首页 > 最新文献

Journal of memory and language最新文献

英文 中文
Processing of linguistic focus depends on contrastive alternatives 语言焦点的处理取决于对比选择
IF 4.3 1区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2023-10-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.jml.2023.104444
Morwenna Hoeks, Maziar Toosarvandani, Amanda Rysling

Readers progressed through a sentence in the Maze task (Forster et al., 2009), deciding at each word between a sensical and a non-sensical continuation. Contexts presented before these sentences manipulated whether words were linguistically focused and whether they were given or new (Experiment 1); focused targets were read more slowly even when they were given, and new targets were read slowly in general. This both replicated earlier results in which slowdowns were found in the reading of focus (Benatar and Clifton, 2014; Birch and Rayner, 1997; Lowder and Gordon, 2015), and demonstrated that focus slowdowns are not reducible to newness. To clarify earlier results in which speed-ups were found on focused words (Birch and Rayner, 2010; Morris and Folk, 1998), contexts manipulated whether contrastive alternatives to focused words were presented with a focus particle (Experiment 2) or in a cleft construction (Experiment 3). Focused targets were read less slowly when a contrastive alternative was present in the context. This effect of contrastive alternatives cannot be reduced to simple semantic associate priming: Contexts also manipulated whether a semantically associated expression was present independently of the presence of a contrastive alternative (Experiment 4). Readers slowed down less when an alternative was present in the context, even when this alternative was not semantically associated to the target. These results indicate that the processing of focus depends on contrastive alternatives, in their interaction with newness, semantic association, and focus construction.

在迷宫任务中,读者在一个句子中前进(Forster et al., 2009),决定每个单词是有意义的还是无意义的延续。在这些句子之前呈现的语境操纵了单词在语言上是否集中,以及它们是给定的还是新的(实验1);注意力集中的目标即使在给定的情况下也会被读得更慢,而新目标的阅读速度一般也会更慢。这两者都重复了先前的结果,即在焦点阅读中发现了减速(Benatar和Clifton, 2014;Birch and Rayner, 1997;Lowder和Gordon, 2015),并证明焦点减速不能简化为新鲜感。为了澄清之前的结果,即在重点词上发现了加速(Birch和Rayner, 2010;Morris和Folk, 1998),语境控制了焦点词的对比替代词是用焦点粒子呈现(实验2)还是用裂口结构呈现(实验3)。当语境中存在对比替代词时,焦点目标的阅读速度会降低。对比选项的这种影响不能简化为简单的语义关联启动:上下文也会操纵语义相关表达是否独立于对比选项的存在而存在(实验4)。当上下文中存在替代选项时,读者的阅读速度会降低,即使该替代选项与目标没有语义关联。这些结果表明,焦点加工依赖于对比选择,以及它们与新颖性、语义关联和焦点构建的相互作用。
{"title":"Processing of linguistic focus depends on contrastive alternatives","authors":"Morwenna Hoeks,&nbsp;Maziar Toosarvandani,&nbsp;Amanda Rysling","doi":"10.1016/j.jml.2023.104444","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.jml.2023.104444","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Readers progressed through a sentence in the Maze task (Forster et al., 2009), deciding at each word between a sensical and a non-sensical continuation. Contexts presented before these sentences manipulated whether words were linguistically focused and whether they were given or new (Experiment 1); focused targets were read more slowly even when they were given, and new targets were read slowly in general. This both replicated earlier results in which slowdowns were found in the reading of focus (Benatar and Clifton, 2014; Birch and Rayner, 1997; Lowder and Gordon, 2015), and demonstrated that focus slowdowns are not reducible to newness. To clarify earlier results in which speed-ups were found on focused words (Birch and Rayner, 2010; Morris and Folk, 1998), contexts manipulated whether contrastive alternatives to focused words were presented with a focus particle (Experiment 2) or in a cleft construction (Experiment 3). Focused targets were read less slowly when a contrastive alternative was present in the context. This effect of contrastive alternatives cannot be reduced to simple semantic associate priming: Contexts also manipulated whether a semantically associated expression was present independently of the presence of a contrastive alternative (Experiment 4). Readers slowed down less when an alternative was present in the context, even when this alternative was not semantically associated to the target. These results indicate that the processing of focus depends on contrastive alternatives, in their interaction with newness, semantic association, and focus construction.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":16493,"journal":{"name":"Journal of memory and language","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":4.3,"publicationDate":"2023-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"45357819","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Understanding the complexity of computational models through optimization and sloppy parameter analyses: The case of the Connectionist Dual-Process Model 通过优化和草率的参数分析来理解计算模型的复杂性:以连接主义双进程模型为例
IF 4.3 1区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2023-09-16 DOI: 10.1016/j.jml.2023.104468
Conrad Perry , Rick Evertz , Marco Zorzi , Johannes C. Ziegler

A major strength of computational cognitive models is their capacity to accurately predict empirical data. However, challenges in understanding how complex models work and the risk of overfitting have often been addressed by trading off predictive accuracy with model simplification. Here, we introduce state-of-the-art model analysis techniques to show how a large number of parameters in a cognitive model can be reduced into a smaller set that is simpler to understand and can be used to make more constrained predictions with. As a test case, we created different versions of the Connectionist Dual-Process model (CDP) of reading aloud whose parameters were optimized on seven different databases. The results showed that CDP was not overfit and could predict a large amount of variance across those databases. Indeed, the quantitative performance of CDP was higher than that of previous models in this area. Moreover, sloppy parameter analysis, a mathematical technique used to quantify the effects of different parameters on model performance, revealed that many of the parameters in CDP have very little effect on its performance. This shows that the dynamics of CDP are much simpler than its relatively large number of parameters might suggest. Overall, our study shows that cognitive models with large numbers of parameters do not necessarily overfit the empirical data and that understanding the behavior of complex models is more tractable using appropriate mathematical tools. The same techniques could be applied to many different complex cognitive models whenever appropriate datasets for model optimization exist.

计算认知模型的一个主要优势是它们准确预测经验数据的能力。然而,在理解复杂模型如何工作以及过度拟合的风险方面的挑战往往是通过权衡预测准确性和模型简化来解决的。在这里,我们介绍了最先进的模型分析技术,以展示如何将认知模型中的大量参数简化为更易于理解的较小集合,并可用于进行更受约束的预测。作为一个测试案例,我们创建了不同版本的连接主义双过程朗读模型(CDP),其参数在七个不同的数据库上进行了优化。结果表明,CDP没有过度拟合,并且可以预测这些数据库之间的大量方差。事实上,CDP在这一领域的定量性能高于以前的模型。此外,草率参数分析(一种用于量化不同参数对模型性能影响的数学技术)表明,CDP中的许多参数对其性能的影响非常小。这表明CDP的动力学比其相对大量的参数可能暗示的要简单得多。总的来说,我们的研究表明,具有大量参数的认知模型不一定与经验数据过度拟合,并且使用适当的数学工具更容易理解复杂模型的行为。只要存在用于模型优化的适当数据集,相同的技术就可以应用于许多不同的复杂认知模型。
{"title":"Understanding the complexity of computational models through optimization and sloppy parameter analyses: The case of the Connectionist Dual-Process Model","authors":"Conrad Perry ,&nbsp;Rick Evertz ,&nbsp;Marco Zorzi ,&nbsp;Johannes C. Ziegler","doi":"10.1016/j.jml.2023.104468","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2023.104468","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>A major strength of computational cognitive models is their capacity to accurately predict empirical data. However, challenges in understanding how complex models work and the risk of overfitting have often been addressed by trading off predictive accuracy with model simplification. Here, we introduce state-of-the-art model analysis techniques to show how a large number of parameters in a cognitive model can be reduced into a smaller set that is simpler to understand and can be used to make more constrained predictions with. As a test case, we created different versions of the Connectionist Dual-Process model (CDP) of reading aloud whose parameters were optimized on seven different databases. The results showed that CDP was not overfit and could predict a large amount of variance across those databases. Indeed, the quantitative performance of CDP was higher than that of previous models in this area. Moreover, sloppy parameter analysis, a mathematical technique used to quantify the effects of different parameters on model performance, revealed that many of the parameters in CDP have very little effect on its performance. This shows that the dynamics of CDP are much simpler than its relatively large number of parameters might suggest. Overall, our study shows that cognitive models with large numbers of parameters do not necessarily overfit the empirical data and that understanding the behavior of complex models is more tractable using appropriate mathematical tools. The same techniques could be applied to many different complex cognitive models whenever appropriate datasets for model optimization exist.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":16493,"journal":{"name":"Journal of memory and language","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":4.3,"publicationDate":"2023-09-16","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"49871684","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
What’s in a name? A large-scale computational study on how competition between names affects naming variation 名字里有什么?名称之间竞争如何影响命名变化的大规模计算研究
IF 4.3 1区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2023-08-31 DOI: 10.1016/j.jml.2023.104459
Eleonora Gualdoni , Thomas Brochhagen , Andreas Mädebach , Gemma Boleda

Different speakers often use different names to refer to the same entity (e.g., “woman” vs. “tennis player” for a given woman playing tennis). We study how visual typicality affects variation in naming behavior. We use a novel computational approach to estimate visual typicality from images, and analyze a large dataset containing naming data for realistic images. In contrast to previous work, we take into account the visual properties of both the object and the scene in which it appears; and factor in multiple candidate names. We show that visual typicality mediates competition between candidate names: high competition, induced by the relationship between the visual properties of the object and the visual representations associated to names, predicts higher naming variation. On a methodological level, we demonstrate the potential of using large-scale datasets with realistic images in conjunction with computational methods to shed light on how people name objects.

不同的说话者通常用不同的名字来指代同一个实体(例如,“woman”和“tennis player”分别指代一个正在打网球的女人)。我们研究视觉典型性如何影响命名行为的变化。我们使用了一种新的计算方法来估计图像的视觉典型性,并分析了包含真实图像命名数据的大型数据集。与以前的工作相反,我们考虑到物体和它出现的场景的视觉属性;并考虑多个候选人的名字。我们发现,视觉典型化介导了候选名字之间的竞争:由物体的视觉属性和与名字相关的视觉表征之间的关系引起的高度竞争预示着更高的命名变化。在方法层面上,我们展示了使用具有逼真图像的大规模数据集与计算方法相结合的潜力,以阐明人们如何命名对象。
{"title":"What’s in a name? A large-scale computational study on how competition between names affects naming variation","authors":"Eleonora Gualdoni ,&nbsp;Thomas Brochhagen ,&nbsp;Andreas Mädebach ,&nbsp;Gemma Boleda","doi":"10.1016/j.jml.2023.104459","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.jml.2023.104459","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Different speakers often use different names to refer to the same entity (e.g., “woman” vs. “tennis player” for a given woman playing tennis). We study how visual typicality affects variation in naming behavior. We use a novel computational approach to estimate visual typicality from images, and analyze a large dataset containing naming data for realistic images. In contrast to previous work, we take into account the visual properties of both the object and the scene in which it appears; and factor in multiple candidate names. We show that visual typicality mediates competition between candidate names: high competition, induced by the relationship between the visual properties of the object and the visual representations associated to names, predicts higher naming variation. On a methodological level, we demonstrate the potential of using large-scale datasets with realistic images in conjunction with computational methods to shed light on how people name objects.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":16493,"journal":{"name":"Journal of memory and language","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":4.3,"publicationDate":"2023-08-31","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"43733436","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
Pragmatic effects on semantic learnability: Insights from evidentiality 语用学对语义可学习性的影响:证据性的启示
IF 4.3 1区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2023-08-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.jml.2023.104426
Dionysia Saratsli , Anna Papafragou

Cross-linguistically prevalent semantic distinctions are widely assumed to be easier to learn because they reflect natural concepts. Here we propose an alternative, pragmatic perspective that links both the cross-linguistic prevalence and the learnability of semantic distinctions to communicative pressures. We focus on evidentiality (the encoding of the speaker’s information source). Across languages, grammatical evidential systems are more likely to encode indirect sources (especially, reported information) compared to direct sources (e.g., visual perception). On a conceptual account, this seems puzzling, since humans reason naturally about how seeing connects to knowing. On a pragmatic account, however, the predominant encoding of the speaker’s reportative compared to visual information sources can be explained in terms of informativeness (visual access is ubiquitous and potentially more reliable, hence less marked). We tested the pragmatic account in four experiments. Adult English speakers exposed to novel miniature evidential morphological systems consistently showed higher learning rates for systems with a single indirect (reportative) compared to a single direct (visual) evidential morpheme (Experiment 1). This pattern persisted even when participants were given specific cues to the target meanings (Experiment 2) and partly extended to cases where evidential meanings were conveyed through visual, not linguistic, means (Experiment 3). It also persisted when the evidential morphemes had to be learned from different materials (Experiment 4). We conclude that the cross-linguistic bias to mark reportative/indirect over visual/direct sources of information has pragmatic roots that also shape the learnability of evidential semantic distinctions.

人们普遍认为,跨语言普遍存在的语义区别更容易学习,因为它们反映了自然概念。在这里,我们提出了另一种语用观点,将跨语言流行和语义区分的可学习性与交际压力联系起来。我们关注的是证据性(说话人信息源的编码)。在各种语言中,与直接来源(如视觉感知)相比,语法证据系统更有可能对间接来源(特别是报告信息)进行编码。从概念上讲,这似乎令人费解,因为人类自然会推理看到和知道之间的联系。然而,从语用角度来看,与视觉信息源相比,说话人报告性的主要编码可以用信息性来解释(视觉获取无处不在,可能更可靠,因此不那么明显)。我们在四个实验中检验了语用说。与单一直接(视觉)证据语素相比,暴露于新型微型证据语素系统的成年英语使用者在单一间接(报告)证据语素系统中始终表现出更高的学习率(实验1)。即使参与者被给予目标意义的特定线索(实验2),这种模式仍然存在,并且部分扩展到通过视觉而非语言传达证据意义的情况。方法(实验3)。当证据语素必须从不同的材料中学习时(实验4),这种情况也会持续存在。我们得出结论,标记报告/间接而不是视觉/直接信息来源的跨语言偏见具有语用根源,这也塑造了证据语义区分的可学习性。
{"title":"Pragmatic effects on semantic learnability: Insights from evidentiality","authors":"Dionysia Saratsli ,&nbsp;Anna Papafragou","doi":"10.1016/j.jml.2023.104426","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.jml.2023.104426","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Cross-linguistically prevalent semantic distinctions are widely assumed to be easier to learn because they reflect natural concepts. Here we propose an alternative, <em>pragmatic</em> perspective that links both the cross-linguistic prevalence and the learnability of semantic distinctions to communicative pressures. We focus on evidentiality (the encoding of the speaker’s information source). Across languages, grammatical evidential systems are more likely to encode indirect sources (especially, reported information) compared to direct sources (e.g., visual perception). On a conceptual account, this seems puzzling, since humans reason naturally about how seeing connects to knowing. On a pragmatic account, however, the predominant encoding of the speaker’s reportative compared to visual information sources can be explained in terms of informativeness (visual access is ubiquitous and potentially more reliable, hence less marked). We tested the pragmatic account in four experiments. Adult English speakers exposed to novel miniature evidential morphological systems consistently showed higher learning rates for systems with a single indirect (reportative) compared to a single direct (visual) evidential morpheme (Experiment 1). This pattern persisted even when participants were given specific cues to the target meanings (Experiment 2) and partly extended to cases where evidential meanings were conveyed through visual, not linguistic, means (Experiment 3). It also persisted when the evidential morphemes had to be learned from different materials (Experiment 4). We conclude that the cross-linguistic bias to mark reportative/indirect over visual/direct sources of information has pragmatic roots that also shape the learnability of evidential semantic distinctions.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":16493,"journal":{"name":"Journal of memory and language","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":4.3,"publicationDate":"2023-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"49484903","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Children and adults use pragmatic principles to interpret non-linguistic symbols 儿童和成人使用语用原则来解释非语言符号
IF 4.3 1区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2023-08-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.jml.2023.104429
Alyssa Kampa , Anna Papafragou

A foundational principle of communication is that speakers should offer as much information as required during conversation. Thus, if a speaker offers a statement with limited information (e.g., “I like the candle” when asked about a gift containing a candle and a sweater), the listener often takes the speaker to imply that a more informative statement (“I like the candle and the sweater”) does not hold. Classic theories of communication have proposed that the principle of informativeness extends to purposeful exchanges beyond linguistic interactions, but relevant evidence so far is limited. In a set of studies, we adopt a simple visual-world paradigm to investigate whether 4- and 5-year-old children and adults expect drawings, like utterances, to be informative in accordance with the creator’s knowledge. We find that 5-year-olds and adults (but not 4-year-olds) apply the principle of informativeness to non-linguistic symbols; furthermore, the 5-year-olds’ success in this task depends on features of the symbols. We discuss the implications of these findings for debates over the mechanisms underlying pragmatic inference, as well as for children’s developing understanding of the symbolic function of drawings.

沟通的一个基本原则是说话者应该在谈话中提供尽可能多的信息。因此,如果说话者提供了一个信息有限的陈述(例如,当被问及一件包含蜡烛和毛衣的礼物时,“我喜欢蜡烛”),听者通常会认为说话者暗示了一个更有信息的陈述(“我喜欢蜡烛和毛衣”)是不成立的。经典的交际理论提出,信息性原则延伸到语言互动之外的有目的的交流,但迄今为止相关证据有限。在一系列研究中,我们采用了一个简单的视觉世界范式来调查4- 5岁的儿童和成人是否期望图画像话语一样,根据创作者的知识提供信息。我们发现,5岁儿童和成人(而不是4岁儿童)对非语言符号适用信息性原则;此外,5岁儿童在这项任务中的成功取决于符号的特征。我们讨论了这些发现对语用推理机制的争论的影响,以及对儿童发展对绘画符号功能的理解。
{"title":"Children and adults use pragmatic principles to interpret non-linguistic symbols","authors":"Alyssa Kampa ,&nbsp;Anna Papafragou","doi":"10.1016/j.jml.2023.104429","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.jml.2023.104429","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>A foundational principle of communication is that speakers should offer as much information as required during conversation. Thus, if a speaker offers a statement with limited information (e.g., “I like the candle” when asked about a gift containing a candle and a sweater), the listener often takes the speaker to imply that a more informative statement (“I like the candle and the sweater”) does not hold. Classic theories of communication have proposed that the principle of informativeness extends to purposeful exchanges beyond linguistic interactions, but relevant evidence so far is limited. In a set of studies, we adopt a simple visual-world paradigm to investigate whether 4- and 5-year-old children and adults expect drawings, like utterances, to be informative in accordance with the creator’s knowledge. We find that 5-year-olds and adults (but not 4-year-olds) apply the principle of informativeness to non-linguistic symbols; furthermore, the 5-year-olds’ success in this task depends on features of the symbols. We discuss the implications of these findings for debates over the mechanisms underlying pragmatic inference, as well as for children’s developing understanding of the symbolic function of drawings.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":16493,"journal":{"name":"Journal of memory and language","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":4.3,"publicationDate":"2023-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"46064036","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Conceptualising acoustic and cognitive contributions to divided-attention listening within a data-limit versus resource-limit framework 在数据限制与资源限制框架内概念化声学和认知对注意力分散听力的贡献
IF 4.3 1区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2023-08-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.jml.2023.104427
Sarah Knight, Lyndon Rakusen, Sven Mattys

An understanding of how listeners divide their attention between two simultaneous talkers requires modelling the interaction between acoustic factors (energetic masking) and cognitive processes (control of auditory attention). The impact of spatial separation between the two talkers on this interaction is unclear, since separation is likely to create both acoustic benefits (release from energetic masking) and cognitive costs (increased demands on spatial attentional control). To explore this question, we manipulated the degree of energetic masking (high vs. low) and spatial separation (collocated to dichotic) between two simultaneous talkers. When energetic masking was high (Experiment 1, unmanipulated talker voices), transcription performance improved monotonically from collocated to dichotic, owing to a gradual release from energetic masking. When energetic masking was low (Experiment 2, bandpass-filtered talker voices), the benefit of spatial separation disappeared; performance even worsened in the dichotic condition. Additionally, across both experiments, individual differences in working memory best predicted transcription performance in conditions where energetic masking was low. These results suggest that energetic masking is the dominant challenge during divided-attention listening, but that the contribution of cognitive control and working memory can be observed when energetic masking is reduced, at least in the context of the current paradigm. The findings are discussed in light of Norman and Bobrow’s (1975) concept of data-limited vs. resource-limited tasks, which we propose is a promising framework for reinterpreting existing results from speech-in-noise perception research.

要理解听者如何在两个同时说话的人之间分配注意力,需要对声学因素(能量掩蔽)和认知过程(听觉注意控制)之间的相互作用进行建模。两个说话者之间的空间分离对这种互动的影响尚不清楚,因为分离可能会产生声学上的好处(释放能量掩蔽)和认知上的成本(增加对空间注意力控制的要求)。为了探讨这个问题,我们操纵了两个同时说话者之间的能量掩蔽程度(高与低)和空间分离(并置到二分法)。当能量掩蔽高时(实验1,未处理的说话声音),由于能量掩蔽的逐渐释放,转录性能从并置到二分性单调提高。当能量掩蔽较低时(实验2,带通滤波的话音),空间分离的优势消失;在双重状态下,表现甚至更差。此外,在这两个实验中,工作记忆的个体差异最好地预测了低能量掩蔽条件下的转录表现。这些结果表明,能量掩蔽是分散注意倾听过程中的主要挑战,但至少在当前范式的背景下,当能量掩蔽减少时,可以观察到认知控制和工作记忆的贡献。研究结果是根据Norman和Bobrow(1975)的数据有限与资源有限任务的概念进行讨论的,我们认为这是一个有希望的框架,可以重新解释噪音中语音感知研究的现有结果。
{"title":"Conceptualising acoustic and cognitive contributions to divided-attention listening within a data-limit versus resource-limit framework","authors":"Sarah Knight,&nbsp;Lyndon Rakusen,&nbsp;Sven Mattys","doi":"10.1016/j.jml.2023.104427","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.jml.2023.104427","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>An understanding of how listeners divide their attention between two simultaneous talkers requires modelling the interaction between acoustic factors (energetic masking) and cognitive processes (control of auditory attention). The impact of spatial separation between the two talkers on this interaction is unclear, since separation is likely to create both acoustic benefits (release from energetic masking) and cognitive costs (increased demands on spatial attentional control). To explore this question, we manipulated the degree of energetic masking (high vs. low) and spatial separation (collocated to dichotic) between two simultaneous talkers. When energetic masking was high (Experiment 1, unmanipulated talker voices), transcription performance improved monotonically from collocated to dichotic, owing to a gradual release from energetic masking. When energetic masking was low (Experiment 2, bandpass-filtered talker voices), the benefit of spatial separation disappeared; performance even worsened in the dichotic condition. Additionally, across both experiments, individual differences in working memory best predicted transcription performance in conditions where energetic masking was low. These results suggest that energetic masking is the dominant challenge during divided-attention listening, but that the contribution of cognitive control and working memory can be observed when energetic masking is reduced, at least in the context of the current paradigm. The findings are discussed in light of Norman and Bobrow’s (1975) concept of data-limited vs. resource-limited tasks, which we propose is a promising framework for reinterpreting existing results from speech-in-noise perception research.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":16493,"journal":{"name":"Journal of memory and language","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":4.3,"publicationDate":"2023-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"45024772","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Corrigendum to “Prediction involves two stages: Evidence from visual-world eye-tracking” [J. Memory Lang. 122 (2022) 104298] “预测涉及两个阶段:来自视觉世界眼动追踪的证据”的更正[J.Memory Lang.122(2022)104298]
IF 4.3 1区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2023-08-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.jml.2023.104431
Ruth E. Corps , Charlotte Brooke , Martin J. Pickering
{"title":"Corrigendum to “Prediction involves two stages: Evidence from visual-world eye-tracking” [J. Memory Lang. 122 (2022) 104298]","authors":"Ruth E. Corps ,&nbsp;Charlotte Brooke ,&nbsp;Martin J. Pickering","doi":"10.1016/j.jml.2023.104431","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.jml.2023.104431","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":16493,"journal":{"name":"Journal of memory and language","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":4.3,"publicationDate":"2023-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"45467526","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The head constituent plays a key role in the lexical boost in syntactic priming 头成分在句法启动中的词汇提升中起着关键作用
IF 4.3 1区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2023-08-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.jml.2023.104416
Jian Huang , Xiqin Liu , Meiling Lu , Yingying Sun , Suiping Wang , Holly P. Branigan , Martin J. Pickering

Accounts of language production make different predictions about the conditions under which structural priming should be enhanced by lexical repetition (the lexical boost). Repetition of the head verb strongly enhances structural priming of a sentence, but studies of English have found contradictory results regarding the effects of noun repetition. In two experiments, Mandarin participants read a prime sentence aloud and then produced a target picture description of a dative event. In Experiment 1, the verb was printed on the target picture, and we found that repetition of the verb enhanced priming (vs. no repetition) but repetition of the agent, theme, or recipient argument did not. In Experiment 2, both the agent noun and the verb were printed on the picture, and we found that verb repetition enhanced priming but agent repetition did not. These results indicate that the lexical boost is restricted to the head verb in Mandarin and therefore support lemma-based residual activation accounts of language production in which activation of a head leads to activation of its associated grammatical construction.

关于在何种条件下结构启动应该通过词汇重复(词汇提升)得到加强,语言产生的各种说法做出了不同的预测。首动词的重复可以增强句子的结构启动,但是关于名词重复的影响,英语研究发现了矛盾的结果。在两个实验中,普通话参与者大声朗读一个基本句子,然后制作一个与格事件的目标图片描述。在实验1中,动词被印在目标图片上,我们发现动词的重复增强了启动(与不重复相比),但重复的主体、主题或接受者参数没有。在实验2中,在图片上同时印上代理名词和动词,我们发现动词重复对启动有促进作用,而代理重复对启动没有促进作用。这些结果表明,汉语词汇量的增加仅限于头部动词,因此支持基于引理的语言生成残余激活说,即头部的激活导致相关语法结构的激活。
{"title":"The head constituent plays a key role in the lexical boost in syntactic priming","authors":"Jian Huang ,&nbsp;Xiqin Liu ,&nbsp;Meiling Lu ,&nbsp;Yingying Sun ,&nbsp;Suiping Wang ,&nbsp;Holly P. Branigan ,&nbsp;Martin J. Pickering","doi":"10.1016/j.jml.2023.104416","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.jml.2023.104416","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Accounts of language production make different predictions about the conditions under which structural priming should be enhanced by lexical repetition (the lexical boost). Repetition of the head verb strongly enhances structural priming of a sentence, but studies of English have found contradictory results regarding the effects of noun repetition. In two experiments, Mandarin participants read a prime sentence aloud and then produced a target picture description of a dative event. In Experiment 1, the verb was printed on the target picture, and we found that repetition of the verb enhanced priming (vs. no repetition) but repetition of the agent, theme, or recipient argument did not. In Experiment 2, both the agent noun and the verb were printed on the picture, and we found that verb repetition enhanced priming but agent repetition did not. These results indicate that the lexical boost is restricted to the head verb in Mandarin and therefore support lemma-based residual activation accounts of language production in which activation of a head leads to activation of its associated grammatical construction.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":16493,"journal":{"name":"Journal of memory and language","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":4.3,"publicationDate":"2023-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"46895467","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 4
Haven’t I seen you before? Conceptual but not perceptual prior familiarity enhances face recognition memory 我以前没见过你吗?概念而非感知的先验熟悉增强了人脸识别记忆
IF 4.3 1区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2023-08-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.jml.2023.104433
Melisa Akan, Aaron S. Benjamin

Prior familiarity with a face seems to substantively change the way we encode and recognize later instances of that face. We report five experiments that examine the effects of varying levels of prior familiarity and conceptual knowledge on face recognition memory. All experiments employed a 3-phase procedure, in which faces were familiarized in varying ways and to varying extents prior to study and test. Across experiments, increased prior familiarity led to a simultaneous increase in both correct and false identification rates, either when familiarity was gained through passive exposures or conceptual processing. Discriminability, on the other hand, was enhanced by prior familiarity only when the level of familiarity was high and when it involved conceptual processing (Experiments 1–3). Familiarity engendered by passive exposure affected response bias equivalently to more active orienting tasks, but it reduced discriminability in a standard Old/New recognition test (Experiment 4) and did not lead to an enhancement in discriminability in a lineup identification task (Experiment 5). Familiarity engendered by trait evaluations (Experiments 1–3) or name learning (Experiments 2–5) increased discriminability and yielded a more liberal response bias. These results suggest that the benefits of prior familiarity on discriminability in recognition memory are determined by the presence of prior conceptual knowledge. The implications of this work for eyewitness identification situations in which the suspect is known or familiar to the witness are discussed.

先前对一张脸的熟悉程度似乎从本质上改变了我们对这张脸的编码和识别方式。我们报告了五个实验,研究了不同程度的先验熟悉度和概念知识对人脸识别记忆的影响。所有的实验都采用了三个阶段的程序,在研究和测试之前,以不同的方式和程度熟悉面孔。在实验中,无论是通过被动接触还是概念加工获得熟悉度,增加的先验熟悉度都会导致正确和错误识别率同时增加。另一方面,只有当熟悉程度较高且涉及概念加工时,可判别性才会被先前熟悉所增强(实验1-3)。被动暴露产生的熟悉度对更主动定向任务的反应偏倚的影响相当,但它降低了标准新旧识别测试(实验4)的判别性,并没有导致队列识别任务(实验5)的判别性增强。由特质评估(实验1-3)或名称学习(实验2-5)产生的熟悉度增加了判别性,并产生了更自由的反应偏倚。这些结果表明,先验熟悉度对识别记忆中可判别性的影响是由先验概念知识的存在决定的。讨论了这项工作对证人识别情况的影响,其中嫌疑人是已知的或熟悉的证人。
{"title":"Haven’t I seen you before? Conceptual but not perceptual prior familiarity enhances face recognition memory","authors":"Melisa Akan,&nbsp;Aaron S. Benjamin","doi":"10.1016/j.jml.2023.104433","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.jml.2023.104433","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Prior familiarity with a face seems to substantively change the way we encode and recognize later instances of that face. We report five experiments that examine the effects of varying levels of prior familiarity and conceptual knowledge on face recognition memory. All experiments employed a 3-phase procedure, in which faces were familiarized in varying ways and to varying extents prior to study and test. Across experiments, increased prior familiarity led to a simultaneous increase in both correct and false identification rates, either when familiarity was gained through passive exposures or conceptual processing. Discriminability, on the other hand, was enhanced by prior familiarity only when the level of familiarity was high and when it involved conceptual processing (Experiments 1–3). Familiarity engendered by passive exposure affected response bias equivalently to more active orienting tasks, but it <em>reduced</em> discriminability in a standard Old/New recognition test (Experiment 4) and did not lead to an enhancement in discriminability in a lineup identification task (Experiment 5). Familiarity engendered by trait evaluations (Experiments 1–3) or name learning (Experiments 2–5) increased discriminability and yielded a more liberal response bias. These results suggest that the benefits of prior familiarity on discriminability in recognition memory are determined by the presence of prior conceptual knowledge. The implications of this work for eyewitness identification situations in which the suspect is known or familiar to the witness are discussed.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":16493,"journal":{"name":"Journal of memory and language","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":4.3,"publicationDate":"2023-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"49564787","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Perceiving speech during orthographic syllable recognition: Beyond phonemic identity 正字法音节识别过程中的言语感知:超越音位同一性
IF 4.3 1区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2023-08-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.jml.2023.104430
Daniel Williams , Adamantios Gafos , Payam Ghaffarvand-Mokari

In the cue-distractor paradigm, individuals observe a spoken distractor syllable while responding to a visual cue referring to a syllable. When the task is to utter the cued syllable, distractors sharing fewer subphonemic properties with the cued syllable (below the level of phonemes) lead to slower reaction times (RTs), indicating representations involved in speech perception and production are closely linked. The present study investigated whether a subphonemic level of representation is involved when the task was to manually indicate (but not produce) an orthographically cued syllable. Results revealed RT modulations closely mirroring those reported previously for uttered responses. In an additional experiment, phonetic variants of phonologically identical distractors were presented, but RT modulations were unaffected by this manipulation. The present findings indicate that perceiving speech accesses a relatively detailed phonological level of representation which is closely aligned with representations pertinent in orthographic syllable recognition and in speech production.

在线索-干扰物范式中,个体在对与音节相关的视觉线索做出反应的同时,观察到一个口头干扰音节。当任务是说出提示音节时,干扰物与提示音节共享较少的次音素属性(低于音素水平)导致反应时间(RTs)变慢,这表明涉及语音感知和产生的表征密切相关。本研究调查了当任务是手动指示(但不产生)正字法提示音节时,是否涉及次音素水平的表征。结果显示,RT调制密切反映了先前报道的发出反应。在另一项实验中,语音相同的干扰物的语音变体被呈现,但RT调制不受这种操纵的影响。目前的研究结果表明,语音感知进入了一个相对详细的语音表征水平,这与正字法音节识别和语音产生相关的表征密切相关。
{"title":"Perceiving speech during orthographic syllable recognition: Beyond phonemic identity","authors":"Daniel Williams ,&nbsp;Adamantios Gafos ,&nbsp;Payam Ghaffarvand-Mokari","doi":"10.1016/j.jml.2023.104430","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2023.104430","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>In the cue-distractor paradigm, individuals observe a spoken distractor syllable while responding to a visual cue referring to a syllable. When the task is to utter the cued syllable, distractors sharing fewer subphonemic properties with the cued syllable (below the level of phonemes) lead to slower reaction times (RTs), indicating representations involved in speech perception and production are closely linked. The present study investigated whether a subphonemic level of representation is involved when the task was to manually indicate (but not produce) an orthographically cued syllable. Results revealed RT modulations closely mirroring those reported previously for uttered responses. In an additional experiment, phonetic variants of phonologically identical distractors were presented, but RT modulations were unaffected by this manipulation. The present findings indicate that perceiving speech accesses a relatively detailed phonological level of representation which is closely aligned with representations pertinent in orthographic syllable recognition and in speech production.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":16493,"journal":{"name":"Journal of memory and language","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":4.3,"publicationDate":"2023-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"49905972","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Journal of memory and language
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1