首页 > 最新文献

Neuropsychology最新文献

英文 中文
Supplemental Material for Cardiovascular Health and Rate of Cognitive Decline in Preclinical Dementia: A 12-Year Population-Based Study 临床前痴呆症患者心血管健康与认知能力下降率的补充材料:一项为期 12 年的人群研究
IF 2.4 3区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-02-01 DOI: 10.1037/neu0000925.supp
{"title":"Supplemental Material for Cardiovascular Health and Rate of Cognitive Decline in Preclinical Dementia: A 12-Year Population-Based Study","authors":"","doi":"10.1037/neu0000925.supp","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/neu0000925.supp","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":19205,"journal":{"name":"Neuropsychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.4,"publicationDate":"2024-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139886940","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Dissociating the impact of alexithymia and impaired self-awareness on emotional distress and aggression after traumatic brain injury. 消除述情障碍和自我意识受损对创伤性脑损伤后情绪困扰和攻击性的影响。
IF 2.4 3区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-02-01 Epub Date: 2023-10-26 DOI: 10.1037/neu0000926
Suvi P Dockree, Cathal W Ffrench, Jodie A L O'Hara, Paul A Carroll, Paul M Dockree, Brian E McGuire

Objective: Alexithymia, a deficit in identifying and describing feelings, is prevalent in traumatic brain injury (TBI). Sometimes referred to as "emotional unawareness," we sought to investigate whether alexithymia after TBI was related to, or distinct from, impaired self-awareness (ISA) and whether the two predicted differentiable emotional and aggression profiles. Further, the mediating role of frontal system behaviors (disinhibition, dysexecutive function, apathy) was explored.

Method: Participants with TBI (N = 40) from diverse backgrounds completed self-report measures of alexithymia, emotional distress, aggression, and frontal system behaviors. For the assessment of ISA, significant other ratings were obtained to identify discrepancies from self-ratings. Data were analyzed quantitatively using independent samples t tests, correlations, partial correlations, and simple mediation.

Results: There was a negative correlation between alexithymia and ISA. Alexithymia, but not ISA, was associated with higher expressions of emotional distress and aggression even after controlling for the effects of ISA via partial correlations. Exploratory analyses found that frontal system behaviors mediated the relationships between alexithymia and aggression and alexithymia and emotional distress.

Conclusions: Alexithymia is more accurately conceptualized as an emotional processing deficit than an awareness deficit. Indeed, self-awareness may be a prerequisite for the ability to identify alexithymic tendencies. Negative psychological effects of alexithymia are compounded by poorer executive function and disinhibition and call for the development of TBI-specific alexithymia screening tools and interventions. Alexithymia interventions are best delivered in conjunction with rehabilitation of emotion regulation and executive function. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

目的:述情障碍是一种识别和描述情感的缺陷,在创伤性脑损伤中普遍存在。有时被称为“情绪无意识”,我们试图调查TBI后的述情障碍是否与自我意识受损(ISA)有关或不同,以及两者是否预测了可区分的情绪和攻击特征。此外,还探讨了额叶系统行为(去抑制、执行功能障碍、冷漠)的中介作用。方法:来自不同背景的TBI参与者(N=40)完成述情障碍、情绪困扰、攻击性和额系统行为的自我报告测量。对于ISA的评估,获得了重要的其他评级,以确定与自我评级的差异。使用独立样本t检验、相关性、偏相关性和简单中介对数据进行定量分析。结果:述情障碍与ISA呈负相关。Alexithymia,而不是ISA,与更高的情绪困扰和攻击性表达有关,即使在通过部分相关性控制了ISA的影响之后也是如此。探索性分析发现,额叶系统行为介导述情障碍与攻击、述情障碍和情绪困扰之间的关系。结论:与意识缺陷相比,述情障碍更准确地定义为情绪处理缺陷。事实上,自我意识可能是识别述情障碍倾向的能力的先决条件。述情障碍的负面心理影响因执行功能较差和去抑制而加剧,需要开发TBI特异性述情障碍筛查工具和干预措施。述情障碍干预最好与情绪调节和执行功能的康复相结合。(PsycInfo数据库记录(c)2023 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"Dissociating the impact of alexithymia and impaired self-awareness on emotional distress and aggression after traumatic brain injury.","authors":"Suvi P Dockree, Cathal W Ffrench, Jodie A L O'Hara, Paul A Carroll, Paul M Dockree, Brian E McGuire","doi":"10.1037/neu0000926","DOIUrl":"10.1037/neu0000926","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Objective: </strong>Alexithymia, a deficit in identifying and describing feelings, is prevalent in traumatic brain injury (TBI). Sometimes referred to as \"emotional unawareness,\" we sought to investigate whether alexithymia after TBI was related to, or distinct from, impaired self-awareness (ISA) and whether the two predicted differentiable emotional and aggression profiles. Further, the mediating role of frontal system behaviors (disinhibition, dysexecutive function, apathy) was explored.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>Participants with TBI (<i>N</i> = 40) from diverse backgrounds completed self-report measures of alexithymia, emotional distress, aggression, and frontal system behaviors. For the assessment of ISA, significant other ratings were obtained to identify discrepancies from self-ratings. Data were analyzed quantitatively using independent samples t tests, correlations, partial correlations, and simple mediation.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>There was a negative correlation between alexithymia and ISA. Alexithymia, but not ISA, was associated with higher expressions of emotional distress and aggression even after controlling for the effects of ISA via partial correlations. Exploratory analyses found that frontal system behaviors mediated the relationships between alexithymia and aggression and alexithymia and emotional distress.</p><p><strong>Conclusions: </strong>Alexithymia is more accurately conceptualized as an emotional processing deficit than an awareness deficit. Indeed, self-awareness may be a prerequisite for the ability to identify alexithymic tendencies. Negative psychological effects of alexithymia are compounded by poorer executive function and disinhibition and call for the development of TBI-specific alexithymia screening tools and interventions. Alexithymia interventions are best delivered in conjunction with rehabilitation of emotion regulation and executive function. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":19205,"journal":{"name":"Neuropsychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.4,"publicationDate":"2024-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"50162348","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
A diffusion decision model analysis of the cognitive effects of neurofeedback for ADHD. 神经反馈治疗ADHD认知效果的扩散决策模型分析。
IF 2.4 3区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-02-01 Epub Date: 2023-11-16 DOI: 10.1037/neu0000932
Nadja R Ging-Jehli, Quinn A Painter, Helena A Kraemer, Michelle E Roley-Roberts, Catherine Panchyshyn, Roger deBeus, L Eugene Arnold

Objective: To examine cognitive effects of neurofeedback (NF) for attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) as a secondary outcome of a randomized clinical trial.

Method: In a double-blind randomized clinical trial (NCT02251743), 133 7-10-year olds with ADHD received either 38 sessions of NF (n = 78) or control treatment (n = 55) and performed an integrated visual and auditory continuous performance test at baseline, mid- and end-treatment. We used the diffusion decision model to decompose integrated visual and auditory continuous performance test performance at each assessment into cognitive components: efficiency of integrating stimulus information (v), context sensitivity (cv), response cautiousness (a), response bias (z/a), and nondecision time for perceptual encoding and response execution (Ter). Based on prior findings, we tested whether the components known to be deficient improved with NF and explored whether other cognitive components improved using linear mixed modeling.

Results: Before NF, children with ADHD showed main deficits in integrating stimulus information (v), which led to less accurate and slower responses than healthy controls (p = .008). The NF group showed significantly more improvement in integrating auditory stimulus information (v) than control treatment (significant group-by-time-by-modality effect: p = .044).

Conclusions: NF seems to improve v, deficient in ADHD. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

目的:研究神经反馈(NF)作为一项随机临床试验的次要结果对注意缺陷多动障碍(ADHD)的认知影响。方法:在一项双盲随机临床试验(NCT02251743)中,133名7-10岁ADHD患者接受38次NF治疗(n = 78)或对照治疗(n = 55),并在基线、治疗中期和治疗结束时进行综合视觉和听觉连续表现测试。我们使用扩散决策模型将每个评估的综合视觉和听觉连续表现测试的表现分解为认知成分:整合刺激信息的效率(v)、情境敏感性(cv)、反应谨慎性(a)、反应偏差(z/a)和知觉编码和反应执行的非决策时间(Ter)。基于先前的研究结果,我们测试了已知的缺陷成分是否通过NF得到改善,并探索了其他认知成分是否通过线性混合模型得到改善。结果:在NF治疗前,ADHD儿童在整合刺激信息方面表现出主要缺陷(v),导致反应的准确性低于健康对照组,反应速度较慢(p = 0.008)。NF组在整合听觉刺激信息(v)方面明显优于对照组(p = 0.044)。结论:NF似乎可以改善ADHD缺陷。(PsycInfo数据库记录(c) 2023 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"A diffusion decision model analysis of the cognitive effects of neurofeedback for ADHD.","authors":"Nadja R Ging-Jehli, Quinn A Painter, Helena A Kraemer, Michelle E Roley-Roberts, Catherine Panchyshyn, Roger deBeus, L Eugene Arnold","doi":"10.1037/neu0000932","DOIUrl":"10.1037/neu0000932","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Objective: </strong>To examine cognitive effects of neurofeedback (NF) for attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) as a secondary outcome of a randomized clinical trial.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>In a double-blind randomized clinical trial (NCT02251743), 133 7-10-year olds with ADHD received either 38 sessions of NF (<i>n</i> = 78) or control treatment (<i>n</i> = 55) and performed an integrated visual and auditory continuous performance test at baseline, mid- and end-treatment. We used the diffusion decision model to decompose integrated visual and auditory continuous performance test performance at each assessment into cognitive components: efficiency of integrating stimulus information (<i>v</i>), context sensitivity (<i>c<sub>v</sub></i>), response cautiousness (<i>a</i>), response bias (<i>z/a</i>), and nondecision time for perceptual encoding and response execution (<i>T<sub>er</sub></i>). Based on prior findings, we tested whether the components known to be deficient improved with NF and explored whether other cognitive components improved using linear mixed modeling.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>Before NF, children with ADHD showed main deficits in integrating stimulus information (<i>v</i>), which led to less accurate and slower responses than healthy controls (<i>p</i> = .008). The NF group showed significantly more improvement in integrating auditory stimulus information (<i>v</i>) than control treatment (significant group-by-time-by-modality effect: <i>p</i> = .044).</p><p><strong>Conclusions: </strong>NF seems to improve <i>v</i>, deficient in ADHD. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":19205,"journal":{"name":"Neuropsychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.4,"publicationDate":"2024-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10842533/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"136398484","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Increased intraindividual variability in reaction time performance is associated with emerging cognitive decline in cognitively unimpaired adults. 在认知能力未受损的成年人中,反应时间表现的个体差异性增加与新出现的认知能力下降有关。
IF 2.4 3区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-02-01 Epub Date: 2023-11-16 DOI: 10.1037/neu0000928
Roos J Jutten, Rebecca E Amariglio, Paul Maruff, Michael J Properzi, Dorene M Rentz, Keith A Johnson, Reisa A Sperling, Kathryn V Papp

Objective: To investigate whether intraindividual variability (IIV) in reaction time (RT) over monthly administered cognitive tasks is increased in cognitively unimpaired older adults who are at risk for cognitive decline, and whether this is independent of mean RT performance.

Method: N = 109 cognitively unimpaired individuals (age 77.4 ± 5.0, 61.5% female, Mini-Mental State Examination 29.1 ± 1.3) from the Harvard Aging Brain Study completed the self-administered Computerized Cognitive Composite (C3) monthly at home for up to 1 year (12.7 ± 3.2 C3 assessments). Baseline C3 assessment coincided with routine in-clinic visits, including amyloid and tau positron emission tomography imaging and standardized cognitive testing, with cognitive testing repeated annually (1.6 ± 1.2 years follow-up). The C3 includes two simple RT tasks and two complex RT tasks. IIV estimates were derived by computing intraindividual standard deviations on residual RT scores after regressing out age and session order effects. Cross-sectional associations of IIV with cognition (global cognition, memory, executive functions [EF], processing speed) and amyloid and tau burden were examined using linear regression analyses correcting for demographics and mean RT. The association between IIV and cognitive decline was assessed using linear mixed models correcting for demographic factors, mean RT, and amyloid burden.

Results: After adjusting for mean RT, increased IIV on complex RT tasks was independently associated with worse EF performance (β = -0.10, 95% CI [-.16, -0.03], p = .004), greater inferior-temporal tau deposition (β = 0.18, 95% CI [0.02, 0.34], p = .024), and faster cognitive decline in those with elevated amyloid (β = -0.62, 95% CI [-1.18, -0.06], p = .033).

Conclusions: Increased variability in monthly RT may reflect subtle EF deficits and provide unique information about short-term cognitive decline in preclinical Alzheimer's disease. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

目的:研究在有认知能力下降风险的认知功能未受损的老年人中,反应时间(RT)在每月进行的认知任务中的个体变异性(IIV)是否增加,以及这是否独立于平均RT表现。方法:来自哈佛老化脑研究的109名认知功能未受损的个体(年龄77.4±5.0岁,61.5%为女性,Mini-Mental State Examination 29.1±1.3)每月在家中完成自我管理的计算机认知复合(C3)测试,为期1年(12.7±3.2次C3评估)。基线C3评估与常规门诊就诊一致,包括淀粉样蛋白和tau正电子发射断层成像和标准化认知测试,每年重复认知测试(随访1.6±1.2年)。C3包括两个简单的RT任务和两个复杂的RT任务。iv估计值是通过计算残差RT评分的个体内标准差,在剔除年龄和治疗顺序的影响后得出的。IIV与认知(整体认知、记忆、执行功能[EF]、处理速度)以及淀粉样蛋白和tau蛋白负担的横断面关联采用校正人口统计学因素和平均RT的线性回归分析进行检验。IIV与认知能力下降之间的关联采用校正人口统计学因素、平均RT和淀粉样蛋白负担的线性混合模型进行评估。结果:在调整平均RT后,复杂RT任务中IIV的增加与EF表现较差独立相关(β = -0.10, 95% CI[-])。16, -0.03], p = 0.004),颞下tau沉积更大(β = 0.18, 95% CI [0.02, 0.34], p = 0.024),淀粉样蛋白升高的患者认知能力下降更快(β = -0.62, 95% CI [-1.18, -0.06], p = 0.033)。结论:每月RT变动性的增加可能反映了微妙的EF缺陷,并提供了临床前阿尔茨海默病短期认知能力下降的独特信息。(PsycInfo数据库记录(c) 2023 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"Increased intraindividual variability in reaction time performance is associated with emerging cognitive decline in cognitively unimpaired adults.","authors":"Roos J Jutten, Rebecca E Amariglio, Paul Maruff, Michael J Properzi, Dorene M Rentz, Keith A Johnson, Reisa A Sperling, Kathryn V Papp","doi":"10.1037/neu0000928","DOIUrl":"10.1037/neu0000928","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Objective: </strong>To investigate whether intraindividual variability (IIV) in reaction time (RT) over monthly administered cognitive tasks is increased in cognitively unimpaired older adults who are at risk for cognitive decline, and whether this is independent of mean RT performance.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong><i>N</i> = 109 cognitively unimpaired individuals (age 77.4 ± 5.0, 61.5% female, Mini-Mental State Examination 29.1 ± 1.3) from the Harvard Aging Brain Study completed the self-administered Computerized Cognitive Composite (C3) monthly at home for up to 1 year (12.7 ± 3.2 C3 assessments). Baseline C3 assessment coincided with routine in-clinic visits, including amyloid and tau positron emission tomography imaging and standardized cognitive testing, with cognitive testing repeated annually (1.6 ± 1.2 years follow-up). The C3 includes two simple RT tasks and two complex RT tasks. IIV estimates were derived by computing intraindividual standard deviations on residual RT scores after regressing out age and session order effects. Cross-sectional associations of IIV with cognition (global cognition, memory, executive functions [EF], processing speed) and amyloid and tau burden were examined using linear regression analyses correcting for demographics and mean RT. The association between IIV and cognitive decline was assessed using linear mixed models correcting for demographic factors, mean RT, and amyloid burden.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>After adjusting for mean RT, increased IIV on complex RT tasks was independently associated with worse EF performance (β = -0.10, 95% CI [-.16, -0.03], <i>p</i> = .004), greater inferior-temporal tau deposition (β = 0.18, 95% CI [0.02, 0.34], <i>p</i> = .024), and faster cognitive decline in those with elevated amyloid (β = -0.62, 95% CI [-1.18, -0.06], <i>p</i> = .033).</p><p><strong>Conclusions: </strong>Increased variability in monthly RT may reflect subtle EF deficits and provide unique information about short-term cognitive decline in preclinical Alzheimer's disease. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":19205,"journal":{"name":"Neuropsychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.4,"publicationDate":"2024-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"136398488","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Supplemental Material for Processing Speed in First Episode of Psychosis and First-Degree Relatives: A Candidate Endophenotype of Spectrum Schizophrenia Disorders 精神病首次发作和一级亲属处理速度的补充材料:精神分裂症谱系障碍的一种候选内表型
IF 2.4 3区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-02-01 DOI: 10.1037/neu0000931.supp
{"title":"Supplemental Material for Processing Speed in First Episode of Psychosis and First-Degree Relatives: A Candidate Endophenotype of Spectrum Schizophrenia Disorders","authors":"","doi":"10.1037/neu0000931.supp","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/neu0000931.supp","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":19205,"journal":{"name":"Neuropsychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.4,"publicationDate":"2024-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139684292","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Social cognitive disruptions in multiple sclerosis: The role of executive (dys)function. 多发性硬化症的社会认知障碍:执行(障碍)功能的作用
IF 2.4 3区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-02-01 Epub Date: 2023-06-29 DOI: 10.1037/neu0000917
Charlotte R Pennington, Michelle C-S-Y Oxtoby, Daniel J Shaw

Objective: Multiple sclerosis (MS) is a chronic demyelinating disease of the central nervous system, resulting in a range of potential motor and cognitive impairments. The latter can affect both executive functions that orchestrate general goal-directed behavior and social cognitive processes that support our ability to interact with others and maintain healthy interpersonal relationships. Despite a long history of research into the cognitive symptoms of MS, it remains uncertain if social cognitive disruptions occur independently of, or reflect underlying disturbances to, more foundational executive functions. The present preregistered study investigated this directly.

Method: Employing an experimental design, we administered a battery of computerized tasks online to a large sample comprising 134 individuals with MS and 134 age- and sex-matched healthy controls (HCs). Three tasks measured elements of executive function (working memory, response inhibition, and switching) and two assessed components of social cognition disrupted most commonly in MS (emotion perception and theory of mind).

Results: Individuals with MS exhibited poorer working memory (d = .31), response inhibition (d = -.26), emotion perception (d = .32), and theory of mind (d = .35) compared with matched HCs. Furthermore, exploratory mediation analyses revealed that working memory performance accounted for approximately 20% of the group differences in both measures of social cognition.

Conclusions: Disruptions to working memory appear to serve as one of the mechanisms underpinning disturbances to social cognition in MS. Future research should examine if the benefits of cognitive rehabilitation programs that incorporate working memory training transfer to these social cognitive processes. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

目的:多发性硬化症(MS)是一种慢性中枢神经系统脱髓鞘疾病,会导致一系列潜在的运动和认知障碍。后者既会影响协调一般目标导向行为的执行功能,也会影响支持我们与他人互动和维持健康人际关系能力的社会认知过程。尽管对多发性硬化症认知症状的研究由来已久,但目前仍不确定社会认知障碍是独立于更基础的执行功能而发生的,还是反映了更基础的执行功能的潜在障碍。本研究对这一问题进行了直接调查:采用实验设计,我们对 134 名多发性硬化症患者和 134 名年龄和性别匹配的健康对照者(HCs)进行了一系列在线计算机化任务。三项任务测量了执行功能的要素(工作记忆、反应抑制和转换),两项任务评估了多发性硬化症患者最常受到干扰的社会认知要素(情绪感知和心智理论):结果:与匹配的普通人相比,多发性硬化症患者的工作记忆(d = .31)、反应抑制(d = -.26)、情绪感知(d = .32)和心智理论(d = .35)均较差。此外,探索性中介分析显示,工作记忆的表现约占社会认知两项测量中群体差异的 20%:结论:工作记忆紊乱似乎是多发性硬化症社会认知紊乱的机制之一。未来的研究应探讨将工作记忆训练纳入认知康复计划的益处是否会转移到这些社会认知过程中。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Social cognitive disruptions in multiple sclerosis: The role of executive (dys)function.","authors":"Charlotte R Pennington, Michelle C-S-Y Oxtoby, Daniel J Shaw","doi":"10.1037/neu0000917","DOIUrl":"10.1037/neu0000917","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Objective: </strong>Multiple sclerosis (MS) is a chronic demyelinating disease of the central nervous system, resulting in a range of potential motor and cognitive impairments. The latter can affect both executive functions that orchestrate general goal-directed behavior and social cognitive processes that support our ability to interact with others and maintain healthy interpersonal relationships. Despite a long history of research into the cognitive symptoms of MS, it remains uncertain if social cognitive disruptions occur independently of, or reflect underlying disturbances to, more foundational executive functions. The present preregistered study investigated this directly.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>Employing an experimental design, we administered a battery of computerized tasks online to a large sample comprising 134 individuals with MS and 134 age- and sex-matched healthy controls (HCs). Three tasks measured elements of executive function (working memory, response inhibition, and switching) and two assessed components of social cognition disrupted most commonly in MS (emotion perception and theory of mind).</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>Individuals with MS exhibited poorer working memory (<i>d</i> = .31), response inhibition (<i>d</i> = -.26), emotion perception (<i>d</i> = .32), and theory of mind (<i>d</i> = .35) compared with matched HCs. Furthermore, exploratory mediation analyses revealed that working memory performance accounted for approximately 20% of the group differences in both measures of social cognition.</p><p><strong>Conclusions: </strong>Disruptions to working memory appear to serve as one of the mechanisms underpinning disturbances to social cognition in MS. Future research should examine if the benefits of cognitive rehabilitation programs that incorporate working memory training transfer to these social cognitive processes. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":19205,"journal":{"name":"Neuropsychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.4,"publicationDate":"2024-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9690136","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Catastrophizing is associated with excess cognitive symptom reporting after mild traumatic brain injury. 轻度创伤性脑损伤后的过度认知症状报告与灾难有关。
IF 2.4 3区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-02-01 Epub Date: 2023-11-02 DOI: 10.1037/neu0000930
Shuyuan Shi, Edwina L Picon, Mathilde Rioux, William J Panenka, Noah D Silverberg

Objective: Persistent cognitive symptoms after mild traumatic brain injury (mTBI) often do not correlate with objective neuropsychological performance. Catastrophizing (i.e., excessively negative interpretations of symptoms) may help explain this discrepancy. We hypothesize that symptom catastrophizing will be associated with greater cognitive symptom reporting relative to neuropsychological test performance in people seeking treatment for mTBI.

Method: Secondary analysis of baseline data from a randomized controlled trial. Adults with mTBI (N = 77) were recruited from two outpatient mTBI clinics in British Columbia, Canada. Questionnaires and assessments were administered at baseline (M = 17.7 weeks postinjury). The sample was 64% women with a mean age of 42 years (SD = 11.5). Validated questionnaires were used to assess catastrophizing, cognitive symptoms, and affective distress. Neuropsychological performance was assessed using the National Institutes of Health Toolbox Cognition Battery. Discrepancies between cognitive symptoms and cognitive functioning were operationalized using standard residuals from neuropsychological test performance scores regressed on cognitive symptom scores. Generalized linear models were run to measure the association between symptom catastrophizing, cognitive variables, and their discrepancy, with affective distress as a covariate.

Results: Symptom catastrophizing was associated with more severe cognitive symptoms when controlling for neuropsychological performance (β = 0.44, 95% CI [0.23, 0.65]). Symptom catastrophizing was also associated with higher subjective-objective cognition residuals (β = 0.43, 95% CI [0.22, 0.64]). Catastrophizing remained a significant predictor after affective distress was introduced as a covariate.

Conclusions: Catastrophizing is associated with misperceptions of cognitive functioning following mTBI, specifically overreporting cognitive symptoms relative to neuropsychological performance. Symptom catastrophizing may be an important determinant of cognitive symptom reporting months after mTBI. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

目的:轻度颅脑损伤(mTBI)后持续的认知症状通常与客观的神经心理表现无关。灾难性的(即对症状的过度负面解释)可能有助于解释这种差异。我们假设,在寻求mTBI治疗的人中,相对于神经心理测试表现,症状灾难化将与更大的认知症状报告有关。方法:对随机对照试验的基线数据进行二次分析。从加拿大不列颠哥伦比亚省的两个门诊mTBI诊所招募患有mTBI的成年人(N=77)。在基线时进行问卷调查和评估(M=用药后17.7周)。样本中64%的女性平均年龄为42岁(SD=11.5)。经验证的问卷用于评估灾难、认知症状和情感痛苦。使用美国国立卫生研究院工具箱认知电池评估神经心理学表现。认知症状和认知功能之间的差异是使用神经心理测试成绩分数的标准残差来操作的,这些分数是根据认知症状分数回归的。运行广义线性模型来测量症状突变、认知变量及其差异之间的关联,情感痛苦是协变量。结果:在控制神经心理表现时,症状突变与更严重的认知症状相关(β=0.44,95%CI[0.23,0.65])。症状突变还与更高的主观-客观认知残差相关(β=0.043,95%CI[0.22,0.64])协变。结论:突变与mTBI后对认知功能的误解有关,特别是相对于神经心理表现过度报告认知症状。症状突变可能是mTBI后数月认知症状报告的重要决定因素。(PsycInfo数据库记录(c)2023 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"Catastrophizing is associated with excess cognitive symptom reporting after mild traumatic brain injury.","authors":"Shuyuan Shi, Edwina L Picon, Mathilde Rioux, William J Panenka, Noah D Silverberg","doi":"10.1037/neu0000930","DOIUrl":"10.1037/neu0000930","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Objective: </strong>Persistent cognitive symptoms after mild traumatic brain injury (mTBI) often do not correlate with objective neuropsychological performance. Catastrophizing (i.e., excessively negative interpretations of symptoms) may help explain this discrepancy. We hypothesize that symptom catastrophizing will be associated with greater cognitive symptom reporting relative to neuropsychological test performance in people seeking treatment for mTBI.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>Secondary analysis of baseline data from a randomized controlled trial. Adults with mTBI (<i>N</i> = 77) were recruited from two outpatient mTBI clinics in British Columbia, Canada. Questionnaires and assessments were administered at baseline (<i>M</i> = 17.7 weeks postinjury). The sample was 64% women with a mean age of 42 years (<i>SD</i> = 11.5). Validated questionnaires were used to assess catastrophizing, cognitive symptoms, and affective distress. Neuropsychological performance was assessed using the National Institutes of Health Toolbox Cognition Battery. Discrepancies between cognitive symptoms and cognitive functioning were operationalized using standard residuals from neuropsychological test performance scores regressed on cognitive symptom scores. Generalized linear models were run to measure the association between symptom catastrophizing, cognitive variables, and their discrepancy, with affective distress as a covariate.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>Symptom catastrophizing was associated with more severe cognitive symptoms when controlling for neuropsychological performance (β = 0.44, 95% CI [0.23, 0.65]). Symptom catastrophizing was also associated with higher subjective-objective cognition residuals (β = 0.43, 95% CI [0.22, 0.64]). Catastrophizing remained a significant predictor after affective distress was introduced as a covariate.</p><p><strong>Conclusions: </strong>Catastrophizing is associated with misperceptions of cognitive functioning following mTBI, specifically overreporting cognitive symptoms relative to neuropsychological performance. Symptom catastrophizing may be an important determinant of cognitive symptom reporting months after mTBI. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":19205,"journal":{"name":"Neuropsychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.4,"publicationDate":"2024-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"71425475","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Capturing learning curves with the multiday Boston Remote Assessment of Neurocognitive Health (BRANCH): Feasibility, reliability, and validity. 用多日波士顿神经认知健康远程评估(BRANCH)捕捉学习曲线:可行性、可靠性和效度。
IF 2.4 3区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-02-01 Epub Date: 2023-11-16 DOI: 10.1037/neu0000933
Emma L Weizenbaum, Daniel Soberanes, Stephanie Hsieh, Cassidy P Molinare, Rachel F Buckley, Rebecca A Betensky, Michael J Properzi, Gad A Marshall, Dorene M Rentz, Keith A Johnson, Reisa A Sperling, Rebecca E Amariglio, Kathryn V Papp

Objective: Unsupervised remote digital cognitive assessment makes frequent testing feasible and allows for measurement of learning over repeated evaluations on participants' own devices. This provides the opportunity to derive individual multiday learning curve scores over short intervals. Here, we report feasibility, reliability, and validity, of a 7-day cognitive battery from the Boston Remote Assessment for Neurocognitive Health (Multiday BRANCH), an unsupervised web-based assessment.

Method: Multiday BRANCH was administered remotely to 181 cognitively unimpaired older adults using their own electronic devices. For 7 consecutive days, participants completed three tests with associative memory components (Face-Name, Groceries-Prices, Digit Signs), using the same stimuli, to capture multiday learning curves for each test. We assessed the feasibility of capturing learning curves across the 7 days. Additionally, we examined the reliability and associations of learning curves with demographics, and traditional cognitive and subjective report measures.

Results: Multiday BRANCH was feasible with 96% of participants completing all study assessments; there were no differences dependent on type of device used (t = 0.71, p = .48) or time of day completed (t = -0.08, p = .94). Psychometric properties of the learning curves were sound including good test-retest reliability of individuals' curves (intraclass correlation = 0.94). Learning curves were positively correlated with in-person cognitive tests and subjective report of cognitive complaints.

Conclusions: Multiday BRANCH is a feasible, reliable, and valid cognitive measure that may be useful for identifying subtle changes in learning and memory processes in older adults. In the future, we will determine whether Multiday BRANCH is predictive of the presence of preclinical Alzheimer's disease. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

目的:无监督远程数字认知评估使频繁测试成为可能,并允许在参与者自己的设备上重复评估学习的测量。这提供了在短时间间隔内获得个人多日学习曲线分数的机会。在这里,我们报告了来自波士顿神经认知健康远程评估(Multiday BRANCH)的7天认知电池的可行性、可靠性和有效性,这是一种无监督的基于网络的评估。方法:使用自己的电子设备对181名认知功能正常的老年人进行多天远程BRANCH治疗。在连续7天的时间里,参与者使用相同的刺激物完成了包含联想记忆成分(面孔-名字、杂货店-价格、数字符号)的三个测试,以捕捉每个测试的多日学习曲线。我们评估了在7天内捕捉学习曲线的可行性。此外,我们还检验了学习曲线与人口统计学、传统认知和主观报告测量的可靠性和相关性。结果:多日BRANCH是可行的,96%的参与者完成了所有的研究评估;使用的器械类型(t = 0.71, p = 0.48)或完成一天的时间(t = -0.08, p = 0.94)没有差异。学习曲线的心理测量特性良好,个体曲线的重测信度良好(类内相关= 0.94)。学习曲线与面对面的认知测试和主观的认知抱怨报告呈正相关。结论:Multiday BRANCH是一种可行、可靠和有效的认知测量方法,可用于识别老年人学习和记忆过程的细微变化。在未来,我们将确定Multiday BRANCH是否可以预测临床前阿尔茨海默病的存在。(PsycInfo数据库记录(c) 2023 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"Capturing learning curves with the multiday Boston Remote Assessment of Neurocognitive Health (BRANCH): Feasibility, reliability, and validity.","authors":"Emma L Weizenbaum, Daniel Soberanes, Stephanie Hsieh, Cassidy P Molinare, Rachel F Buckley, Rebecca A Betensky, Michael J Properzi, Gad A Marshall, Dorene M Rentz, Keith A Johnson, Reisa A Sperling, Rebecca E Amariglio, Kathryn V Papp","doi":"10.1037/neu0000933","DOIUrl":"10.1037/neu0000933","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Objective: </strong>Unsupervised remote digital cognitive assessment makes frequent testing feasible and allows for measurement of learning over repeated evaluations on participants' own devices. This provides the opportunity to derive individual multiday learning curve scores over short intervals. Here, we report feasibility, reliability, and validity, of a 7-day cognitive battery from the Boston Remote Assessment for Neurocognitive Health (Multiday BRANCH), an unsupervised web-based assessment.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>Multiday BRANCH was administered remotely to 181 cognitively unimpaired older adults using their own electronic devices. For 7 consecutive days, participants completed three tests with associative memory components (Face-Name, Groceries-Prices, Digit Signs), using the same stimuli, to capture multiday learning curves for each test. We assessed the feasibility of capturing learning curves across the 7 days. Additionally, we examined the reliability and associations of learning curves with demographics, and traditional cognitive and subjective report measures.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>Multiday BRANCH was feasible with 96% of participants completing all study assessments; there were no differences dependent on type of device used (<i>t</i> = 0.71, <i>p</i> = .48) or time of day completed (<i>t</i> = -0.08, <i>p</i> = .94). Psychometric properties of the learning curves were sound including good test-retest reliability of individuals' curves (intraclass correlation = 0.94). Learning curves were positively correlated with in-person cognitive tests and subjective report of cognitive complaints.</p><p><strong>Conclusions: </strong>Multiday BRANCH is a feasible, reliable, and valid cognitive measure that may be useful for identifying subtle changes in learning and memory processes in older adults. In the future, we will determine whether Multiday BRANCH is predictive of the presence of preclinical Alzheimer's disease. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":19205,"journal":{"name":"Neuropsychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.4,"publicationDate":"2024-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10841660/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"136398485","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Executive functions beyond the "Holy Trinity": A scoping review. 超越 "神圣三位一体 "的执行功能:范围审查。
IF 2.4 3区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-02-01 Epub Date: 2023-07-24 DOI: 10.1037/neu0000922
Natália Martins Dias, Isabela Espezin Helsdingen, Eduarda Kammers Rita Momm de Lins, Camila Erlinda Etcheverria, Vanessa de Araújo Dechen, Luana Steffen, Caroline de Oliveira Cardoso, Fernanda Machado Lopes

Objective: Executive functions (EFs) are a multifaceted construct, important for several outcomes throughout life. The most commonly addressed executive components are inhibitory control, working memory, and cognitive flexibility. To map how other executive components are addressed conceptually and methodologically in the literature, a scoping review was carried out.

Method: The Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic reviews and Meta-Analyses extension for Scoping Reviews guideline was used. The database search (2017-2022) was carried out on the APA PsycInfo, Medline, Scopus, LILACS, and SciELO databases. A total of 3,473 articles were identified; 1,302 remained after screening. Given the large number of articles, a sample calculation was performed (95% CI and 5% error), with 297 articles randomly selected for full-text reading and 242 studies included for analysis.

Results: There was a predominance of health application studies in adults and older adults and in developed countries. There is relative conceptual consensus that EF are a multidimensional construct; however, there is no explicit identification of the theoretical model used in the studies. A diversity of EF was evidenced, which broadens the understanding of the construct, its assessment, and intervention targets. Furthermore, there was a variety of instruments used to assess these skills, with a predominance of performance tests, often with construct-instrument ambivalence between studies.

Conclusions: The mapping showed a diversity of skills referred to as EF in addition to the core components and identified the instruments used in their assessment. Greater conceptual and structural clarity and greater methodological care are fundamental to support the assessment of EF and their intervention. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

目的:执行功能(EFs)是一个多层面的概念,对人的一生有重要影响。最常涉及的执行成分是抑制控制、工作记忆和认知灵活性。为了从概念和方法上了解文献中是如何论述其他执行成分的,我们进行了一次范围界定综述:采用了《系统综述和元分析首选报告项目扩展范围综述指南》。在 APA PsycInfo、Medline、Scopus、LILACS 和 SciELO 数据库中进行了数据库检索(2017-2022 年)。共检索到 3473 篇文章,经筛选后剩余 1302 篇。鉴于文章数量众多,我们进行了样本计算(95% CI 和 5%误差),随机抽取了 297 篇文章进行全文阅读,并纳入了 242 项研究进行分析:以成人和老年人以及发达国家的健康应用研究为主。在概念上,EF 是一个多维度的建构,这一点已达成相对一致的共识;然而,研究中使用的理论模型却没有明确的标识。研究证明了情绪情感因素的多样性,这拓宽了对该因素、其评估和干预目标的理解。此外,用于评估这些技能的工具也多种多样,其中以表现测试为主,不同研究之间往往存在建构与工具之间的矛盾:研究结果表明,除核心成分外,被称为 EF 的技能多种多样,并确定了用于评估这些技能的工具。更清晰的概念和结构以及更谨慎的方法是支持评价 EF 及其干预的基础。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Executive functions beyond the \"Holy Trinity\": A scoping review.","authors":"Natália Martins Dias, Isabela Espezin Helsdingen, Eduarda Kammers Rita Momm de Lins, Camila Erlinda Etcheverria, Vanessa de Araújo Dechen, Luana Steffen, Caroline de Oliveira Cardoso, Fernanda Machado Lopes","doi":"10.1037/neu0000922","DOIUrl":"10.1037/neu0000922","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Objective: </strong>Executive functions (EFs) are a multifaceted construct, important for several outcomes throughout life. The most commonly addressed executive components are inhibitory control, working memory, and cognitive flexibility. To map how other executive components are addressed conceptually and methodologically in the literature, a scoping review was carried out.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>The Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic reviews and Meta-Analyses extension for Scoping Reviews guideline was used. The database search (2017-2022) was carried out on the APA PsycInfo, Medline, Scopus, LILACS, and SciELO databases. A total of 3,473 articles were identified; 1,302 remained after screening. Given the large number of articles, a sample calculation was performed (95% CI and 5% error), with 297 articles randomly selected for full-text reading and 242 studies included for analysis.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>There was a predominance of health application studies in adults and older adults and in developed countries. There is relative conceptual consensus that EF are a multidimensional construct; however, there is no explicit identification of the theoretical model used in the studies. A diversity of EF was evidenced, which broadens the understanding of the construct, its assessment, and intervention targets. Furthermore, there was a variety of instruments used to assess these skills, with a predominance of performance tests, often with construct-instrument ambivalence between studies.</p><p><strong>Conclusions: </strong>The mapping showed a diversity of skills referred to as EF in addition to the core components and identified the instruments used in their assessment. Greater conceptual and structural clarity and greater methodological care are fundamental to support the assessment of EF and their intervention. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":19205,"journal":{"name":"Neuropsychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.4,"publicationDate":"2024-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9859275","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Supplemental Material for Antecedents of Mind Wandering States in Healthy Aging and Mild Cognitive Impairment 健康老龄化和轻度认知障碍中思维游离状态的前因后果》补充材料
IF 2.4 3区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-01-29 DOI: 10.1037/neu0000941.supp
{"title":"Supplemental Material for Antecedents of Mind Wandering States in Healthy Aging and Mild Cognitive Impairment","authors":"","doi":"10.1037/neu0000941.supp","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/neu0000941.supp","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":19205,"journal":{"name":"Neuropsychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.4,"publicationDate":"2024-01-29","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140489098","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Neuropsychology
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1