首页 > 最新文献

Neuropsychology最新文献

英文 中文
Dimensional attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder symptoms and executive functioning in adolescence: A multi-informant, population-based twin study. 青少年时期的多维注意缺陷/多动障碍症状和执行功能:一项基于人口的多信息双胞胎研究。
IF 2.6 3区 心理学 Q3 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-11-18 DOI: 10.1037/neu0000983
Laila Ketvel, Eero Vuoksimaa, Lea Pulkkinen, Richard J Rose, Ulla Vedenkannas, Pekka Rapeli, Anu Raevuori, Antti Latvala

Objective: To investigate associations of executive function (EF) performance in adolescence with dimensional symptoms of inattention and hyperactivity-impulsivity assessed by multiple informants as well as ADHD (attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder) symptoms based on the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders criteria, and whether familial factors and co-occurring symptoms of depressive disorder and conduct disorder explain these associations in a population-based sample.

Method: In 14-year-old twins from the population-based FinnTwin12 study (N = 638-1,227), we assessed EF with commonly used neuropsychological tests. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders symptoms of ADHD and psychiatric disorders were assessed with a semistructured interview, and dimensional symptoms of inattention and hyperactivity-impulsivity with behavioral ratings made by the twins, their co-twins, and teachers at age 14, and by parents and teachers at age 12, the latter being different from those at age 14.

Results: Teacher-rated inattention had the strongest association with poorer EF performance across two measurement points with different teachers; these associations were not affected by adding symptoms of depressive disorder and conduct disorder as covariates. Within-pair analyses suggested that the associations of inattention and hyperactivity-impulsivity with EF were partly explained by familial factors.

Conclusion: Even at a subclinical level in a sample of adolescents representing general population, ADHD symptoms are associated with EF performance. Teachers' evaluations appear especially valuable when assessing adolescents' ADHD symptoms. Our findings support the notion of dimensional ADHD symptoms in the population. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

目的调查青少年时期执行功能(EF)表现与多方信息提供者评估的注意力不集中和多动冲动症状以及基于《精神疾病诊断与统计手册》标准的注意力缺陷/多动障碍(ADHD)症状之间的关联,以及在基于人口的样本中,家族因素和抑郁障碍与行为障碍的共存症状是否可以解释这些关联:方法:在基于人口的芬兰双胞胎研究(FinnTwin12)(N = 638-1,227 人)中的 14 岁双胞胎中,我们使用常用的神经心理学测试评估了 EF。通过半结构化访谈评估了《精神障碍诊断与统计手册》中的多动症和精神障碍症状,并通过双胞胎、同卵双胞胎和教师在14岁时的行为评分评估了注意力不集中和多动冲动的维度症状,以及家长和教师在12岁时的行为评分,后者与14岁时的评分有所不同:在不同教师的两个测量点上,教师评定的注意力不集中与较差的EF表现有最密切的联系;将抑郁障碍和行为障碍症状作为协变量并不影响这些联系。对内分析表明,注意力不集中和多动-冲动与EF的关联部分是由家庭因素造成的:结论:在代表普通人群的青少年样本中,即使在亚临床水平上,ADHD症状也与EF表现相关。在评估青少年的多动症症状时,教师的评价显得尤为重要。我们的研究结果支持在人群中存在多维ADHD症状的概念。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Dimensional attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder symptoms and executive functioning in adolescence: A multi-informant, population-based twin study.","authors":"Laila Ketvel, Eero Vuoksimaa, Lea Pulkkinen, Richard J Rose, Ulla Vedenkannas, Pekka Rapeli, Anu Raevuori, Antti Latvala","doi":"10.1037/neu0000983","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/neu0000983","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Objective: </strong>To investigate associations of executive function (EF) performance in adolescence with dimensional symptoms of inattention and hyperactivity-impulsivity assessed by multiple informants as well as ADHD (attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder) symptoms based on the <i>Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders</i> criteria, and whether familial factors and co-occurring symptoms of depressive disorder and conduct disorder explain these associations in a population-based sample.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>In 14-year-old twins from the population-based FinnTwin12 study (<i>N</i> = 638-1,227), we assessed EF with commonly used neuropsychological tests. <i>Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders</i> symptoms of ADHD and psychiatric disorders were assessed with a semistructured interview, and dimensional symptoms of inattention and hyperactivity-impulsivity with behavioral ratings made by the twins, their co-twins, and teachers at age 14, and by parents and teachers at age 12, the latter being different from those at age 14.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>Teacher-rated inattention had the strongest association with poorer EF performance across two measurement points with different teachers; these associations were not affected by adding symptoms of depressive disorder and conduct disorder as covariates. Within-pair analyses suggested that the associations of inattention and hyperactivity-impulsivity with EF were partly explained by familial factors.</p><p><strong>Conclusion: </strong>Even at a subclinical level in a sample of adolescents representing general population, ADHD symptoms are associated with EF performance. Teachers' evaluations appear especially valuable when assessing adolescents' ADHD symptoms. Our findings support the notion of dimensional ADHD symptoms in the population. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":19205,"journal":{"name":"Neuropsychology","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-11-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142648499","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Capturing cognitive capacity in the everyday environment across a continuum of cognitive decline using a smartwatch n-back task and ecological momentary assessment. 利用智能手表 n-back 任务和生态瞬间评估,在认知能力下降的连续过程中捕捉日常生活环境中的认知能力。
IF 2.6 3区 心理学 Q3 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-11-18 DOI: 10.1037/neu0000984
Maureen Schmitter-Edgecombe, Catherine Luna, Brooke Beech, Shenghai Dai, Diane J Cook

Objective: Cognitive testing using mobile technologies can assist with early detection of cognitive decline. We use ecological momentary assessment to investigate the feasibility, reliability, and validity of a smartwatch n-back task (1-back) delivered to adults across the cognitive continuum.

Method: One hundred seventy-four community-dwelling individuals (Mage = 70.51) representing healthy older adults, individuals with subjective cognitive decline, and mild cognitive impairment completed a neurocognitive assessment battery and wore a smartwatch for 7+ days. Participants were prompted 4 times per day to complete an n-back task on the smartwatch.

Results: Across all groups, findings indicated an acceptable task adherence rate (> 78%; n = 174) and response rate (> 89%; n = 158 n-back analysis sample). Supporting external validity, participants with mild cognitive impairment were less accurate, had fewer total correct responses, and performed at lower initial levels than both healthy older adults and subjective cognitive decline, ω²s > .09. Intraindividual variability was greater for the mild cognitive impairment group compared to healthy older adults, but subjective cognitive decline did not differ significantly from either group, ω² = .12. For discriminant and convergent validity, n-back total correct correlated with performance on standardized assessments of executive attention, whereas intraindividual variability correlated with real-world factors (i.e., context, everyday function). Reliability assessment revealed stability for n-back measures after four to six posttraining trials and excellent test-retest reliability for total correct after 5 months. Finally, combining n-back and clinical measures improved classification accuracy.

Conclusions: Findings suggest the smartwatch n-back task is feasible for collecting cognitive data across the cognitive continuum with demonstrated reliability and validity in the everyday environment using ecological momentary assessment. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

目的:利用移动技术进行认知测试有助于早期发现认知能力下降。我们采用生态学瞬间评估方法,研究了智能手表n-back任务(1-back)对不同认知能力的成年人的可行性、可靠性和有效性:174名居住在社区的健康老年人、主观认知能力下降者和轻度认知障碍者(Mage = 70.51)完成了神经认知评估电池,并佩戴智能手表7天以上。每天提示参与者在智能手表上完成 4 次 "n-back "任务:在所有组别中,研究结果表明任务坚持率(> 78%;n = 174)和响应率(> 89%;n = 158 n-back分析样本)均可接受。与健康老年人和主观认知能力下降者相比,轻度认知障碍参与者的准确率较低、总正确率较低、初始水平较低,ω²s > .09,这证明了外部有效性。与健康老年人相比,轻度认知障碍组的个体内部变异性更大,但主观认知能力下降组与这两组没有显著差异(ω² = .12)。在判别效度和收敛效度方面,n-back总正确率与执行注意力标准化评估的表现相关,而个体内变异性则与现实世界因素(即情境、日常功能)相关。可靠性评估显示,经过四到六次训练后,n-back 测量结果保持稳定,5 个月后,总正确率的测试-再测试可靠性极佳。最后,N-back和临床测量相结合提高了分类的准确性:研究结果表明,智能手表的n-back任务可用于收集整个认知连续体的认知数据,在日常环境中使用生态瞬时评估的可靠性和有效性已得到证实。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Capturing cognitive capacity in the everyday environment across a continuum of cognitive decline using a smartwatch n-back task and ecological momentary assessment.","authors":"Maureen Schmitter-Edgecombe, Catherine Luna, Brooke Beech, Shenghai Dai, Diane J Cook","doi":"10.1037/neu0000984","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/neu0000984","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Objective: </strong>Cognitive testing using mobile technologies can assist with early detection of cognitive decline. We use ecological momentary assessment to investigate the feasibility, reliability, and validity of a smartwatch n-back task (1-back) delivered to adults across the cognitive continuum.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>One hundred seventy-four community-dwelling individuals (<i>M</i><sub>age</sub> = 70.51) representing healthy older adults, individuals with subjective cognitive decline, and mild cognitive impairment completed a neurocognitive assessment battery and wore a smartwatch for 7+ days. Participants were prompted 4 times per day to complete an n-back task on the smartwatch.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>Across all groups, findings indicated an acceptable task adherence rate (> 78%; <i>n</i> = 174) and response rate (> 89%; <i>n</i> = 158 <i>n</i>-back analysis sample). Supporting external validity, participants with mild cognitive impairment were less accurate, had fewer total correct responses, and performed at lower initial levels than both healthy older adults and subjective cognitive decline, ω²s > .09. Intraindividual variability was greater for the mild cognitive impairment group compared to healthy older adults, but subjective cognitive decline did not differ significantly from either group, ω² = .12. For discriminant and convergent validity, <i>n</i>-back total correct correlated with performance on standardized assessments of executive attention, whereas intraindividual variability correlated with real-world factors (i.e., context, everyday function). Reliability assessment revealed stability for <i>n</i>-back measures after four to six posttraining trials and excellent test-retest reliability for total correct after 5 months. Finally, combining <i>n</i>-back and clinical measures improved classification accuracy.</p><p><strong>Conclusions: </strong>Findings suggest the smartwatch <i>n</i>-back task is feasible for collecting cognitive data across the cognitive continuum with demonstrated reliability and validity in the everyday environment using ecological momentary assessment. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":19205,"journal":{"name":"Neuropsychology","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-11-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142648497","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Affliction class moderates the dementing impact of adipokines. 痛苦等级可减轻脂肪因子对痴呆症的影响。
IF 2.6 3区 心理学 Q3 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-11-14 DOI: 10.1037/neu0000968
Donald R Royall, Raymond F Palmer

Objective: Biomarker-specific interventions (e.g., for dementia) will necessitate an individualized approach to treatment. We have constructed a psychometric classifier to identify persons adversely impacted by plasma adipokines.

Method: The subjects (N = 1,737) of the Alzheimer's Disease Neuroimaging Initiative were assigned to groups "afflicted" by versus "resilient" against the unique effect of plasma adipokines using a classifier derived by confirmatory factor analysis in a structural equation model framework. The impact of affliction class above and beyond observed biomarker levels and covariates was tested by multivariate regression using CDR "Sum of Boxes" as the dependent variable. The affliction class' moderation of adipokines' effect was tested by chi-square difference. The effect of affliction class on prospective conversion risk was tested by Cox's proportional hazards models.

Results: Seven hundred four out of the 1,737 subjects (40.53%) were assigned to the afflicted class. The afflicted subjects had greater dementia severity, lower (adverse) Adipokines factor composite scores (by analysis of variance, F(1, 1,735) 2619.68, p < .001) and higher observed levels of plasma adipokines (by Tukey's honestly significant difference test, all p < .001). Adipokines' association with dementia severity was moderated by affliction class. The effect persisted at 48 months. Afflicted cases were more likely to convert to Alzheimer's disease in that timeframe, by Cox's F: F(234, 286) = 3.89, p < .001.

Conclusions: Our approach could guide precision interventions against specific biomarkers. This classifier could be administered by telephone, making class assignment feasible without direct patient contact or biomarker assessment. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

目标:针对特定生物标志物的干预措施(如针对痴呆症的干预措施)需要采用个体化的治疗方法。我们构建了一个心理测量分类器来识别受血浆脂肪因子不利影响的人群:方法:通过结构方程模型框架下的确证因子分析得出的分类器,将阿尔茨海默病神经影像研究项目的受试者(1,737 人)分配到受血浆脂肪因子独特影响的 "受影响 "组和 "有抵抗力 "组。以 CDR "方框总和 "为因变量,通过多变量回归测试了在观察到的生物标志物水平和协变量之外,疾病类别的影响。痛苦等级对脂肪因子影响的调节作用通过卡方差进行检验。用 Cox 比例危险度模型检验了疾病等级对预期转归风险的影响:在 1737 名受试者中,有 74 人(40.53%)被分到了受苦受难组。罹患痴呆症的受试者痴呆症严重程度更高,脂肪因子综合得分(不利)更低(方差分析,F(1,1 735)2619.68,p < .001),血浆脂肪因子的观察水平更高(Tukey诚实显著差异检验,均为p < .001)。脂肪因子与痴呆症严重程度的关系受到疾病等级的影响。这种影响在 48 个月后依然存在。根据 Cox's F:F(234, 286)= 3.89,p < .001,患病病例在48个月内更有可能转为阿尔茨海默病:我们的方法可以指导针对特定生物标志物的精准干预。该分类器可通过电话进行管理,因此无需直接接触患者或进行生物标志物评估即可进行分级。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Affliction class moderates the dementing impact of adipokines.","authors":"Donald R Royall, Raymond F Palmer","doi":"10.1037/neu0000968","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/neu0000968","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Objective: </strong>Biomarker-specific interventions (e.g., for dementia) will necessitate an individualized approach to treatment. We have constructed a psychometric classifier to identify persons adversely impacted by plasma adipokines.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>The subjects (<i>N</i> = 1,737) of the Alzheimer's Disease Neuroimaging Initiative were assigned to groups \"afflicted\" by versus \"resilient\" against the unique effect of plasma adipokines using a classifier derived by confirmatory factor analysis in a structural equation model framework. The impact of affliction class above and beyond observed biomarker levels and covariates was tested by multivariate regression using CDR \"Sum of Boxes\" as the dependent variable. The affliction class' moderation of adipokines' effect was tested by chi-square difference. The effect of affliction class on prospective conversion risk was tested by Cox's proportional hazards models.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>Seven hundred four out of the 1,737 subjects (40.53%) were assigned to the afflicted class. The afflicted subjects had greater dementia severity, lower (adverse) <i>Adipokines</i> factor composite scores (by analysis of variance, <i>F</i>(1, 1,735) 2619.68, <i>p</i> < .001) and higher observed levels of plasma adipokines (by Tukey's honestly significant difference test, all <i>p</i> < .001). <i>Adipokines</i>' association with dementia severity was moderated by affliction class. The effect persisted at 48 months. Afflicted cases were more likely to convert to Alzheimer's disease in that timeframe, by Cox's <i>F</i>: <i>F</i>(234, 286) = 3.89, <i>p</i> < .001.</p><p><strong>Conclusions: </strong>Our approach could guide precision interventions against specific biomarkers. This classifier could be administered by telephone, making class assignment feasible without direct patient contact or biomarker assessment. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":19205,"journal":{"name":"Neuropsychology","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-11-14","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142624954","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Metabolic syndrome and mobility dysfunction in older adults with and without histories of traumatic brain injury: The mediating role of cognition. 有脑外伤史和无脑外伤史的老年人的代谢综合征和行动功能障碍:认知的中介作用
IF 2.6 3区 心理学 Q3 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-11-07 DOI: 10.1037/neu0000980
Timothy Fellin, Jessica Paxton, Adrian Thomas, Susan Torres-Harding

Objective: Older adults are more susceptible than the general population to developing metabolic syndrome (i.e., three or more cardiovascular risk factors [MetS]), physical limitations, and traumatic brain injury (TBI). Recent research has demonstrated that cognition may moderate the negative association between MetS and mobility dysfunction in older adults. This study sought to determine if cognition mediates the relationship between MetS and mobility dysfunction and if this relationship differs in older adults with a history of TBI.

Method: Participants (N = 20,156) were from the National Alzheimer's Coordinating Center's Uniform Data Set. Mediation models were tested to assess if processing speed, executive functioning, immediate memory, and delayed memory would independently mediate the association between MetS and mobility dysfunction.

Results: Investigation of mediation models revealed that MetS had a significant indirect effect on mobility dysfunction through processing speed (b = .0674, 95% CI [.0412, .0953]) and executive functioning (b = .0354, 95% CI [.0228, .0493]). When TBI was included in the model as a moderator, MetS was not found to moderate the mediating effects of any of the cognitive variables. There were no significant indirect effects for immediate or delayed memory in either model.

Conclusions: Findings elucidate potential pathways by which MetS contributes to mobility dysfunction in older adults through specific reductions in processing speed and executive functioning capabilities. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

目的:老年人比普通人更容易患上代谢综合征(即三种或三种以上心血管风险因素 [MetS])、身体机能限制和创伤性脑损伤(TBI)。最近的研究表明,认知能力可能会缓和老年人代谢综合征与行动功能障碍之间的负面关系。本研究旨在确定认知是否能调节 MetS 与行动障碍之间的关系,以及这种关系在有 TBI 病史的老年人中是否存在差异:参与者(N = 20,156 人)来自国家阿尔茨海默氏症协调中心的统一数据集。对中介模型进行了测试,以评估处理速度、执行功能、即时记忆和延迟记忆是否会独立中介 MetS 与行动障碍之间的关系:对中介模型的调查显示,MetS通过处理速度(b = .0674,95% CI [.0412, .0953])和执行功能(b = .0354,95% CI [.0228, .0493])对行动障碍有显著的间接影响。当将创伤性脑损伤作为调节因子纳入模型时,未发现 MetS 对任何认知变量的调节作用具有调节作用。在这两个模型中,即时或延迟记忆都没有明显的间接效应:研究结果阐明了MetS通过特定的处理速度和执行功能能力下降导致老年人行动障碍的潜在途径。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Metabolic syndrome and mobility dysfunction in older adults with and without histories of traumatic brain injury: The mediating role of cognition.","authors":"Timothy Fellin, Jessica Paxton, Adrian Thomas, Susan Torres-Harding","doi":"10.1037/neu0000980","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/neu0000980","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Objective: </strong>Older adults are more susceptible than the general population to developing metabolic syndrome (i.e., three or more cardiovascular risk factors [MetS]), physical limitations, and traumatic brain injury (TBI). Recent research has demonstrated that cognition may moderate the negative association between MetS and mobility dysfunction in older adults. This study sought to determine if cognition mediates the relationship between MetS and mobility dysfunction and if this relationship differs in older adults with a history of TBI.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>Participants (<i>N</i> = 20,156) were from the National Alzheimer's Coordinating Center's Uniform Data Set. Mediation models were tested to assess if processing speed, executive functioning, immediate memory, and delayed memory would independently mediate the association between MetS and mobility dysfunction.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>Investigation of mediation models revealed that MetS had a significant indirect effect on mobility dysfunction through processing speed (<i>b</i> = .0674, 95% CI [.0412, .0953]) and executive functioning (<i>b</i> = .0354, 95% CI [.0228, .0493]). When TBI was included in the model as a moderator, MetS was not found to moderate the mediating effects of any of the cognitive variables. There were no significant indirect effects for immediate or delayed memory in either model.</p><p><strong>Conclusions: </strong>Findings elucidate potential pathways by which MetS contributes to mobility dysfunction in older adults through specific reductions in processing speed and executive functioning capabilities. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":19205,"journal":{"name":"Neuropsychology","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-11-07","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142605772","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Association between social networks and cognition among middle-aged and older adults in rural India. 印度农村中老年人社交网络与认知能力之间的关系。
IF 2.6 3区 心理学 Q3 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-11-01 DOI: 10.1037/neu0000971
Pooja Rai, Jonas S Sundarakumar

Objective: Prior studies using global cognitive measures have shown that social connectedness is linked with cognitive performance. We investigate the role of different social network dimensions on performance across distinct cognitive domains among dementia-free middle-aged and older rural Indians.

Method: We utilized baseline assessment data of 2,525 participants (≥45 years) of an ongoing, prospective, aging cohort in rural India (Srinivaspura Aging, Neuro Senescence, and COGnition) for this study. We evaluated social networks using Cohen's Social Network Index on three dimensions: network diversity, network size, and network embeddedness. We measured cognitive performance on memory (narrative recall), visuospatial ability (geometric figure spatial recognition, visuospatial span), language (verbal fluency, semantic association, word comprehension, reading comprehension), and attention domains of a culturally adapted, computerized, neurocognitive test battery. Linear regression models, adjusted for age and sex, were used for statistical analysis.

Results: In the linear regression model adjusted for age and sex, we found that greater network diversity was significantly associated with better performance in narrative recall, geometric figure spatial recognition, reading comprehension, semantic association, and attention tests. Participants with a larger network size had significantly better performance in verbal fluency and semantic association tests. Further, participants with greater network embeddedness had better visuospatial span and verbal fluency.

Conclusions: Individuals having a greater network diversity, larger network size, and greater network embeddedness had better cognitive performance in multiple distinct cognitive domains. Following up these participants with serial cognitive monitoring can help understand if social networks play a role in delaying cognitive decline and protecting against dementia. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

研究目的先前使用全球认知测量方法进行的研究表明,社会联系与认知表现有关。我们研究了不同社会网络维度对未患痴呆症的中老年印度农村人在不同认知领域的表现所起的作用:我们利用印度农村正在进行的前瞻性老龄化队列(Srinivaspura Aging, Neuro Senescence, and COGnition)中 2525 名参与者(≥45 岁)的基线评估数据进行了研究。我们使用科恩社会网络指数(Cohen's Social Network Index)从网络多样性、网络规模和网络嵌入度三个维度对社会网络进行了评估。我们测量了记忆(叙事回忆)、视觉空间能力(几何图形空间识别、视觉空间跨度)、语言(语言流畅性、语义联想、词语理解、阅读理解)和注意力等方面的认知表现,这些都是根据文化背景进行调整的计算机化神经认知测试。统计分析采用线性回归模型,并对年龄和性别进行了调整:在调整了年龄和性别的线性回归模型中,我们发现网络多样性越大,在叙述回忆、几何图形空间识别、阅读理解、语义联想和注意力测试中的表现就越好。网络规模较大的参与者在语言流畅性和语义联想测试中的表现明显更好。此外,网络嵌入度越高的受试者,其视觉空间跨度和言语流畅性也越好:结论:网络多样性更强、网络规模更大和网络嵌入度更高的个体在多个不同的认知领域都有更好的认知表现。对这些参与者进行连续的认知监测有助于了解社交网络是否在延缓认知衰退和预防痴呆症方面发挥作用。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Association between social networks and cognition among middle-aged and older adults in rural India.","authors":"Pooja Rai, Jonas S Sundarakumar","doi":"10.1037/neu0000971","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/neu0000971","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Objective: </strong>Prior studies using global cognitive measures have shown that social connectedness is linked with cognitive performance. We investigate the role of different social network dimensions on performance across distinct cognitive domains among dementia-free middle-aged and older rural Indians.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>We utilized baseline assessment data of 2,525 participants (≥45 years) of an ongoing, prospective, aging cohort in rural India (Srinivaspura Aging, Neuro Senescence, and COGnition) for this study. We evaluated social networks using Cohen's Social Network Index on three dimensions: network diversity, network size, and network embeddedness. We measured cognitive performance on memory (narrative recall), visuospatial ability (geometric figure spatial recognition, visuospatial span), language (verbal fluency, semantic association, word comprehension, reading comprehension), and attention domains of a culturally adapted, computerized, neurocognitive test battery. Linear regression models, adjusted for age and sex, were used for statistical analysis.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>In the linear regression model adjusted for age and sex, we found that greater network diversity was significantly associated with better performance in narrative recall, geometric figure spatial recognition, reading comprehension, semantic association, and attention tests. Participants with a larger network size had significantly better performance in verbal fluency and semantic association tests. Further, participants with greater network embeddedness had better visuospatial span and verbal fluency.</p><p><strong>Conclusions: </strong>Individuals having a greater network diversity, larger network size, and greater network embeddedness had better cognitive performance in multiple distinct cognitive domains. Following up these participants with serial cognitive monitoring can help understand if social networks play a role in delaying cognitive decline and protecting against dementia. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":19205,"journal":{"name":"Neuropsychology","volume":"38 8","pages":"740-748"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142546471","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Executive function is associated with the development of math performance in children born at <30 weeks' gestation or <1250 g birthweight. 执行功能与妊娠期小于 30 周或出生体重小于 1250 克的儿童的数学成绩发展有关。
IF 2.6 3区 心理学 Q3 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-11-01 DOI: 10.1037/neu0000970
Simonne E Collins, Alice C Burnett, Philippa Pyman, Rheanna M Mainzer, Leona Pascoe, Kristina M Haebich, Jeanie L Y Cheong, Lex W Doyle, Deanne K Thompson, Peter J Anderson

Objective: To describe associations between executive function (EF) domains (attentional control, information processing, cognitive flexibility, and goal setting) and concurrent math computation performance at age 7 and 13 years in children born <30 weeks' gestation or weighing <1,250 g, and second, to examine the impact of 7-year EF on math performance at 13 years.

Method: In a prospective, longitudinal cohort of children born <30 weeks' gestation or with a birthweight <1,250 g, assessment of EF and math performance was undertaken at 7 (n = 187) and 13 years (n = 174). Linear regression models were used to describe associations between EF domains with math performance at both time points, as well as to examine the impact of EF at 7 years on math performance at 13 years.

Results: At 7 and 13 years, all EF domains were positively and strongly associated with concurrent math performance (β = 11.35, 95% CI [9.28, 13.41] to β = 13.79, 95% CI [11.59, 15.98]). All EF domains at age 7 years were positively associated with math performance at 13 years, with the strongest associations observed for cognitive flexibility (β = 10.79 [8.64, 12.94]) and goal setting (β = 10.37 [8.08, 12.67]).

Conclusions: This study provides evidence that EF is strongly associated with math performance in children born <30 weeks' gestation or with a birthweight <1,250 g and highlights the importance of early cognitive flexibility and goal setting performance for future math performance. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

目的描述出生儿童的执行功能(EF)领域(注意控制、信息处理、认知灵活性和目标设定)与 7 岁和 13 岁时同时进行的数学计算成绩之间的关系:对出生儿童进行前瞻性纵向队列研究 结果在 7 岁和 13 岁时,所有 EF 领域均与同时数学计算成绩呈强正相关(β = 11.35,95% CI [9.28,13.41] 至 β = 13.79,95% CI [11.59,15.98])。7 岁时的所有 EF 领域均与 13 岁时的数学成绩呈正相关,其中认知灵活性(β = 10.79 [8.64, 12.94])和目标设定(β = 10.37 [8.08, 12.67])的相关性最强:本研究提供的证据表明,EF 与出生儿童的数学成绩密切相关。
{"title":"Executive function is associated with the development of math performance in children born at <30 weeks' gestation or <1250 g birthweight.","authors":"Simonne E Collins, Alice C Burnett, Philippa Pyman, Rheanna M Mainzer, Leona Pascoe, Kristina M Haebich, Jeanie L Y Cheong, Lex W Doyle, Deanne K Thompson, Peter J Anderson","doi":"10.1037/neu0000970","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/neu0000970","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Objective: </strong>To describe associations between executive function (EF) domains (attentional control, information processing, cognitive flexibility, and goal setting) and concurrent math computation performance at age 7 and 13 years in children born <30 weeks' gestation or weighing <1,250 g, and second, to examine the impact of 7-year EF on math performance at 13 years.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>In a prospective, longitudinal cohort of children born <30 weeks' gestation or with a birthweight <1,250 g, assessment of EF and math performance was undertaken at 7 (n = 187) and 13 years (n = 174). Linear regression models were used to describe associations between EF domains with math performance at both time points, as well as to examine the impact of EF at 7 years on math performance at 13 years.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>At 7 and 13 years, all EF domains were positively and strongly associated with concurrent math performance (β = 11.35, 95% CI [9.28, 13.41] to β = 13.79, 95% CI [11.59, 15.98]). All EF domains at age 7 years were positively associated with math performance at 13 years, with the strongest associations observed for cognitive flexibility (β = 10.79 [8.64, 12.94]) and goal setting (β = 10.37 [8.08, 12.67]).</p><p><strong>Conclusions: </strong>This study provides evidence that EF is strongly associated with math performance in children born <30 weeks' gestation or with a birthweight <1,250 g and highlights the importance of early cognitive flexibility and goal setting performance for future math performance. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":19205,"journal":{"name":"Neuropsychology","volume":"38 8","pages":"714-726"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142546473","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Is hyperactivity in children with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) a functional response to demands on specific executive functions or cognitive demands in general? 注意力缺陷/多动障碍(ADHD)儿童的多动症是对特定执行功能要求的功能性反应,还是对一般认知要求的功能性反应?
IF 2.6 3区 心理学 Q3 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-11-01 DOI: 10.1037/neu0000975
Elia F Soto, Katie Black, Michael J Kofler

Objective: Hyperactivity is a core and impairing deficit in the clinical model of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). However, the extent to which hyperactivity in ADHD is evoked by cognitively challenging tasks in general or by demands on specific executive functions remains unclear.

Method: A clinically evaluated and carefully phenotyped community-referred sample of 184 children ages 8-13 (M = 10.40, SD = 1.50; 61 girls) with ADHD (n = 119) and without ADHD (neurotypical children and children with psychiatric disorders other than ADHD) were administered multiple, counterbalanced executive (working memory, inhibitory control, set shifting) and nonexecutive tests. Objective measures of gross motor movement (hyperactivity) were obtained using actigraphy.

Results: Using bifactor s-1 modeling, results indicate that children with ADHD demonstrate moderately elevated levels of motor movement relative to non-ADHD children. Additionally, findings indicated that hyperactivity in ADHD reflects the outcome of at least two similarly important factors: (a) a baseline level of elevated motor movement that is independent of environmental demands on their executive and nonexecutive cognitive abilities (d = 0.72); and (b) additional elevations attributable to demands placed on specific executive functions, with working memory and inhibition demands evoking similarly large, differential increases in movement for children with ADHD above and beyond their elevated baselines (Δd = 0.80).

Conclusions: These findings suggests that executive function demands exacerbate, but do not fully explain, hyperactivity in ADHD, and/or there are at least two pathways to hyperactivity in ADHD-hyperactivity caused by environmental demands that challenge their underdeveloped executive functions, and hyperactivity caused by one or more other factors that need future research to identify. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

目的:多动是注意力缺陷/多动障碍(ADHD)临床模型中的一个核心和损害性缺陷。然而,ADHD 中的多动在多大程度上是由具有认知挑战性的任务引起的,还是由对特定执行功能的要求引起的,目前仍不清楚:方法:对 184 名 8-13 岁患有多动症(ADHD)的儿童(男 = 10.40,女 = 1.50;61 名女孩)(n = 119)和未患有多动症的儿童(神经正常儿童和患有非 ADHD 精神障碍的儿童)进行了临床评估和仔细的表型分析,并对他们进行了多重平衡执行测试(工作记忆、抑制控制、集合转移)和非执行测试。客观测量粗大运动(多动)的方法是使用行为记录仪:结果:使用双因子 s-1 建模,结果表明,相对于非多动症儿童,多动症儿童的运动水平适度升高。此外,研究结果表明,多动症儿童的多动反映了至少两个类似的重要因素:(a) 与环境对执行和非执行认知能力的要求无关的运动基线水平升高(d = 0.72);(b) 因对特定执行功能的要求而导致的额外升高,工作记忆和抑制要求导致多动症儿童的运动水平在基线升高的基础上出现类似的大幅差异升高(Δd = 0.80):这些研究结果表明,执行功能要求会加剧多动症儿童的多动状况,但并不能完全解释多动症儿童的多动状况,而且/或者多动症儿童的多动状况至少有两种途径--一种是由于环境要求挑战了多动症儿童发育不全的执行功能而导致的多动状况,另一种是由于一种或多种其他因素导致的多动状况,这需要未来的研究来确定。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Is hyperactivity in children with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) a functional response to demands on specific executive functions or cognitive demands in general?","authors":"Elia F Soto, Katie Black, Michael J Kofler","doi":"10.1037/neu0000975","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/neu0000975","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Objective: </strong>Hyperactivity is a core and impairing deficit in the clinical model of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). However, the extent to which hyperactivity in ADHD is evoked by cognitively challenging tasks in general or by demands on specific executive functions remains unclear.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>A clinically evaluated and carefully phenotyped community-referred sample of 184 children ages 8-13 (M = 10.40, SD = 1.50; 61 girls) with ADHD (n = 119) and without ADHD (neurotypical children and children with psychiatric disorders other than ADHD) were administered multiple, counterbalanced executive (working memory, inhibitory control, set shifting) and nonexecutive tests. Objective measures of gross motor movement (hyperactivity) were obtained using actigraphy.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>Using bifactor s-1 modeling, results indicate that children with ADHD demonstrate moderately elevated levels of motor movement relative to non-ADHD children. Additionally, findings indicated that hyperactivity in ADHD reflects the outcome of at least two similarly important factors: (a) a baseline level of elevated motor movement that is independent of environmental demands on their executive and nonexecutive cognitive abilities (d = 0.72); and (b) additional elevations attributable to demands placed on specific executive functions, with working memory and inhibition demands evoking similarly large, differential increases in movement for children with ADHD above and beyond their elevated baselines (Δd = 0.80).</p><p><strong>Conclusions: </strong>These findings suggests that executive function demands exacerbate, but do not fully explain, hyperactivity in ADHD, and/or there are at least two pathways to hyperactivity in ADHD-hyperactivity caused by environmental demands that challenge their underdeveloped executive functions, and hyperactivity caused by one or more other factors that need future research to identify. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":19205,"journal":{"name":"Neuropsychology","volume":"38 8","pages":"699-713"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142546474","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Getting oriented: Redefining attention deficits in Parkinson's disease. 明确方向:重新定义帕金森病的注意力缺陷。
IF 2.6 3区 心理学 Q3 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-11-01 Epub Date: 2024-09-26 DOI: 10.1037/neu0000973
Ori Peleg, Rébaï Soret, Pom Charras, Vsevolod Peysakhovich, Anat Mirelman, Inbal Maidan, Daniel A Levy

Objective: Parkinson's disease (PD) may affect not only motor functions, but also cognitive processes such as attention. While past research has examined PD impact on spatial attention, it has not addressed how the key functions of attentional orienting and alerting in PD are mediated by cueing format, an ecologically relevant parameter. We assessed how exogenous and endogenous orienting cue modes affect PD patients' visuospatial attention expressed as dorsal attention network orienting benefits, ventral attention network reorienting costs, and alerting abilities.

Method: Ninety PD patients and 72 healthy comparison participants performed a spatial attention task in an engaging game format which required selection of a target location without prior cueing, or with temporal, valid spatial, or invalid spatial exogenous or endogenous cueing.

Results: PD patients differed from healthy participants only in response time benefits in orienting under endogenous probabilistically predictive cue processing. They did not exhibit greater reorienting costs, differences in inhibition of return, or alerting deficits, irrespective of modes of cueing.

Conclusion: These results suggest that fundamental orienting and alerting functions might be intact in PD, with challenges emerging only if additional cognitive processes, including those related to motor preparation, are required to utilize cue information. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

目的:帕金森病(PD)不仅会影响运动功能,还会影响注意力等认知过程。虽然过去的研究已经探讨了帕金森病对空间注意力的影响,但还没有涉及帕金森病中注意力定向和警觉的关键功能是如何通过提示形式这一生态相关参数来介导的。我们评估了外源性和内源性定向提示模式如何影响帕金森病患者的视觉空间注意,具体表现为背侧注意网络定向益处、腹侧注意网络重定向代价和警觉能力:90名帕金森氏症患者和72名健康对比参与者以引人入胜的游戏形式完成了一项空间注意力任务,该任务要求在没有事先提示的情况下选择目标位置,或在有时间、有效空间或无效空间外源性或内源性提示的情况下选择目标位置:与健康参与者相比,帕金森病患者仅在内源性概率预测线索处理下的定向反应时间优势方面与健康参与者不同。无论采用哪种线索模式,他们都没有表现出更大的重新定向成本、回返抑制差异或警觉缺陷:这些结果表明,帕金森病患者的基本定向和警觉功能可能完好无损,只有在需要额外的认知过程(包括与运动准备有关的认知过程)来利用线索信息时,才会出现挑战。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved)。
{"title":"Getting oriented: Redefining attention deficits in Parkinson's disease.","authors":"Ori Peleg, Rébaï Soret, Pom Charras, Vsevolod Peysakhovich, Anat Mirelman, Inbal Maidan, Daniel A Levy","doi":"10.1037/neu0000973","DOIUrl":"10.1037/neu0000973","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Objective: </strong>Parkinson's disease (PD) may affect not only motor functions, but also cognitive processes such as attention. While past research has examined PD impact on spatial attention, it has not addressed how the key functions of attentional orienting and alerting in PD are mediated by cueing format, an ecologically relevant parameter. We assessed how exogenous and endogenous orienting cue modes affect PD patients' visuospatial attention expressed as dorsal attention network orienting benefits, ventral attention network reorienting costs, and alerting abilities.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>Ninety PD patients and 72 healthy comparison participants performed a spatial attention task in an engaging game format which required selection of a target location without prior cueing, or with temporal, valid spatial, or invalid spatial exogenous or endogenous cueing.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>PD patients differed from healthy participants only in response time benefits in orienting under endogenous probabilistically predictive cue processing. They did not exhibit greater reorienting costs, differences in inhibition of return, or alerting deficits, irrespective of modes of cueing.</p><p><strong>Conclusion: </strong>These results suggest that fundamental orienting and alerting functions might be intact in PD, with challenges emerging only if additional cognitive processes, including those related to motor preparation, are required to utilize cue information. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":19205,"journal":{"name":"Neuropsychology","volume":" ","pages":"749-762"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142350923","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Sociodemographic predictors and cross-cultural comparisons in tests performance from the Cambridge Neuropsychological Testing Automated Battery (CANTAB) among children aged 6-8 years from Montevideo, Uruguay. 乌拉圭蒙得维的亚 6-8 岁儿童剑桥神经心理测试自动测试库 (CANTAB) 测试成绩的社会人口学预测因素和跨文化比较。
IF 2.6 3区 心理学 Q3 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-11-01 DOI: 10.1037/neu0000966
Danelly Rodriguez, Elena I Queirolo, Katarzyna Kordas, Daniel Costa-Ball, Gabriel Barg

Objective: Cross-culturally comparative data on measures of executive function (EF) are essential, but the 6-8-year group remains insufficiently described. This study examined the sociodemographic predictors of EF test performance employing the Cambridge Neuropsychological Testing Automated Battery (CANTAB). It also compared developmental trends in EF among children from Uruguay, the United States, and Mexico.

Method: EFs were assessed with the Intra-dimensional/Extra-dimensional shift, Spatial Span (SSP), and Stockings of Cambridge (SOC) tests from the CANTAB. The study sample consisted of 6-8-year-old children from the Salud Ambiental Montevideo (SAM) cohort in Uruguay. Differences between cohorts were examined, and we performed generalized linear regressions to assess the association between sociodemographic factors, and each EF domain.

Results: The final sample consisted of 525 participants (mean age in months 82.5 ± 6.0). Across all ages, SAM children had significantly lower performance in the SSP and SOC tasks compared to U.S. and Mexican children. On the Intra-dimensional/Extra-dimensional shift task, SAM children had similar scores to U.S. and Mexican children. Mother's intelligence quotient (IQ; β = 0.01; 95% CI [0.005, 0.02]), child's IQ (0.02 [0.02, 0.03]), the HOME total score (0.02 [0.01, 0.03]), as well as HOME subscales of accompaniment (0.13 [0.07, 0.20]), enrichment (0.11 [0.06,0.16]), and physical environment (0.07 [0.03, 0.10]) were positively associated with the span length (SSP task). Child's IQ (0.02 [0.01,0.03]) was positively associated with the number of problems solved on the SOC test.

Conclusion: Uruguayan children perform lower in working memory and planning tests than U.S. children but similarly to Mexican children, while cognitive flexibility is consistent across all groups. Further, mother and child IQ, as well as the home environment, are important predictors of EF. These differences should be examined in the context of diverse cultural values and sociodemographic factors affecting CANTAB construct validity in this population. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

目的:执行功能(EF)测量的跨文化比较数据至关重要,但对 6-8 岁年龄组的描述仍然不足。本研究采用剑桥神经心理测试自动测试库(CANTAB)对预测执行功能测试成绩的社会人口因素进行了研究。研究还比较了乌拉圭、美国和墨西哥儿童的EF发展趋势:采用 CANTAB 中的内维度/外维度移位、空间跨度 (SSP) 和剑桥长袜 (SOC) 测试对幼儿的幼儿注意力进行评估。研究样本包括来自乌拉圭蒙得维的亚环境健康(SAM)队列的 6-8 岁儿童。我们研究了不同组群之间的差异,并进行了广义线性回归,以评估社会人口因素与每个EF领域之间的关联:最终样本包括 525 名参与者(平均年龄为 82.5 ± 6.0 个月)。与美国和墨西哥儿童相比,所有年龄段的萨姆儿童在SSP和SOC任务中的表现都明显较低。在内维/外维转换任务中,萨姆儿童的得分与美国和墨西哥儿童相似。母亲的智商 (IQ; β = 0.01; 95% CI [0.005, 0.02])、儿童的智商 (0.02 [0.02, 0.03])、HOME 总分 (0.02 [0.01, 0.03])、HOME 的陪伴分量表 (0.13 [0.07, 0.20])、丰富性(0.11 [0.06, 0.16])和物理环境(0.07 [0.03, 0.10])与跨度长度(SSP 任务)呈正相关。儿童的智商(0.02 [0.01, 0.03])与 SOC 测试中解决问题的数量呈正相关:结论:乌拉圭儿童在工作记忆和计划测试中的表现低于美国儿童,但与墨西哥儿童相似,而认知灵活性在所有群体中都是一致的。此外,母亲和儿童的智商以及家庭环境也是预测 EF 的重要因素。在研究这些差异时,应考虑到不同的文化价值观和社会人口因素会影响 CANTAB 在这一人群中的建构有效性。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Sociodemographic predictors and cross-cultural comparisons in tests performance from the Cambridge Neuropsychological Testing Automated Battery (CANTAB) among children aged 6-8 years from Montevideo, Uruguay.","authors":"Danelly Rodriguez, Elena I Queirolo, Katarzyna Kordas, Daniel Costa-Ball, Gabriel Barg","doi":"10.1037/neu0000966","DOIUrl":"10.1037/neu0000966","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Objective: </strong>Cross-culturally comparative data on measures of executive function (EF) are essential, but the 6-8-year group remains insufficiently described. This study examined the sociodemographic predictors of EF test performance employing the Cambridge Neuropsychological Testing Automated Battery (CANTAB). It also compared developmental trends in EF among children from Uruguay, the United States, and Mexico.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>EFs were assessed with the Intra-dimensional/Extra-dimensional shift, Spatial Span (SSP), and Stockings of Cambridge (SOC) tests from the CANTAB. The study sample consisted of 6-8-year-old children from the Salud Ambiental Montevideo (SAM) cohort in Uruguay. Differences between cohorts were examined, and we performed generalized linear regressions to assess the association between sociodemographic factors, and each EF domain.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>The final sample consisted of 525 participants (mean age in months 82.5 ± 6.0). Across all ages, SAM children had significantly lower performance in the SSP and SOC tasks compared to U.S. and Mexican children. On the Intra-dimensional/Extra-dimensional shift task, SAM children had similar scores to U.S. and Mexican children. Mother's intelligence quotient (IQ; β = 0.01; 95% CI [0.005, 0.02]), child's IQ (0.02 [0.02, 0.03]), the HOME total score (0.02 [0.01, 0.03]), as well as HOME subscales of accompaniment (0.13 [0.07, 0.20]), enrichment (0.11 [0.06,0.16]), and physical environment (0.07 [0.03, 0.10]) were positively associated with the span length (SSP task). Child's IQ (0.02 [0.01,0.03]) was positively associated with the number of problems solved on the SOC test.</p><p><strong>Conclusion: </strong>Uruguayan children perform lower in working memory and planning tests than U.S. children but similarly to Mexican children, while cognitive flexibility is consistent across all groups. Further, mother and child IQ, as well as the home environment, are important predictors of EF. These differences should be examined in the context of diverse cultural values and sociodemographic factors affecting CANTAB construct validity in this population. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":19205,"journal":{"name":"Neuropsychology","volume":"38 8","pages":"727-739"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142546541","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Corpus callosum structure and auditory interhemispheric transfer in spina bifida myelomeningocele. 脊柱裂髓母细胞瘤患者的胼胝体结构和听觉半球间转移。
IF 2.6 3区 心理学 Q3 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-11-01 DOI: 10.1037/neu0000915
Kailyn A Bradley, Jenifer J Juranek, H Julia Hannay, Paul T Cirino, Larry A Kramer, Jack M Fletcher

Objective: Maldevelopment of the posterior corpus callosum is common in spina bifida myelomeningocele (SBM) due to hydrocephalus-related hypoplasia and congenital partial hypogenesis. This study examined the relations of macro- and microstructural integrity of the interhemispheric temporal tract in SBM and auditory interhemispheric transfer using consonant-vowel dichotic listening.

Method: We collected T₁-weighted and diffusion tensor imaging data from 46 individuals with SBM and 15 typically developing individuals. Probabilistic tractography was used to isolate the interhemispheric white matter connecting auditory processing regions in both hemispheres. Interhemispheric transfer was assessed with a dichotic listening task.

Results: Although the typically developing group and the group with SBM showed the normative right-ear advantage, fewer participants showed a right-ear advantage in the group with SBM. The absence of the right-ear advantage was largely in the subgroup with hypogenesis of the splenium or severe posterior hypoplasia. Sex, anterior commissure cross-sectional area, and number of shunt pathways visible on magnetic resonance imaging predicted right-ear superiority.

Conclusions: Interhemispheric transfer is disrupted in individuals with SBM and hypogenesis or severe hypoplasia of the posterior corpus callosum. Preservation of interhemispheric transfer is related to expected connections through the posterior corpus callosum and possibly compensatory pathways in the anterior commissure. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

目的:由于脑积水相关发育不全和先天性部分发育不全,脊柱裂髓母细胞畸形(SBM)患者的后胼胝体发育不良很常见。本研究使用辅音-元音二分听法研究了脊柱裂颞叶间束的宏观和微观结构完整性与听觉半球间转移的关系:我们收集了 46 名 SBM 患者和 15 名发育正常患者的 T₁ 加权和弥散张量成像数据。使用概率束成像技术分离出连接两个半球听觉处理区域的半球间白质。通过二分听任务评估半球间的转移:结果:尽管典型发育组和SBM组都表现出正常的右耳优势,但SBM组中表现出右耳优势的人数较少。没有右耳优势的主要是脾发育不全或严重后发育不良的亚组。性别、前会厌横截面积和磁共振成像中可见的分流通道数量预示着右耳优势:结论:SBM 和后胼胝体发育不全或严重发育不全患者的大脑半球间转移会受到干扰。半球间转移的保留与通过胼胝体后部的预期连接有关,也可能与前突的代偿通路有关。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved)。
{"title":"Corpus callosum structure and auditory interhemispheric transfer in spina bifida myelomeningocele.","authors":"Kailyn A Bradley, Jenifer J Juranek, H Julia Hannay, Paul T Cirino, Larry A Kramer, Jack M Fletcher","doi":"10.1037/neu0000915","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/neu0000915","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Objective: </strong>Maldevelopment of the posterior corpus callosum is common in spina bifida myelomeningocele (SBM) due to hydrocephalus-related hypoplasia and congenital partial hypogenesis. This study examined the relations of macro- and microstructural integrity of the interhemispheric temporal tract in SBM and auditory interhemispheric transfer using consonant-vowel dichotic listening.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>We collected <i>T</i>₁-weighted and diffusion tensor imaging data from 46 individuals with SBM and 15 typically developing individuals. Probabilistic tractography was used to isolate the interhemispheric white matter connecting auditory processing regions in both hemispheres. Interhemispheric transfer was assessed with a dichotic listening task.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>Although the typically developing group and the group with SBM showed the normative right-ear advantage, fewer participants showed a right-ear advantage in the group with SBM. The absence of the right-ear advantage was largely in the subgroup with hypogenesis of the splenium or severe posterior hypoplasia. Sex, anterior commissure cross-sectional area, and number of shunt pathways visible on magnetic resonance imaging predicted right-ear superiority.</p><p><strong>Conclusions: </strong>Interhemispheric transfer is disrupted in individuals with SBM and hypogenesis or severe hypoplasia of the posterior corpus callosum. Preservation of interhemispheric transfer is related to expected connections through the posterior corpus callosum and possibly compensatory pathways in the anterior commissure. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":19205,"journal":{"name":"Neuropsychology","volume":"38 8","pages":"687-698"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142546472","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Neuropsychology
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1