异性:德语样本的第二层考试成绩变化(1996-2018)
IF 3 2区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2022-11-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2022.101707
Alexandros Lazaridis , Marco Vetter , Jakob Pietschnig

Generational IQ test score changes (the Flynn effect) were globally positive over large parts of the 20th century. However, accumulating evidence of recent studies shows a rather inconsistent pattern in past decades. Patterns of recently observed test score changes appeared to be markedly different in strength and even signs between countries and domains. Because of between-study design differences and data availability in terms of differing IQ domains, it is so far unclear if these inconsistencies represent a consequence of differences in Flynn effect trajectories between countries, covered time-spans, or investigated IQ domains. Here, we present data from 36 largely population-representative Germanophone standardization samples from 12 well-established psychometric tests (17 subtests) of 10 stratum II domains from 1996 to 2018, thus providing a comprehensive assessment of domain-specific changes according to the Cattell-Horn-Carroll intelligence model. Examination of both raw score and measurement-invariant latent mean changes yielded positive (comprehension-knowledge, learning-efficiency, domain-specific knowledge), negative (working memory capacity), stagnating (processing speed, reading and writing), and ambiguous (fluid reasoning, reaction and decision speed, quantitative knowledge, visual processing) stratum II Flynn effects. This means that in the present sample, the Flynn effect is surprisingly differentiated on domain level and does not conform to the frequently observed IQ test score gains in crystallized and fluid intelligence. These findings could be attributed to either (i) a so far undetected domain-specificity of the Flynn effect due to an unavailability of test data beyond crystallized and fluid domains or (ii) a symptom for an impending stagnation of the Flynn effect.

在20世纪的大部分时间里,智商测试的代际变化(弗林效应)在全球范围内都是积极的。然而,最近研究积累的证据表明,过去几十年的模式相当不一致。最近观察到的考试成绩变化的模式在不同国家和地区的强度甚至迹象上似乎都有显著差异。由于研究之间的设计差异和不同智商领域的数据可用性,目前尚不清楚这些不一致是否代表弗林效应轨迹在国家之间、覆盖的时间跨度或调查的智商领域之间的差异。在这里,我们提供了1996年至2018年10个地层II域的12个完善的心理测量测试(17个子测试)中36个具有很大人口代表性的德语标准化样本的数据,从而根据Cattell-Horn-Carroll智力模型对领域特定变化进行了全面评估。对原始分数和测量不变的潜在平均变化的检查产生了积极的(理解知识、学习效率、特定领域知识)、消极的(工作记忆容量)、停滞的(处理速度、阅读和写作)和模糊的(流动推理、反应和决策速度、定量知识、视觉处理)第II层弗林效应。这意味着,在目前的样本中,弗林效应在域水平上的差异令人惊讶,并且不符合经常观察到的在结晶智力和流体智力中获得的智商测试分数。这些发现可能归因于(i)由于无法获得结晶和流体领域以外的测试数据,迄今尚未检测到Flynn效应的域特异性,或者(ii) Flynn效应即将停滞的症状。
{"title":"Domain-specificity of Flynn effects in the CHC-model: Stratum II test score changes in Germanophone samples (1996–2018)","authors":"Alexandros Lazaridis ,&nbsp;Marco Vetter ,&nbsp;Jakob Pietschnig","doi":"10.1016/j.intell.2022.101707","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.intell.2022.101707","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Generational IQ test score changes (the Flynn effect) were globally positive over large parts of the 20th century. However, accumulating evidence of recent studies shows a rather inconsistent pattern in past decades. Patterns of recently observed test score changes appeared to be markedly different in strength and even signs between countries and domains. Because of between-study design differences and data availability in terms of differing IQ domains, it is so far unclear if these inconsistencies represent a consequence of differences in Flynn effect trajectories between countries, covered time-spans, or investigated IQ domains. Here, we present data from 36 largely population-representative Germanophone standardization samples from 12 well-established psychometric tests (17 subtests) of 10 stratum II domains from 1996 to 2018, thus providing a comprehensive assessment of domain-specific changes according to the Cattell-Horn-Carroll intelligence model. Examination of both raw score and measurement-invariant latent mean changes yielded positive (comprehension-knowledge, learning-efficiency, domain-specific knowledge), negative (working memory capacity), stagnating (processing speed, reading and writing), and ambiguous (fluid reasoning, reaction and decision speed, quantitative knowledge, visual processing) stratum II Flynn effects. This means that in the present sample, the Flynn effect is surprisingly differentiated on domain level and does not conform to the frequently observed IQ test score gains in crystallized and fluid intelligence. These findings could be attributed to either (i) a so far undetected domain-specificity of the Flynn effect due to an unavailability of test data beyond crystallized and fluid domains or (ii) a symptom for an impending stagnation of the Flynn effect.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":13862,"journal":{"name":"Intelligence","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.0,"publicationDate":"2022-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0160289622000885/pdfft?md5=7f677bc4a2a78cdd0c89710394f12571&pid=1-s2.0-S0160289622000885-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"48769455","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 4
Signs of a Flynn effect in rodents? Secular differentiation of the manifold of general cognitive ability in laboratory mice (Mus musculus) and Norwegian rats (Rattus norvegicus) over a century—Results from two cross-temporal meta-analyses 啮齿动物有弗林效应的迹象吗?一个世纪以来实验室小鼠(小家鼠)和挪威大鼠(褐家鼠)一般认知能力多样性的长期分化——来自两个跨时间荟萃分析的结果
IF 3 2区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2022-11-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2022.101700
Michael A. Woodley of Menie , Mateo Peñaherrera-Aguirre , Matthew A. Sarraf

Substantial improvements in factors such as microbiological quality have been noted in laboratory rodent (mouse [Mus musculus] and rat [Rattus norvegicus]) populations over the last 140 years, since domestication of laboratory strains started. These environmental improvements may have caused Flynn effect-like cognitive changes to occur in these populations, perhaps if these improvements enhanced cognitive plasticity and, consequently, learning potential. While lack of relevant data precludes cross-temporal comparison of cognitive performance means of laboratory rodent populations, it is possible to estimate changes in the proportion of cognitive performance variance attributable to general cognitive ability (GCA) over time. This “differentiation effect” has been found to occur along with the Flynn effect in human populations, suggesting that environmental factors, possibly mediated by their effects on life history speed, may weaken the manifold of GCA across time, allowing for greater cultivation of specialized abilities. Meta-analysis of the literature on mouse and rat cognition yielded 25 mouse studies from which 28 GCA effect sizes could be estimated, and 10 rat studies from which 11 effect sizes could be estimated. Cross-temporal meta-analysis yielded evidence of significant “differentiation effects” spanning approximately a century in both mice and rats, which were independent of age, sex, factor estimation technique, and task number in the case of the mice, and both factor estimation technique and task number in the case of the rats. These trends were also independent of the random effect of strain in both cases. While this is suggestive of the presence of the Flynn effect in captive populations of non-human animals, there are still factors that might be confounding these results. This meta-analysis should be followed up with experimental investigation.

自从实验室菌株开始驯化以来,在实验室啮齿动物(小鼠[小家鼠]和大鼠[褐家鼠])种群中,微生物质量等因素在过去140年里得到了实质性的改善。这些环境的改善可能会导致这些人群发生类似弗林效应的认知变化,也许是因为这些改善提高了认知可塑性,从而提高了学习潜力。虽然缺乏相关数据,无法跨时间比较实验室啮齿动物种群的认知表现手段,但可以估计归因于一般认知能力(GCA)的认知表现差异比例随时间的变化。这种“分化效应”已被发现与人类种群中的弗林效应一起发生,这表明环境因素(可能是通过它们对生活史速度的影响来调节的)可能会削弱GCA的多样性,从而使专业能力得到更好的培养。对小鼠和大鼠认知文献的荟萃分析得出,25项小鼠研究中有28项可以估计出GCA效应量,10项大鼠研究中有11项可以估计出GCA效应量。跨时间荟萃分析发现,小鼠和大鼠在大约一个世纪的时间内都存在显著的“分化效应”,小鼠的差异与年龄、性别、因素估计技术和任务数量无关,大鼠的差异与因素估计技术和任务数量无关。在两种情况下,这些趋势也独立于应变的随机效应。虽然这表明弗林效应存在于圈养的非人类动物种群中,但仍有一些因素可能会混淆这些结果。本荟萃分析应进行后续实验研究。
{"title":"Signs of a Flynn effect in rodents? Secular differentiation of the manifold of general cognitive ability in laboratory mice (Mus musculus) and Norwegian rats (Rattus norvegicus) over a century—Results from two cross-temporal meta-analyses","authors":"Michael A. Woodley of Menie ,&nbsp;Mateo Peñaherrera-Aguirre ,&nbsp;Matthew A. Sarraf","doi":"10.1016/j.intell.2022.101700","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.intell.2022.101700","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Substantial improvements in factors such as microbiological quality have been noted in laboratory rodent (mouse [<em>Mus musculus</em>] and rat [<em>Rattus norvegicus</em>]) populations over the last 140 years, since domestication of laboratory strains started. These environmental improvements may have caused Flynn effect-like cognitive changes to occur in these populations, perhaps if these improvements enhanced cognitive plasticity and, consequently, learning potential. While lack of relevant data precludes cross-temporal comparison of cognitive performance means of laboratory rodent populations, it is possible to estimate changes in the proportion of cognitive performance variance attributable to general cognitive ability (GCA) over time. This “differentiation effect” has been found to occur along with the Flynn effect in human populations, suggesting that environmental factors, possibly mediated by their effects on life history speed, may weaken the manifold of GCA across time, allowing for greater cultivation of specialized abilities. Meta-analysis of the literature on mouse and rat cognition yielded 25 mouse studies from which 28 GCA effect sizes could be estimated, and 10 rat studies from which 11 effect sizes could be estimated. Cross-temporal meta-analysis yielded evidence of significant “differentiation effects” spanning approximately a century in both mice and rats, which were independent of age, sex, factor estimation technique, and task number in the case of the mice, and both factor estimation technique and task number in the case of the rats. These trends were also independent of the random effect of strain in both cases. While this is suggestive of the presence of the Flynn effect in captive populations of non-human animals, there are still factors that might be confounding these results. This meta-analysis should be followed up with experimental investigation.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":13862,"journal":{"name":"Intelligence","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.0,"publicationDate":"2022-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"42727341","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
期刊
Intelligence
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1