首页 > 最新文献

Intelligence最新文献

英文 中文
Intelligence and life expectancy in late adulthood: A meta-analysis 成年晚期的智力和预期寿命:一项荟萃分析
IF 3 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2023-05-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2023.101738
Macarena Sánchez-Izquierdo , Rocío Fernández-Ballesteros , Elizabeth Lucía Valeriano-Lorenzo , Juan Botella

In an aging society, it is crucial to understand why some people live long and others do not. There has been a proliferation of studies in recent years that highlight the importance of psycho-behavioural factors in the ways of aging, one of those psychological components is intelligence. In this meta-analysis, the association between intelligence and life expectancy in late adulthood is analysed through the Hazard Ratio (HR). Our objectives are: (i) to update Calvin's meta-analysis, especially the estimate of the association between survival and intelligence; and (ii) to evaluate the role of some moderators, especially the age of the participants, to explore intelligence–mortality throughout adulthood and old age. The results show a positive relationship between intelligence and survival (HR: 0.79; 95% CI: 0.81–0.76). This association is significantly moderated by the years of follow-up, the effect size being smaller the more years elapse between the intelligence assessment and the recording of the outcome. Intelligence is a protective factor to reach middle-high age, but from then on survival depends less and less on intelligence and more on other factors.

在老龄化社会中,理解为什么有些人长寿而有些人不长寿是至关重要的。近年来,越来越多的研究强调了心理行为因素在衰老过程中的重要性,其中一个心理因素就是智力。在本荟萃分析中,通过风险比(HR)分析了成年后期智力与预期寿命之间的关系。我们的目标是:(i)更新Calvin的元分析,特别是对生存和智力之间关系的估计;(ii)评估一些调节因素的作用,特别是参与者的年龄,以探索整个成年期和老年期的智力死亡率。结果表明,智力与生存呈正相关(HR: 0.79;95% ci: 0.81-0.76)。这种关联被随访年份显著地缓和了,在智力评估和结果记录之间的时间间隔越长,效果越小。智力是达到中老年的保护因素,但从那时起,生存对智力的依赖越来越少,而更多地依赖于其他因素。
{"title":"Intelligence and life expectancy in late adulthood: A meta-analysis","authors":"Macarena Sánchez-Izquierdo ,&nbsp;Rocío Fernández-Ballesteros ,&nbsp;Elizabeth Lucía Valeriano-Lorenzo ,&nbsp;Juan Botella","doi":"10.1016/j.intell.2023.101738","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.intell.2023.101738","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>In an aging society, it is crucial to understand why some people live long and others do not. There has been a proliferation of studies in recent years that highlight the importance of psycho-behavioural factors in the ways of aging, one of those psychological components is intelligence. In this meta-analysis, the association between intelligence and life expectancy in late adulthood is analysed through the Hazard Ratio (HR). Our objectives are: (i) to update Calvin's meta-analysis, especially the estimate of the association between survival and intelligence; and (ii) to evaluate the role of some moderators, especially the age of the participants, to explore intelligence–mortality throughout adulthood and old age. The results show a positive relationship between intelligence and survival (HR<sub>•</sub>: 0.79; 95% CI: 0.81–0.76). This association is significantly moderated by the years of follow-up, the effect size being smaller the more years elapse between the intelligence assessment and the recording of the outcome. Intelligence is a protective factor to reach middle-high age, but from then on survival depends less and less on intelligence and more on other factors.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":13862,"journal":{"name":"Intelligence","volume":"98 ","pages":"Article 101738"},"PeriodicalIF":3.0,"publicationDate":"2023-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"49373951","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
General intelligence in middle school students from different Russian regions: Results of PISA-like tests 俄罗斯不同地区中学生的一般智力:类似pisa测试的结果
IF 3 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2023-05-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2023.101756
Nikita Kolachev , Galina Kovaleva

This study is aimed at investigating the contribution of the general intelligence factor if six PISA domains (reading, mathematical, scientific, financial literacies, global competence, and creative thinking) are combined in one measurement instrument. For achieving our goal, items based on the PISA frameworks are developed, students in grades 5–8 from three different Russian regions are assessed, and three IRT models (unidimensional, multidimensional, and bifactor) are applied to process the data. In addition, the correlations from the multidimensional model are estimated to examine the degree of cognitive specificity and mixture modeling is implemented to investigate ability differentiation across grades. Statistical analysis reveals that the bifactor model comprising one general and six specific factors, has a better fit in each grade. Based on this model, we compute the variance explained by the general factor, with the estimates varying between 60% and 70%. In general, the pure variance explained by specific factors does not exceed 10%. The correlations are above 0.40 in each grade and the averaged associations tend to increase from 6th to 8th grade, although they are smaller in years 6 and 7 compared to year 5. The general ability differentiation effect is observed in grades 6 to 8 and is not present in grade 5. Specific ability differentiation is more pronounced in reading literacy, especially in grade 5 to 7. The results obtained are discussed from the perspective of the ability and developmental differentiation/dedifferentiation problem.

本研究旨在探讨如果将六个PISA领域(阅读、数学、科学、金融素养、全球能力和创造性思维)结合在一个测量工具中,一般智力因素的贡献。为了实现我们的目标,我们开发了基于PISA框架的项目,对来自俄罗斯三个不同地区的5-8年级学生进行了评估,并应用了三个IRT模型(一维、多维和双因素)来处理数据。此外,通过估算多维模型的相关性来检验认知特异性程度,并采用混合模型来研究年级间的能力差异。统计分析表明,由1个一般因子和6个特殊因子组成的双因子模型在各等级均具有较好的拟合效果。基于该模型,我们计算了由一般因素解释的方差,估计值在60%到70%之间变化。一般来说,由特定因素解释的纯方差不超过10%。各年级的相关系数均在0.40以上,从6年级到8年级,平均相关性有增加的趋势,尽管6年级和7年级的相关性比5年级小。一般的能力分化效应在6至8年级观察到,而在5年级不存在。特异能力分化在阅读素养中更为明显,特别是在5 - 7年级。从能力和发育分化/去分化问题的角度对所得结果进行了讨论。
{"title":"General intelligence in middle school students from different Russian regions: Results of PISA-like tests","authors":"Nikita Kolachev ,&nbsp;Galina Kovaleva","doi":"10.1016/j.intell.2023.101756","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.intell.2023.101756","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>This study is aimed at investigating the contribution of the general intelligence factor if six PISA domains (reading, mathematical, scientific, financial literacies, global competence, and creative thinking) are combined in one measurement instrument. For achieving our goal, items based on the PISA frameworks are developed, students in grades 5–8 from three different Russian regions are assessed, and three IRT models (unidimensional, multidimensional, and bifactor) are applied to process the data. In addition, the correlations from the multidimensional model are estimated to examine the degree of cognitive specificity and mixture modeling is implemented to investigate ability differentiation across grades. Statistical analysis reveals that the bifactor model comprising one general and six specific factors, has a better fit in each grade. Based on this model, we compute the variance explained by the general factor, with the estimates varying between 60% and 70%. In general, the pure variance explained by specific factors does not exceed 10%. The correlations are above 0.40 in each grade and the averaged associations tend to increase from 6th to 8th grade, although they are smaller in years 6 and 7 compared to year 5. The general ability differentiation effect is observed in grades 6 to 8 and is not present in grade 5. Specific ability differentiation is more pronounced in reading literacy, especially in grade 5 to 7. The results obtained are discussed from the perspective of the ability and developmental differentiation/dedifferentiation problem.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":13862,"journal":{"name":"Intelligence","volume":"98 ","pages":"Article 101756"},"PeriodicalIF":3.0,"publicationDate":"2023-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"47614191","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Further arguments that ability tilt correlations are spurious: A reply to Coyle (2022) 关于能力倾斜相关性的进一步论证是错误的:对科伊尔的回复(2022)
IF 3 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2023-05-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2022.101706
Kimmo Sorjonen , Michael Ingre , Gustav Nilsonne , Bo Melin

Ability tilt refers to a within-individual difference between two abilities, e.g. a difference between math and verbal ability. Coyle and colleagues have demonstrated correlations between ability tilts and measures of the constituent abilities. We have previously pointed out that such measures may be spurious as the tilt variable is dependent on the constituent abilities. We have further shown that reported tilt associations are inconsistent with simulations including non-spurious tilt-effects, and concluded that tilt-correlations demonstrated by Coyle and colleagues are spurious. In a recent paper, Coyle responded with a series of arguments, including that the validity of tilt correlations is supported by their agreement with theoretical predictions, and that the analyses we used in our previous critique (regression effects) differ from tilt-correlations. Here, we advance the discussion by responding to the arguments put forward by Coyle. We show that the difference between regression effects and correlations is not material to the validity of our argument. Furthermore, we discuss the relation of tilt correlations to theory, and show that many empirical tilt-correlations, e.g. between the birth rate – death rate difference and fertility in US states, can be observed although such correlations can hardly be explained by differential investment theories. Therefore, we maintain that tilt correlations are spurious and that they offer little support for theories concerning the development of intelligence.

能力倾斜指的是两种能力之间的个体内部差异,例如数学和语言能力之间的差异。科伊尔和他的同事已经证明了能力倾斜度和构成能力的测量之间的相关性。我们以前曾指出,这些措施可能是虚假的,因为倾斜变量取决于组成能力。我们进一步表明,报告的倾斜关联与模拟不一致,包括非虚假倾斜效应,并得出结论,Coyle及其同事所证明的倾斜相关性是虚假的。在最近的一篇论文中,Coyle用一系列的论点来回应,包括倾斜相关性的有效性得到了它们与理论预测一致的支持,以及我们在之前的批评中使用的分析(回归效应)不同于倾斜相关性。在这里,我们通过回应科伊尔提出的论点来推进讨论。我们表明,回归效应和相关性之间的差异对我们论证的有效性并不重要。此外,我们讨论了倾斜相关性与理论的关系,并表明可以观察到许多经验倾斜相关性,例如美国各州的出生率-死亡率差异与生育率之间的相关性,尽管这种相关性很难用差分投资理论来解释。因此,我们认为倾斜相关性是虚假的,它们对智力发展的理论提供了很少的支持。
{"title":"Further arguments that ability tilt correlations are spurious: A reply to Coyle (2022)","authors":"Kimmo Sorjonen ,&nbsp;Michael Ingre ,&nbsp;Gustav Nilsonne ,&nbsp;Bo Melin","doi":"10.1016/j.intell.2022.101706","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.intell.2022.101706","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Ability tilt refers to a within-individual difference between two abilities, e.g. a difference between math and verbal ability. Coyle and colleagues have demonstrated correlations between ability tilts and measures of the constituent abilities. We have previously pointed out that such measures may be spurious as the tilt variable is dependent on the constituent abilities. We have further shown that reported tilt associations are inconsistent with simulations including non-spurious tilt-effects, and concluded that tilt-correlations demonstrated by Coyle and colleagues are spurious. In a recent paper, Coyle responded with a series of arguments, including that the validity of tilt correlations is supported by their agreement with theoretical predictions, and that the analyses we used in our previous critique (regression effects) differ from tilt-correlations. Here, we advance the discussion by responding to the arguments put forward by Coyle. We show that the difference between regression effects and correlations is not material to the validity of our argument. Furthermore, we discuss the relation of tilt correlations to theory, and show that many empirical tilt-correlations, e.g. between the birth rate – death rate difference and fertility in US states, can be observed although such correlations can hardly be explained by differential investment theories. Therefore, we maintain that tilt correlations are spurious and that they offer little support for theories concerning the development of intelligence.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":13862,"journal":{"name":"Intelligence","volume":"98 ","pages":"Article 101706"},"PeriodicalIF":3.0,"publicationDate":"2023-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"42936370","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
Cognitive characteristics of intellectually gifted children with a diagnosis of ADHD 诊断为多动症的智力天才儿童的认知特征
IF 3 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2023-03-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2023.101736
Cesare Cornoldi , David Giofrè , Enrico Toffalini

Some children may be intellectually gifted, and yet experience behavioral and academic difficulties. We examined 82 twice exceptional children (2e-ADHD), having an excellent General Ability Index (GAI) derived from the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-IV (GAI ≥ 125), and a diagnosis of Attention Deficit and Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD). They accounted for 8.8% of a large sample of children with ADHD, which is twice as high as the proportion of intellectually gifted children in a typical population. This over-representation does not reflect a misdiagnosis of ADHD, as these children showed the typical features predicted on the grounds of data regarding the ADHD sample, including lower scores in working memory and processing speed measures, combined with the inclusion criteria for giftedness. Based on information concerning intellectually gifted children with either a Specific Learning Disorder (SLD) or typical development, we observed that these characteristics of intelligence are similar to those seen in SLD, but not in typical development, irrespective of whether 2e-ADHD children had a comorbid SLD.

有些孩子可能有智力上的天赋,但却经历了行为和学业上的困难。我们检查了82名两次异常儿童(2 -ADHD),他们的一般能力指数(GAI)来自韦氏儿童智力量表- iv (GAI≥125),并且诊断为注意缺陷和多动障碍(ADHD)。他们占了大量ADHD儿童样本的8.8%,是典型人群中智力超常儿童比例的两倍。这种过度代表并不能反映ADHD的误诊,因为这些孩子表现出了根据ADHD样本数据预测的典型特征,包括工作记忆和处理速度测量的较低分数,以及天才的纳入标准。基于关于患有特殊学习障碍(SLD)或典型发展的智力天才儿童的信息,我们观察到这些智力特征与特殊学习障碍(SLD)相似,但与典型发展不同,无论2 - adhd儿童是否患有共病的SLD。
{"title":"Cognitive characteristics of intellectually gifted children with a diagnosis of ADHD","authors":"Cesare Cornoldi ,&nbsp;David Giofrè ,&nbsp;Enrico Toffalini","doi":"10.1016/j.intell.2023.101736","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.intell.2023.101736","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Some children may be intellectually gifted, and yet experience behavioral and academic difficulties. We examined 82 twice exceptional children (2e-ADHD), having an excellent General Ability Index (GAI) derived from the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-IV (GAI ≥ 125), and a diagnosis of Attention Deficit and Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD). They accounted for 8.8% of a large sample of children with ADHD, which is twice as high as the proportion of intellectually gifted children in a typical population. This over-representation does not reflect a misdiagnosis of ADHD, as these children showed the typical features predicted on the grounds of data regarding the ADHD sample, including lower scores in working memory and processing speed measures, combined with the inclusion criteria for giftedness. Based on information concerning intellectually gifted children with either a Specific Learning Disorder (SLD) or typical development, we observed that these characteristics of intelligence are similar to those seen in SLD, but not in typical development, irrespective of whether 2e-ADHD children had a comorbid SLD.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":13862,"journal":{"name":"Intelligence","volume":"97 ","pages":"Article 101736"},"PeriodicalIF":3.0,"publicationDate":"2023-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"44652753","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
Stability of mental abilities and physical growth from 6 months to 65 years: Findings from the Zurich Longitudinal Studies 从6个月到65岁的心理能力和身体发育的稳定性:来自苏黎世纵向研究的发现
IF 3 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2023-03-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2023.101730
Dominique A. Eichelberger , Fabio Sticca , Dinah R. Kübler , Tanja H. Kakebeeke , Jon A. Caflisch , Oskar G. Jenni , Flavia M. Wehrle

Mental abilities and physical growth are important determinants of health across the lifespan. Here, the stability of these traits was assessed from 6 months to 65 years of age to investigate periods of stability and malleability. Mental abilities, height, and weight were assessed at 11 time-points in participants of the Zurich Longitudinal Studies. Individuals with more than three missing data points per trait across the 11 assessment time-points (i.e., more than approx. 25% missing data) were excluded from further analyses (final N = 281). Bivariate cross-time correlations showed that the stability of mental abilities was low in infancy and gradually increased. The stability of growth measures was uniform across development, with height being highly stable and weight moderately so. When a latent model was used, the overall stability of mental abilities approached that of weight. The findings indicate that stability and malleability across development differ between mental abilities and growth measures. This requires consideration in interventions targeting these traits as facilitators for improving health outcomes.

智力和身体发育是一生健康的重要决定因素。在这里,从6个月到65岁评估这些特征的稳定性,以研究稳定性和延展性的时期。苏黎世纵向研究的参与者在11个时间点评估了他们的智力、身高和体重。在11个评估时间点中,每个特征缺失3个以上数据点的个体(即,超过大约。25%缺失资料)被排除在进一步分析之外(最终N = 281)。双变量跨时间相关分析表明,幼儿时期心智能力的稳定性较低,并逐渐增强。生长指标的稳定性在整个发育过程中是均匀的,其中身高高度稳定,体重中等稳定。当使用潜在模型时,心理能力的总体稳定性接近体重。研究结果表明,发展过程中的稳定性和可塑性在心理能力和成长指标之间存在差异。这就要求在干预措施中考虑将这些特征作为改善健康结果的促进因素。
{"title":"Stability of mental abilities and physical growth from 6 months to 65 years: Findings from the Zurich Longitudinal Studies","authors":"Dominique A. Eichelberger ,&nbsp;Fabio Sticca ,&nbsp;Dinah R. Kübler ,&nbsp;Tanja H. Kakebeeke ,&nbsp;Jon A. Caflisch ,&nbsp;Oskar G. Jenni ,&nbsp;Flavia M. Wehrle","doi":"10.1016/j.intell.2023.101730","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.intell.2023.101730","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Mental abilities and physical growth are important determinants of health across the lifespan. Here, the stability of these traits was assessed from 6 months to 65 years of age to investigate periods of stability and malleability. Mental abilities, height, and weight were assessed at 11 time-points in participants of the Zurich Longitudinal Studies. Individuals with more than three missing data points per trait across the 11 assessment time-points (i.e., more than approx. 25% missing data) were excluded from further analyses (final <em>N</em> = 281). Bivariate cross-time correlations showed that the stability of mental abilities was low in infancy and gradually increased. The stability of growth measures was uniform across development, with height being highly stable and weight moderately so. When a latent model was used, the overall stability of mental abilities approached that of weight. The findings indicate that stability and malleability across development differ between mental abilities and growth measures. This requires consideration in interventions targeting these traits as facilitators for improving health outcomes.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":13862,"journal":{"name":"Intelligence","volume":"97 ","pages":"Article 101730"},"PeriodicalIF":3.0,"publicationDate":"2023-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"45828356","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Opinions on intelligence: An Arab perspective 对智力的看法:一个阿拉伯人的观点
IF 3 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2023-03-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2023.101731
David Becker, S. Bakhiet, A. Alshahomee, Abdelbasit Gadour, Fadil Elmenfi, Y. Essa, E. Dutton
{"title":"Opinions on intelligence: An Arab perspective","authors":"David Becker, S. Bakhiet, A. Alshahomee, Abdelbasit Gadour, Fadil Elmenfi, Y. Essa, E. Dutton","doi":"10.1016/j.intell.2023.101731","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2023.101731","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":13862,"journal":{"name":"Intelligence","volume":"1 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":3.0,"publicationDate":"2023-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"54477590","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
What lies beneath the structure of intelligence? Overview of the special issue on the processes underlying intelligence 智力结构的下面隐藏着什么?概述了关于底层智能过程的特刊
IF 3 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2023-03-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2023.101720
Marco Koch , Nicolas Becker , Samuel Greiff
{"title":"What lies beneath the structure of intelligence? Overview of the special issue on the processes underlying intelligence","authors":"Marco Koch ,&nbsp;Nicolas Becker ,&nbsp;Samuel Greiff","doi":"10.1016/j.intell.2023.101720","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.intell.2023.101720","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":13862,"journal":{"name":"Intelligence","volume":"97 ","pages":"Article 101720"},"PeriodicalIF":3.0,"publicationDate":"2023-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"45331643","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Opinions on intelligence: An Arab perspective 对智力的看法:一个阿拉伯人的观点
IF 3 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2023-03-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2023.101731
David Becker , Salaheldin Farah Bakhiet , Alsedig Abdalgadr Alshahomee , Abdelbasit Gadour , Fadil Elmenfi , Yossry Ahmed Sayed Essa , Edward Dutton

The issue of cultural differences in how the concept of intelligence is understood has long been debated. But do such differences really exist and, if so, to what extent do they exist and between which cultures are they the most pronounced? To better understand this, we translated a survey from Warne and Burton (2020) on beliefs about intelligence into Arabic, distributed it among psychology and non-psychology students and lecturers at universities within the Arab world, and compared our results with those from the US given by Warne and Burton, and with the current state of research. The survey consisted of 83 items about theories regarding the meaning and testing of intelligence. From our sample of up to 327 Arabic-speaking participants, we found that replies are overall similar to those from the US (r = .59; Nitems = 63), while both samples showed hardly any congruence with the current state of research. Agreements between the Arab and US-sample are mostly to be found on questions in which abilities are seen as important components of intelligence, while Arabs question the predictive validity of IQ test scores for life success much more than do their Western counterparts. Based on our results, we conclude that there is little evidence for the existence of cultural bias in points of view about intelligence research and that the concept of cultural non-transferability – or “cultural bias” – must be examined more critically.

在如何理解智力概念方面的文化差异问题一直存在争议。但是,这种差异真的存在吗?如果存在,它们存在的程度如何?它们在哪些文化之间最明显?为了更好地理解这一点,我们将Warne和Burton(2020)关于智力信念的一项调查翻译成阿拉伯语,将其分发给阿拉伯世界大学的心理学和非心理学学生和讲师,并将我们的结果与Warne和伯顿在美国给出的结果以及当前的研究状况进行了比较。这项调查包括83项关于智力的含义和测试的理论。从我们多达327名讲阿拉伯语的参与者的样本中,我们发现他们的回答与美国的回答总体相似(r=.59;Nitems=63),而这两个样本与目前的研究状况几乎没有一致性。阿拉伯和美国样本之间的一致性主要体现在能力被视为智力的重要组成部分的问题上,而阿拉伯人对智商测试分数对人生成功的预测有效性的质疑远远超过了西方同行。基于我们的研究结果,我们得出的结论是,从智力研究的角度来看,几乎没有证据表明文化偏见的存在,文化不可转移性或“文化偏见”的概念必须得到更严格的审查。
{"title":"Opinions on intelligence: An Arab perspective","authors":"David Becker ,&nbsp;Salaheldin Farah Bakhiet ,&nbsp;Alsedig Abdalgadr Alshahomee ,&nbsp;Abdelbasit Gadour ,&nbsp;Fadil Elmenfi ,&nbsp;Yossry Ahmed Sayed Essa ,&nbsp;Edward Dutton","doi":"10.1016/j.intell.2023.101731","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2023.101731","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>The issue of cultural differences in how the concept of intelligence is understood has long been debated. But do such differences really exist and, if so, to what extent do they exist and between which cultures are they the most pronounced? To better understand this, we translated a survey from <span>Warne and Burton (2020)</span> on beliefs about intelligence into Arabic, distributed it among psychology and non-psychology students and lecturers at universities within the Arab world, and compared our results with those from the US given by Warne and Burton, and with the current state of research. The survey consisted of 83 items about theories regarding the meaning and testing of intelligence. From our sample of up to 327 Arabic-speaking participants, we found that replies are overall similar to those from the US (<em>r</em> = .59; <em>N</em><sub>items</sub> = 63), while both samples showed hardly any congruence with the current state of research. Agreements between the Arab and US-sample are mostly to be found on questions in which abilities are seen as important components of intelligence, while Arabs question the predictive validity of IQ test scores for life success much more than do their Western counterparts. Based on our results, we conclude that there is little evidence for the existence of cultural bias in points of view about intelligence research and that the concept of cultural non-transferability – or “cultural bias” – must be examined more critically.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":13862,"journal":{"name":"Intelligence","volume":"97 ","pages":"Article 101731"},"PeriodicalIF":3.0,"publicationDate":"2023-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"49720123","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Still no Dunning-Kruger effect: A reply to Hiller 仍然没有邓宁-克鲁格效应:对希特勒的回应
IF 3 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2023-03-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2023.101733
Gilles E. Gignac , Marcin Zajenkowski

The primary criticism raised by Hiller (2023) pertained to the transformation of the self-assessed intelligence (SAI) scores applied by Gignac and Zajenkowski (2020), an investigation that suggested the Dunning-Kruger effect (DKE) may be primarily a statistical artefact. Hiller recommended an alternative transformation. Our re-analysis of Gignac and Zajenkowski's data with Hiller's recommended transformation failed to find evidence in favour of the DK hypothesis. Hiller also recommended SAI measurement with a percentile-based approach. Based on a review of the literature, one investigation has employed a percentile-based approach to SAI measurement, and it largely failed to support the DK hypothesis, when analysed with the non-linear regression approach recommended by Gignac and Zajenkowski. We conclude by encouraging researchers to continue to derive novel approaches to testing the DK hypothesis, especially approaches that overcome known methodological challenges.

Hiller(2023)提出的主要批评与Gignac和Zajenkowski(2020)应用的自我评估智力(SAI)分数的转变有关,该调查表明,邓宁-克鲁格效应(DKE)可能主要是一种统计伪像。希勒推荐了另一种转变。我们对吉尼亚克和扎延科夫斯基的数据与希勒推荐的转换的重新分析未能找到支持DK假设的证据。希勒还建议采用基于百分位数的方法测量SAI。基于文献回顾,一项调查采用了基于百分位的方法来测量SAI,当使用Gignac和Zajenkowski推荐的非线性回归方法进行分析时,它在很大程度上无法支持DK假设。最后,我们鼓励研究人员继续推导新的方法来检验DK假设,特别是克服已知方法挑战的方法。
{"title":"Still no Dunning-Kruger effect: A reply to Hiller","authors":"Gilles E. Gignac ,&nbsp;Marcin Zajenkowski","doi":"10.1016/j.intell.2023.101733","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.intell.2023.101733","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>The primary criticism raised by Hiller (2023) pertained to the transformation of the self-assessed intelligence (SAI) scores applied by Gignac and Zajenkowski (2020), an investigation that suggested the Dunning-Kruger effect (DKE) may be primarily a statistical artefact. Hiller recommended an alternative transformation. Our re-analysis of Gignac and Zajenkowski's data with Hiller's recommended transformation failed to find evidence in favour of the DK hypothesis. Hiller also recommended SAI measurement with a percentile-based approach. Based on a review of the literature, one investigation has employed a percentile-based approach to SAI measurement, and it largely failed to support the DK hypothesis, when analysed with the non-linear regression approach recommended by Gignac and Zajenkowski. We conclude by encouraging researchers to continue to derive novel approaches to testing the DK hypothesis, especially approaches that overcome known methodological challenges.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":13862,"journal":{"name":"Intelligence","volume":"97 ","pages":"Article 101733"},"PeriodicalIF":3.0,"publicationDate":"2023-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"47206810","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Comment on Gignac and Zajenkowski, “The Dunning-Kruger effect is (mostly) a statistical artefact: Valid approaches to testing the hypothesis with individual differences data” 评论Gignac和Zajenkowski,“邓宁-克鲁格效应(主要)是一个统计伪产物:用个体差异数据检验假设的有效方法”
IF 3 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2023-03-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2023.101732
Avram Hiller

Gignac and Zajenkowski (2020) find that “the degree to which people mispredicted their objectively measured intelligence was equal across the whole spectrum of objectively measured intelligence”. This Comment shows that Gignac and Zajenkowski (2020) finding of homoscedasticity is likely the result of a recoding choice by the experimenters and does not in fact indicate that the Dunning-Kruger Effect is a mere statistical artifact. Specifically, Gignac and Zajenkowski (2020) recoded test subjects' responses to a question regarding self-assessed comparative IQ onto a linear IQ scale when a normal IQ scale would likely have been more appropriate. More generally, researchers studying self-assessed intelligence should be aware of potential measurement problems that may arise when transforming an ordinal scale onto an interval scale.

Gignac和Zajenkowski(2020)发现,“在整个客观测量的智力范围内,人们对客观测量的智力的错误预测程度是相等的”。这一评论表明,Gignac和Zajenkowski(2020)发现的同差性可能是实验者重新编码选择的结果,实际上并不表明邓宁-克鲁格效应仅仅是一个统计伪像。具体来说,Gignac和Zajenkowski(2020)将测试对象对自我评估比较智商问题的回答重新编码到线性智商量表上,而正常智商量表可能更合适。更一般地说,研究自我评估智力的研究人员应该意识到,当将有序量表转换为间隔量表时,可能会出现潜在的测量问题。
{"title":"Comment on Gignac and Zajenkowski, “The Dunning-Kruger effect is (mostly) a statistical artefact: Valid approaches to testing the hypothesis with individual differences data”","authors":"Avram Hiller","doi":"10.1016/j.intell.2023.101732","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.intell.2023.101732","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Gignac and Zajenkowski (2020) find that “the degree to which people mispredicted their objectively measured intelligence was equal across the whole spectrum of objectively measured intelligence”. This Comment shows that Gignac and Zajenkowski (2020) finding of homoscedasticity is likely the result of a recoding choice by the experimenters and does not in fact indicate that the Dunning-Kruger Effect is a mere statistical artifact. Specifically, Gignac and Zajenkowski (2020) recoded test subjects' responses to a question regarding self-assessed comparative IQ onto a linear IQ scale when a normal IQ scale would likely have been more appropriate. More generally, researchers studying self-assessed intelligence should be aware of potential measurement problems that may arise when transforming an ordinal scale onto an interval scale.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":13862,"journal":{"name":"Intelligence","volume":"97 ","pages":"Article 101732"},"PeriodicalIF":3.0,"publicationDate":"2023-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"47114072","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
期刊
Intelligence
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1