首页 > 最新文献

Neuropsychology最新文献

英文 中文
Getting oriented: Redefining attention deficits in Parkinson's disease. 明确方向:重新定义帕金森病的注意力缺陷。
IF 2.6 3区 心理学 Q3 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-11-01 Epub Date: 2024-09-26 DOI: 10.1037/neu0000973
Ori Peleg, Rébaï Soret, Pom Charras, Vsevolod Peysakhovich, Anat Mirelman, Inbal Maidan, Daniel A Levy

Objective: Parkinson's disease (PD) may affect not only motor functions, but also cognitive processes such as attention. While past research has examined PD impact on spatial attention, it has not addressed how the key functions of attentional orienting and alerting in PD are mediated by cueing format, an ecologically relevant parameter. We assessed how exogenous and endogenous orienting cue modes affect PD patients' visuospatial attention expressed as dorsal attention network orienting benefits, ventral attention network reorienting costs, and alerting abilities.

Method: Ninety PD patients and 72 healthy comparison participants performed a spatial attention task in an engaging game format which required selection of a target location without prior cueing, or with temporal, valid spatial, or invalid spatial exogenous or endogenous cueing.

Results: PD patients differed from healthy participants only in response time benefits in orienting under endogenous probabilistically predictive cue processing. They did not exhibit greater reorienting costs, differences in inhibition of return, or alerting deficits, irrespective of modes of cueing.

Conclusion: These results suggest that fundamental orienting and alerting functions might be intact in PD, with challenges emerging only if additional cognitive processes, including those related to motor preparation, are required to utilize cue information. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

目的:帕金森病(PD)不仅会影响运动功能,还会影响注意力等认知过程。虽然过去的研究已经探讨了帕金森病对空间注意力的影响,但还没有涉及帕金森病中注意力定向和警觉的关键功能是如何通过提示形式这一生态相关参数来介导的。我们评估了外源性和内源性定向提示模式如何影响帕金森病患者的视觉空间注意,具体表现为背侧注意网络定向益处、腹侧注意网络重定向代价和警觉能力:90名帕金森氏症患者和72名健康对比参与者以引人入胜的游戏形式完成了一项空间注意力任务,该任务要求在没有事先提示的情况下选择目标位置,或在有时间、有效空间或无效空间外源性或内源性提示的情况下选择目标位置:与健康参与者相比,帕金森病患者仅在内源性概率预测线索处理下的定向反应时间优势方面与健康参与者不同。无论采用哪种线索模式,他们都没有表现出更大的重新定向成本、回返抑制差异或警觉缺陷:这些结果表明,帕金森病患者的基本定向和警觉功能可能完好无损,只有在需要额外的认知过程(包括与运动准备有关的认知过程)来利用线索信息时,才会出现挑战。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved)。
{"title":"Getting oriented: Redefining attention deficits in Parkinson's disease.","authors":"Ori Peleg, Rébaï Soret, Pom Charras, Vsevolod Peysakhovich, Anat Mirelman, Inbal Maidan, Daniel A Levy","doi":"10.1037/neu0000973","DOIUrl":"10.1037/neu0000973","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Objective: </strong>Parkinson's disease (PD) may affect not only motor functions, but also cognitive processes such as attention. While past research has examined PD impact on spatial attention, it has not addressed how the key functions of attentional orienting and alerting in PD are mediated by cueing format, an ecologically relevant parameter. We assessed how exogenous and endogenous orienting cue modes affect PD patients' visuospatial attention expressed as dorsal attention network orienting benefits, ventral attention network reorienting costs, and alerting abilities.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>Ninety PD patients and 72 healthy comparison participants performed a spatial attention task in an engaging game format which required selection of a target location without prior cueing, or with temporal, valid spatial, or invalid spatial exogenous or endogenous cueing.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>PD patients differed from healthy participants only in response time benefits in orienting under endogenous probabilistically predictive cue processing. They did not exhibit greater reorienting costs, differences in inhibition of return, or alerting deficits, irrespective of modes of cueing.</p><p><strong>Conclusion: </strong>These results suggest that fundamental orienting and alerting functions might be intact in PD, with challenges emerging only if additional cognitive processes, including those related to motor preparation, are required to utilize cue information. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":19205,"journal":{"name":"Neuropsychology","volume":" ","pages":"749-762"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142350923","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Sociodemographic predictors and cross-cultural comparisons in tests performance from the Cambridge Neuropsychological Testing Automated Battery (CANTAB) among children aged 6-8 years from Montevideo, Uruguay. 乌拉圭蒙得维的亚 6-8 岁儿童剑桥神经心理测试自动测试库 (CANTAB) 测试成绩的社会人口学预测因素和跨文化比较。
IF 2.6 3区 心理学 Q3 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-11-01 DOI: 10.1037/neu0000966
Danelly Rodriguez, Elena I Queirolo, Katarzyna Kordas, Daniel Costa-Ball, Gabriel Barg

Objective: Cross-culturally comparative data on measures of executive function (EF) are essential, but the 6-8-year group remains insufficiently described. This study examined the sociodemographic predictors of EF test performance employing the Cambridge Neuropsychological Testing Automated Battery (CANTAB). It also compared developmental trends in EF among children from Uruguay, the United States, and Mexico.

Method: EFs were assessed with the Intra-dimensional/Extra-dimensional shift, Spatial Span (SSP), and Stockings of Cambridge (SOC) tests from the CANTAB. The study sample consisted of 6-8-year-old children from the Salud Ambiental Montevideo (SAM) cohort in Uruguay. Differences between cohorts were examined, and we performed generalized linear regressions to assess the association between sociodemographic factors, and each EF domain.

Results: The final sample consisted of 525 participants (mean age in months 82.5 ± 6.0). Across all ages, SAM children had significantly lower performance in the SSP and SOC tasks compared to U.S. and Mexican children. On the Intra-dimensional/Extra-dimensional shift task, SAM children had similar scores to U.S. and Mexican children. Mother's intelligence quotient (IQ; β = 0.01; 95% CI [0.005, 0.02]), child's IQ (0.02 [0.02, 0.03]), the HOME total score (0.02 [0.01, 0.03]), as well as HOME subscales of accompaniment (0.13 [0.07, 0.20]), enrichment (0.11 [0.06,0.16]), and physical environment (0.07 [0.03, 0.10]) were positively associated with the span length (SSP task). Child's IQ (0.02 [0.01,0.03]) was positively associated with the number of problems solved on the SOC test.

Conclusion: Uruguayan children perform lower in working memory and planning tests than U.S. children but similarly to Mexican children, while cognitive flexibility is consistent across all groups. Further, mother and child IQ, as well as the home environment, are important predictors of EF. These differences should be examined in the context of diverse cultural values and sociodemographic factors affecting CANTAB construct validity in this population. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

目的:执行功能(EF)测量的跨文化比较数据至关重要,但对 6-8 岁年龄组的描述仍然不足。本研究采用剑桥神经心理测试自动测试库(CANTAB)对预测执行功能测试成绩的社会人口因素进行了研究。研究还比较了乌拉圭、美国和墨西哥儿童的EF发展趋势:采用 CANTAB 中的内维度/外维度移位、空间跨度 (SSP) 和剑桥长袜 (SOC) 测试对幼儿的幼儿注意力进行评估。研究样本包括来自乌拉圭蒙得维的亚环境健康(SAM)队列的 6-8 岁儿童。我们研究了不同组群之间的差异,并进行了广义线性回归,以评估社会人口因素与每个EF领域之间的关联:最终样本包括 525 名参与者(平均年龄为 82.5 ± 6.0 个月)。与美国和墨西哥儿童相比,所有年龄段的萨姆儿童在SSP和SOC任务中的表现都明显较低。在内维/外维转换任务中,萨姆儿童的得分与美国和墨西哥儿童相似。母亲的智商 (IQ; β = 0.01; 95% CI [0.005, 0.02])、儿童的智商 (0.02 [0.02, 0.03])、HOME 总分 (0.02 [0.01, 0.03])、HOME 的陪伴分量表 (0.13 [0.07, 0.20])、丰富性(0.11 [0.06, 0.16])和物理环境(0.07 [0.03, 0.10])与跨度长度(SSP 任务)呈正相关。儿童的智商(0.02 [0.01, 0.03])与 SOC 测试中解决问题的数量呈正相关:结论:乌拉圭儿童在工作记忆和计划测试中的表现低于美国儿童,但与墨西哥儿童相似,而认知灵活性在所有群体中都是一致的。此外,母亲和儿童的智商以及家庭环境也是预测 EF 的重要因素。在研究这些差异时,应考虑到不同的文化价值观和社会人口因素会影响 CANTAB 在这一人群中的建构有效性。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Sociodemographic predictors and cross-cultural comparisons in tests performance from the Cambridge Neuropsychological Testing Automated Battery (CANTAB) among children aged 6-8 years from Montevideo, Uruguay.","authors":"Danelly Rodriguez, Elena I Queirolo, Katarzyna Kordas, Daniel Costa-Ball, Gabriel Barg","doi":"10.1037/neu0000966","DOIUrl":"10.1037/neu0000966","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Objective: </strong>Cross-culturally comparative data on measures of executive function (EF) are essential, but the 6-8-year group remains insufficiently described. This study examined the sociodemographic predictors of EF test performance employing the Cambridge Neuropsychological Testing Automated Battery (CANTAB). It also compared developmental trends in EF among children from Uruguay, the United States, and Mexico.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>EFs were assessed with the Intra-dimensional/Extra-dimensional shift, Spatial Span (SSP), and Stockings of Cambridge (SOC) tests from the CANTAB. The study sample consisted of 6-8-year-old children from the Salud Ambiental Montevideo (SAM) cohort in Uruguay. Differences between cohorts were examined, and we performed generalized linear regressions to assess the association between sociodemographic factors, and each EF domain.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>The final sample consisted of 525 participants (mean age in months 82.5 ± 6.0). Across all ages, SAM children had significantly lower performance in the SSP and SOC tasks compared to U.S. and Mexican children. On the Intra-dimensional/Extra-dimensional shift task, SAM children had similar scores to U.S. and Mexican children. Mother's intelligence quotient (IQ; β = 0.01; 95% CI [0.005, 0.02]), child's IQ (0.02 [0.02, 0.03]), the HOME total score (0.02 [0.01, 0.03]), as well as HOME subscales of accompaniment (0.13 [0.07, 0.20]), enrichment (0.11 [0.06,0.16]), and physical environment (0.07 [0.03, 0.10]) were positively associated with the span length (SSP task). Child's IQ (0.02 [0.01,0.03]) was positively associated with the number of problems solved on the SOC test.</p><p><strong>Conclusion: </strong>Uruguayan children perform lower in working memory and planning tests than U.S. children but similarly to Mexican children, while cognitive flexibility is consistent across all groups. Further, mother and child IQ, as well as the home environment, are important predictors of EF. These differences should be examined in the context of diverse cultural values and sociodemographic factors affecting CANTAB construct validity in this population. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":19205,"journal":{"name":"Neuropsychology","volume":"38 8","pages":"727-739"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142546541","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Corpus callosum structure and auditory interhemispheric transfer in spina bifida myelomeningocele. 脊柱裂髓母细胞瘤患者的胼胝体结构和听觉半球间转移。
IF 2.6 3区 心理学 Q3 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-11-01 DOI: 10.1037/neu0000915
Kailyn A Bradley, Jenifer J Juranek, H Julia Hannay, Paul T Cirino, Larry A Kramer, Jack M Fletcher

Objective: Maldevelopment of the posterior corpus callosum is common in spina bifida myelomeningocele (SBM) due to hydrocephalus-related hypoplasia and congenital partial hypogenesis. This study examined the relations of macro- and microstructural integrity of the interhemispheric temporal tract in SBM and auditory interhemispheric transfer using consonant-vowel dichotic listening.

Method: We collected T₁-weighted and diffusion tensor imaging data from 46 individuals with SBM and 15 typically developing individuals. Probabilistic tractography was used to isolate the interhemispheric white matter connecting auditory processing regions in both hemispheres. Interhemispheric transfer was assessed with a dichotic listening task.

Results: Although the typically developing group and the group with SBM showed the normative right-ear advantage, fewer participants showed a right-ear advantage in the group with SBM. The absence of the right-ear advantage was largely in the subgroup with hypogenesis of the splenium or severe posterior hypoplasia. Sex, anterior commissure cross-sectional area, and number of shunt pathways visible on magnetic resonance imaging predicted right-ear superiority.

Conclusions: Interhemispheric transfer is disrupted in individuals with SBM and hypogenesis or severe hypoplasia of the posterior corpus callosum. Preservation of interhemispheric transfer is related to expected connections through the posterior corpus callosum and possibly compensatory pathways in the anterior commissure. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

目的:由于脑积水相关发育不全和先天性部分发育不全,脊柱裂髓母细胞畸形(SBM)患者的后胼胝体发育不良很常见。本研究使用辅音-元音二分听法研究了脊柱裂颞叶间束的宏观和微观结构完整性与听觉半球间转移的关系:我们收集了 46 名 SBM 患者和 15 名发育正常患者的 T₁ 加权和弥散张量成像数据。使用概率束成像技术分离出连接两个半球听觉处理区域的半球间白质。通过二分听任务评估半球间的转移:结果:尽管典型发育组和SBM组都表现出正常的右耳优势,但SBM组中表现出右耳优势的人数较少。没有右耳优势的主要是脾发育不全或严重后发育不良的亚组。性别、前会厌横截面积和磁共振成像中可见的分流通道数量预示着右耳优势:结论:SBM 和后胼胝体发育不全或严重发育不全患者的大脑半球间转移会受到干扰。半球间转移的保留与通过胼胝体后部的预期连接有关,也可能与前突的代偿通路有关。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved)。
{"title":"Corpus callosum structure and auditory interhemispheric transfer in spina bifida myelomeningocele.","authors":"Kailyn A Bradley, Jenifer J Juranek, H Julia Hannay, Paul T Cirino, Larry A Kramer, Jack M Fletcher","doi":"10.1037/neu0000915","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/neu0000915","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Objective: </strong>Maldevelopment of the posterior corpus callosum is common in spina bifida myelomeningocele (SBM) due to hydrocephalus-related hypoplasia and congenital partial hypogenesis. This study examined the relations of macro- and microstructural integrity of the interhemispheric temporal tract in SBM and auditory interhemispheric transfer using consonant-vowel dichotic listening.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>We collected <i>T</i>₁-weighted and diffusion tensor imaging data from 46 individuals with SBM and 15 typically developing individuals. Probabilistic tractography was used to isolate the interhemispheric white matter connecting auditory processing regions in both hemispheres. Interhemispheric transfer was assessed with a dichotic listening task.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>Although the typically developing group and the group with SBM showed the normative right-ear advantage, fewer participants showed a right-ear advantage in the group with SBM. The absence of the right-ear advantage was largely in the subgroup with hypogenesis of the splenium or severe posterior hypoplasia. Sex, anterior commissure cross-sectional area, and number of shunt pathways visible on magnetic resonance imaging predicted right-ear superiority.</p><p><strong>Conclusions: </strong>Interhemispheric transfer is disrupted in individuals with SBM and hypogenesis or severe hypoplasia of the posterior corpus callosum. Preservation of interhemispheric transfer is related to expected connections through the posterior corpus callosum and possibly compensatory pathways in the anterior commissure. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":19205,"journal":{"name":"Neuropsychology","volume":"38 8","pages":"687-698"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142546472","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Nonpharmacological treatment for older adults with mild cognitive impairment: Considerations for culturally informed clinical practice and research. 对患有轻度认知障碍的老年人进行非药物治疗:考虑文化因素的临床实践和研究。
IF 2.6 3区 心理学 Q3 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-10-01 Epub Date: 2024-08-29 DOI: 10.1037/neu0000965
Martina Azar, Julija Stelmokas, Anthony Stringer, Franchesca Arias

Objective: Numerous nonpharmacological treatments (NPTs) have been developed for older adults with mild cognitive impairment (MCI). Two forms of cognition-focused NPTs, cognitive rehabilitation (CR) and cognitive training (CT), demonstrate cognitive benefit, but limitations remain regarding the contribution of cultural and demographic factors to study outcome heterogeneity, generalizability to diverse populations, and feasibility. This article aimed to review demographic and culturally informed NPTs and provides recommendations for culturally informed clinical practice and research.

Method: We conducted a PubMed review to identify CR and CT interventions that incorporated cultural adaptations. Results from the review, combined with the authors' clinical expertise, were used to identify methodological, demographic, social, cultural, and systemic variables relevant to NPTs.

Results: Existing CR and CT studies that included cultural adaptations adopted modifications to language, measures (cognition, function), and lifestyle factors (diet, physical activity) among others. In addition, provider, patient, and group-level factors were then raised to promote inclusivity and increase NPT generalizability. Nevertheless, there is a paucity of research considering cultural and demographic factors when delivering cognition-focused NPTs. Recommendations were generated that incorporated current literature as well as the authors' clinical and research experiences.

Conclusions: Culturally informed NPTs are understudied. Social, demographic, and cultural factors may contribute to the heterogeneity of outcomes, lack of generalizability of findings to diverse groups, and application of intervention to said groups. Several tools are available and can focus on broadening collection of information regarding patients' identities, social network, adapting to literacy level and linguistic diversity needs, and responding to social and structural determinants of health. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

目的:针对患有轻度认知障碍(MCI)的老年人开发了许多非药物疗法(NPT)。认知康复(CR)和认知训练(CT)这两种以认知为重点的非药物治疗方法显示出了认知方面的益处,但在文化和人口因素对研究结果异质性的影响、对不同人群的普适性以及可行性方面仍存在局限性。本文旨在回顾人口和文化因素的 NPT,并为文化因素的临床实践和研究提供建议:方法:我们在 PubMed 上进行了综述,以确定包含文化适应性的 CR 和 CT 干预措施。综述结果与作者的临床专业知识相结合,用于确定与 NPT 相关的方法、人口、社会、文化和系统变量:包括文化适应性在内的现有 CR 和 CT 研究对语言、测量方法(认知、功能)和生活方式因素(饮食、体育活动)等进行了修改。此外,还增加了提供者、患者和群体层面的因素,以促进包容性并提高 NPT 的普遍性。然而,在提供以认知为重点的 NPT 时,考虑文化和人口因素的研究还很少。我们结合当前的文献以及作者的临床和研究经验提出了一些建议:结论:基于文化的 NPT 研究不足。社会、人口和文化因素可能会导致结果的异质性、研究结果缺乏对不同群体的普适性以及对上述群体的干预应用。有几种工具可供选择,重点是扩大收集有关患者身份、社会网络、适应文化水平和语言多样性需求的信息,以及应对健康的社会和结构性决定因素。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Nonpharmacological treatment for older adults with mild cognitive impairment: Considerations for culturally informed clinical practice and research.","authors":"Martina Azar, Julija Stelmokas, Anthony Stringer, Franchesca Arias","doi":"10.1037/neu0000965","DOIUrl":"10.1037/neu0000965","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Objective: </strong>Numerous nonpharmacological treatments (NPTs) have been developed for older adults with mild cognitive impairment (MCI). Two forms of cognition-focused NPTs, cognitive rehabilitation (CR) and cognitive training (CT), demonstrate cognitive benefit, but limitations remain regarding the contribution of cultural and demographic factors to study outcome heterogeneity, generalizability to diverse populations, and feasibility. This article aimed to review demographic and culturally informed NPTs and provides recommendations for culturally informed clinical practice and research.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>We conducted a PubMed review to identify CR and CT interventions that incorporated cultural adaptations. Results from the review, combined with the authors' clinical expertise, were used to identify methodological, demographic, social, cultural, and systemic variables relevant to NPTs.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>Existing CR and CT studies that included cultural adaptations adopted modifications to language, measures (cognition, function), and lifestyle factors (diet, physical activity) among others. In addition, provider, patient, and group-level factors were then raised to promote inclusivity and increase NPT generalizability. Nevertheless, there is a paucity of research considering cultural and demographic factors when delivering cognition-focused NPTs. Recommendations were generated that incorporated current literature as well as the authors' clinical and research experiences.</p><p><strong>Conclusions: </strong>Culturally informed NPTs are understudied. Social, demographic, and cultural factors may contribute to the heterogeneity of outcomes, lack of generalizability of findings to diverse groups, and application of intervention to said groups. Several tools are available and can focus on broadening collection of information regarding patients' identities, social network, adapting to literacy level and linguistic diversity needs, and responding to social and structural determinants of health. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":19205,"journal":{"name":"Neuropsychology","volume":" ","pages":"609-621"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142109928","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The association of sociodemographic factors with total and item-level semantic fluency metrics. 社会人口因素与总语义流畅度和项目语义流畅度指标之间的关联。
IF 2.6 3区 心理学 Q3 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-10-01 Epub Date: 2024-08-29 DOI: 10.1037/neu0000955
Magdalena Beran, Emma L Twait, Annelot P Smit, Marleen F Posthuma, Demi van Dijk, Katherinne M Rabanal, Dayanara Rosado, Roxanna J Flores, Carolyn L Qian, Shana S Samuel, Gelan Ying, Richard Mayeux, Thomas T van Sloten, Miranda T Schram, Jennifer J Manly, Mirjam I Geerlings, Jet M J Vonk

Objective: We aimed to estimate the association of age, education, and sex/gender with semantic fluency performance as measured by the standard total number of words as well as novel item-level metrics and to descriptively compare associations across cohorts with different recruitment strategies and sample compositions.

Method: Cross-sectional data from 2,391 individuals from three cohorts were used: Washington Heights/Inwood Columbia Aging Project, a community-based cohort; Second Manifestations of ARTerial disease-Magnetic Resonance, a clinic-based cohort; and African American Alzheimer's Disease Genetics Study, a volunteer-based cohort. Total number of correct words and six item-level semantic fluency metrics were included as main outcomes: average cluster size, number of cluster switches, lexical/Zipf frequency, age of acquisition, and lexical decision response time. General linear models were run separately in each cohort to model the association between sociodemographic variables and semantic fluency metrics.

Results: Across cohorts, older age was associated with a lower total score and fewer cluster switches. Higher level of education was associated with naming more words, performing more cluster switches, and naming words with a longer lexical decision response time, lower frequency of occurrence, or later age of acquisition. Being female compared to male was associated with naming fewer words, smaller cluster sizes, naming words with a longer lexical decision response time, and lower age of acquisition. The effects varied in strength but were in a similar direction across cohorts.

Conclusions: Item-level semantic fluency metrics-similar to the standard total score-are sensitive to the effects of age, education, and sex/gender. The results suggest geographical, cultural, and cross-linguistic generalizability of these sociodemographic effects on semantic fluency performance. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

目的我们的目的是估计年龄、教育程度和性别与语义流利程度的关系,语义流利程度是通过标准单词总数以及新的条目级指标来衡量的,并对不同招募策略和样本组成的队列之间的关系进行描述性比较:研究使用了来自三个队列的 2391 人的横截面数据:华盛顿高地/英伍德哥伦比亚老龄化项目是一项基于社区的队列研究;动脉疾病的第二次表现--磁共振是一项基于诊所的队列研究;非裔美国人阿尔茨海默病遗传学研究是一项基于志愿者的队列研究。主要结果包括正确单词总数和六个项目级语义流畅度指标:平均聚类大小、聚类切换次数、词性/Zipf频率、习得年龄和词性决策响应时间。在每个队列中分别运行一般线性模型,以模拟社会人口变量与语义流畅度指标之间的关联:结果:在所有组群中,年龄越大,总分越低,组群切换越少。受教育程度越高,命名的单词越多,群组转换越多,命名单词的词性判断反应时间越长,出现频率越低,或获得单词的年龄越晚。与男性相比,女性与命名较少的单词、较小的词群规模、命名单词的词性判断反应时间较长以及较低的习得年龄有关。不同组群的影响强度不同,但方向相似:结论:项目层面的语义流畅度指标--类似于标准总分--对年龄、教育程度和性别的影响非常敏感。结果表明,这些社会人口因素对语义流畅性表现的影响具有地域、文化和跨语言的普遍性。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"The association of sociodemographic factors with total and item-level semantic fluency metrics.","authors":"Magdalena Beran, Emma L Twait, Annelot P Smit, Marleen F Posthuma, Demi van Dijk, Katherinne M Rabanal, Dayanara Rosado, Roxanna J Flores, Carolyn L Qian, Shana S Samuel, Gelan Ying, Richard Mayeux, Thomas T van Sloten, Miranda T Schram, Jennifer J Manly, Mirjam I Geerlings, Jet M J Vonk","doi":"10.1037/neu0000955","DOIUrl":"10.1037/neu0000955","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Objective: </strong>We aimed to estimate the association of age, education, and sex/gender with semantic fluency performance as measured by the standard total number of words as well as novel item-level metrics and to descriptively compare associations across cohorts with different recruitment strategies and sample compositions.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>Cross-sectional data from 2,391 individuals from three cohorts were used: Washington Heights/Inwood Columbia Aging Project, a community-based cohort; Second Manifestations of ARTerial disease-Magnetic Resonance, a clinic-based cohort; and African American Alzheimer's Disease Genetics Study, a volunteer-based cohort. Total number of correct words and six item-level semantic fluency metrics were included as main outcomes: average cluster size, number of cluster switches, lexical/Zipf frequency, age of acquisition, and lexical decision response time. General linear models were run separately in each cohort to model the association between sociodemographic variables and semantic fluency metrics.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>Across cohorts, older age was associated with a lower total score and fewer cluster switches. Higher level of education was associated with naming more words, performing more cluster switches, and naming words with a longer lexical decision response time, lower frequency of occurrence, or later age of acquisition. Being female compared to male was associated with naming fewer words, smaller cluster sizes, naming words with a longer lexical decision response time, and lower age of acquisition. The effects varied in strength but were in a similar direction across cohorts.</p><p><strong>Conclusions: </strong>Item-level semantic fluency metrics-similar to the standard total score-are sensitive to the effects of age, education, and sex/gender. The results suggest geographical, cultural, and cross-linguistic generalizability of these sociodemographic effects on semantic fluency performance. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":19205,"journal":{"name":"Neuropsychology","volume":" ","pages":"665-678"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142109929","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The effect of aging on the dual-route model of emotion processing applied to memory recognition. 衰老对应用于记忆识别的情感处理双路径模型的影响。
IF 2.6 3区 心理学 Q3 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-10-01 Epub Date: 2024-08-29 DOI: 10.1037/neu0000972
Giulia Prete, Rocco Palumbo, Irene Ceccato, Adolfo Di Crosta, Pasquale La Malva, Valentina Sforza, Bruno Laeng, Luca Tommasi, Alberto Di Domenico, Nicola Mammarella

Objective: Emotional faces are automatically processed in the human brain through a cortical route (conscious processing based on high spatial frequencies, HSF) and a subcortical route (subliminal processing based on low spatial frequencies, LSF). How each route contributes to emotional face recognition is still debated, and little is known about this process in aging.

Method: Here, 147 younger adults (YA) and 137 older adults (OA) were passively presented with neutral, happy, and angry faces, shown as (a) unfiltered, (b) filtered at LSF, and (c) hybrid (emotional LSF superimposed to the neutral HSF of the same face). In a succeeding recognition phase, the same faces and new faces were shown as unfiltered, and participants were asked whether each face had been already presented in the encoding phase.

Results: Despite the better performance by YA compared with OA for neutral faces presented as unfiltered (cortical route), the performance of OA was better than that of YA for angry faces presented as hybrid and for happy faces presented at LSF and as hybrid.

Conclusions: We conclude that the activity of the subcortical route during the encoding phase facilitates emotional recognition in aging. Results are discussed in accordance with the dual-route model. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

研究目的人脑会通过皮层路径(基于高空间频率的有意识处理,HSF)和皮层下路径(基于低空间频率的潜意识处理,LSF)自动处理情感面孔。这两种途径对情绪化人脸识别的贡献如何仍存在争议,而对这一过程在衰老过程中的作用则知之甚少:在这里,147 名年轻成人(YA)和 137 名老年人(OA)被动地看到了中性、快乐和愤怒的人脸,这些人脸分别显示为(a)未过滤、(b)LSF 过滤和(c)混合(同一人脸的情绪 LSF 与中性 HSF 叠加)。在随后的识别阶段,相同的人脸和新人脸以未经过滤的形式显示,参与者被问及每张人脸是否已在编码阶段出现过:结果:尽管在识别未经过滤的中性面孔时,YA 的表现优于 OA(皮层路径),但在识别混合显示的愤怒面孔时,以及在识别 LSF 和混合显示的快乐面孔时,OA 的表现优于 YA:我们得出的结论是,皮层下途径在编码阶段的活动促进了老龄人的情绪识别。我们将根据双路径模型对结果进行讨论。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"The effect of aging on the dual-route model of emotion processing applied to memory recognition.","authors":"Giulia Prete, Rocco Palumbo, Irene Ceccato, Adolfo Di Crosta, Pasquale La Malva, Valentina Sforza, Bruno Laeng, Luca Tommasi, Alberto Di Domenico, Nicola Mammarella","doi":"10.1037/neu0000972","DOIUrl":"10.1037/neu0000972","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Objective: </strong>Emotional faces are automatically processed in the human brain through a cortical route (conscious processing based on high spatial frequencies, HSF) and a subcortical route (subliminal processing based on low spatial frequencies, LSF). How each route contributes to emotional face recognition is still debated, and little is known about this process in aging.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>Here, 147 younger adults (YA) and 137 older adults (OA) were passively presented with neutral, happy, and angry faces, shown as (a) unfiltered, (b) filtered at LSF, and (c) hybrid (emotional LSF superimposed to the neutral HSF of the same face). In a succeeding recognition phase, the same faces and new faces were shown as unfiltered, and participants were asked whether each face had been already presented in the encoding phase.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>Despite the better performance by YA compared with OA for neutral faces presented as unfiltered (cortical route), the performance of OA was better than that of YA for angry faces presented as hybrid and for happy faces presented at LSF and as hybrid.</p><p><strong>Conclusions: </strong>We conclude that the activity of the subcortical route during the encoding phase facilitates emotional recognition in aging. Results are discussed in accordance with the dual-route model. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":19205,"journal":{"name":"Neuropsychology","volume":" ","pages":"679-686"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142109930","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Cognitive dispersion and its functional relevance in behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia and prodromal behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia. 行为变异型额颞叶痴呆症和前驱行为变异型额颞叶痴呆症的认知分散及其功能相关性。
IF 2.6 3区 心理学 Q3 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-10-01 Epub Date: 2024-08-29 DOI: 10.1037/neu0000969
Troy A Webber, Steven P Woods, Sara A Lorkiewicz, Holley W Yazbeck, Elaine R Schultz, Andrew M Kiselica

Objective: Executive dysfunction is characteristic of behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia (bvFTD) but can be challenging to detect. Dispersion-based intraindividual variability (IIV-d) is hypothesized to reflect a sensitive index of executive dysfunction and has demonstrated relevance to functional decline but has not been evaluated in bvFTD.

Method: We report on 477 demographically matched participants (159 cognitively healthy [CH], 159 clinical Alzheimer's disease [AD], 159 clinical bvFTD/prodromal bvFTD) who completed the Uniform Data Set 3.0 Neuropsychological Battery. IIV-d was measured using the coefficient of variance (CoV; raw and demographically adjusted) across 12 Uniform Data Set 3.0 Neuropsychological Battery indicators and the informant-rated Functional Activities Questionnaire assessed daily functioning.

Results: Analysis of covariance showed that participants in the bvFTD/prodromal bvFTD group exhibited higher raw and demographically adjusted CoV compared to CH participants, at a very large effect size (d = 1.28-1.47). Demographically adjusted (but not raw) CoV was lower in the bvFTD/prodromal bvFTD group than the AD group, though the effect size was small (d = .38). Both CoV metrics accurately differentiated the bvFTD/prodromal bvFTD and CH groups (areas under the curve = .84), but not bvFTD/prodromal bvFTD and AD groups (areas under the curve = .59). Regression analyses in the bvFTD/prodromal bvFTD group indicated that higher IIV-d on both metrics was associated with greater daily functioning impairment, over and above covariates.

Conclusions: Compared to healthy adults, individuals with bvFTD/prodromal bvFTD show greater levels of performance variability across a battery of neuropsychological measures, which interferes with everyday functioning. These data demonstrate the clinical utility and ecological validity of IIV-d in bvFTD/prodromal bvFTD, though these findings should be replicated in more diverse samples. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

目的:执行功能障碍是行为变异性额颞叶痴呆症(bvFTD)的特征,但检测起来却很困难。基于离散度的个体内变异性(IIV-d)被假定为反映执行功能障碍的敏感指标,并已被证明与功能衰退相关,但尚未在bvFTD中进行评估:我们报告了完成统一数据集 3.0 神经心理测试的 477 名人口统计学匹配参与者(159 名认知健康者 [CH]、159 名临床阿尔茨海默病患者 [AD]、159 名临床 bvFTD/前驱 bvFTD)的情况。IIV-d采用12项统一数据集3.0神经心理测验指标的方差系数(CoV;原始数据和人口统计学调整后数据)进行测量,由线人评定的功能活动问卷对日常功能进行评估:协方差分析表明,bvFTD/前驱bvFTD组的参与者与CH组的参与者相比,表现出更高的原始CoV和经人口统计学调整的CoV,其效应大小非常大(d = 1.28-1.47)。bvFTD/prodromal bvFTD 组经人口统计学调整的 CoV(而非原始 CoV)低于 AD 组,但效应大小较小(d = 0.38)。两种 CoV 指标都能准确区分 bvFTD/前驱 bvFTD 组和 CH 组(曲线下面积 = .84),但不能区分 bvFTD/前驱 bvFTD 组和 AD 组(曲线下面积 = .59)。bvFTD/prodromal bvFTD组的回归分析表明,两项指标的IIV-d越高,日常功能损害越大,超过了协变量:结论:与健康成年人相比,bvFTD/前驱bvFTD患者在一系列神经心理测量中表现出更大的变异性,从而影响了日常功能。这些数据证明了IIV-d在bvFTD/萌芽期bvFTD患者中的临床实用性和生态学有效性,尽管这些研究结果应该在更多不同的样本中得到重复。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"Cognitive dispersion and its functional relevance in behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia and prodromal behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia.","authors":"Troy A Webber, Steven P Woods, Sara A Lorkiewicz, Holley W Yazbeck, Elaine R Schultz, Andrew M Kiselica","doi":"10.1037/neu0000969","DOIUrl":"10.1037/neu0000969","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Objective: </strong>Executive dysfunction is characteristic of behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia (bvFTD) but can be challenging to detect. Dispersion-based intraindividual variability (IIV-d) is hypothesized to reflect a sensitive index of executive dysfunction and has demonstrated relevance to functional decline but has not been evaluated in bvFTD.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>We report on 477 demographically matched participants (159 cognitively healthy [CH], 159 clinical Alzheimer's disease [AD], 159 clinical bvFTD/prodromal bvFTD) who completed the Uniform Data Set 3.0 Neuropsychological Battery. IIV-d was measured using the coefficient of variance (CoV; raw and demographically adjusted) across 12 Uniform Data Set 3.0 Neuropsychological Battery indicators and the informant-rated Functional Activities Questionnaire assessed daily functioning.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>Analysis of covariance showed that participants in the bvFTD/prodromal bvFTD group exhibited higher raw and demographically adjusted CoV compared to CH participants, at a very large effect size (<i>d</i> = 1.28-1.47). Demographically adjusted (but not raw) CoV was lower in the bvFTD/prodromal bvFTD group than the AD group, though the effect size was small (<i>d</i> = .38). Both CoV metrics accurately differentiated the bvFTD/prodromal bvFTD and CH groups (areas under the curve = .84), but not bvFTD/prodromal bvFTD and AD groups (areas under the curve = .59). Regression analyses in the bvFTD/prodromal bvFTD group indicated that higher IIV-d on both metrics was associated with greater daily functioning impairment, over and above covariates.</p><p><strong>Conclusions: </strong>Compared to healthy adults, individuals with bvFTD/prodromal bvFTD show greater levels of performance variability across a battery of neuropsychological measures, which interferes with everyday functioning. These data demonstrate the clinical utility and ecological validity of IIV-d in bvFTD/prodromal bvFTD, though these findings should be replicated in more diverse samples. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":19205,"journal":{"name":"Neuropsychology","volume":" ","pages":"637-652"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11449635/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142110010","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Sprinting in two languages: Picture naming performance of older Spanish-English bilinguals on the Multilingual Naming Test Sprint 2.0. 用两种语言冲刺:在多语言命名测试 Sprint 2.0 中,年长的西班牙-英语双语者的图片命名能力。
IF 2.6 3区 心理学 Q3 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-10-01 Epub Date: 2024-07-11 DOI: 10.1037/neu0000958
Tamar H Gollan, Dalia L Garcia, Mayra Murillo, Jocelyn Vargas, Brandon Pulido, David P Salmon

Objective: The present study examined how years of immersion in a nondominant language affect (a) the degree of bilingualism as measured by picture naming scores and (b) the bilingual disadvantage relative to monolinguals.

Method: Forty-two older Spanish-English bilinguals named pictures in an expanded rapid administration version of the Multilingual Naming Test (MINT Sprint 2.0) in both languages and completed a language history questionnaire. English-speaking monolinguals (n = 138; from Gollan et al., 2024) named pictures in just one language.

Results: Spanish-dominant bilinguals named more pictures in the nondominant language but fewer pictures in the dominant language relative to English-dominant bilinguals. Increased years of immersion in the nondominant language increased naming scores in that language but decreased naming scores in the dominant language. When controlling for differences in age and education level, monolinguals named more pictures than bilinguals even in their dominant language, a difference that was numerically smaller for English-dominant bilinguals. However, two bilinguals who stated that they prefer to be tested in English scored much higher in Spanish.

Conclusions: Older bilinguals name fewer pictures than demographically matched monolinguals even when bilinguals are tested in their dominant language and especially if they report many years of immersion in their nondominant language. The bilingual disadvantage can be magnified if self-reported language preference is used to determine the language of testing. Accurate interpretation of bilingual picture naming scores requires a thorough language history and objective assessment in both languages, which can be done in relatively little time using rapid administration procedures. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

研究目的本研究探讨了浸泡在非主导语言环境中的年限如何影响(a)以图片命名得分衡量的双语程度和(b)双语者相对于单语者的劣势:方法:42 名年龄较大的西班牙-英语双语者用两种语言在扩大的多语言命名测试(MINT Sprint 2.0)快速施测版中命名图片,并填写一份语言历史问卷。英语单语者(n = 138;来自 Gollan 等人,2024 年)仅用一种语言命名图片:结果:与英语为主的双语者相比,西班牙语为主的双语者用非主导语言命名的图片较多,而用主导语言命名的图片较少。浸泡在非主导语言环境中的年数增加,会提高该语言的命名分数,但会降低主导语言的命名分数。在控制年龄和教育水平差异的情况下,单语学生即使用主导语言命名的图片也比双语学生多,而英语主导的双语学生在数字上的差异较小。然而,有两名双语者表示他们更喜欢用英语进行测试,他们的西班牙语得分要高得多:结论:老年双语者说出的图片数量少于人口统计学上相匹配的单语者,即使双语者用他们的主导语言进行测试,尤其是当他们报告说多年浸泡在他们的非主导语言环境中时。如果使用自我报告的语言偏好来确定测试语言,那么双语者的劣势就会被放大。准确解释双语图画命名得分需要全面的语言史和两种语言的客观评估,而这些都可以通过快速的管理程序在相对较短的时间内完成。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Sprinting in two languages: Picture naming performance of older Spanish-English bilinguals on the Multilingual Naming Test Sprint 2.0.","authors":"Tamar H Gollan, Dalia L Garcia, Mayra Murillo, Jocelyn Vargas, Brandon Pulido, David P Salmon","doi":"10.1037/neu0000958","DOIUrl":"10.1037/neu0000958","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Objective: </strong>The present study examined how years of immersion in a nondominant language affect (a) the degree of bilingualism as measured by picture naming scores and (b) the bilingual disadvantage relative to monolinguals.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>Forty-two older Spanish-English bilinguals named pictures in an expanded rapid administration version of the Multilingual Naming Test (MINT Sprint 2.0) in both languages and completed a language history questionnaire. English-speaking monolinguals (<i>n</i> = 138; from Gollan et al., 2024) named pictures in just one language.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>Spanish-dominant bilinguals named more pictures in the nondominant language but fewer pictures in the dominant language relative to English-dominant bilinguals. Increased years of immersion in the nondominant language increased naming scores in that language but decreased naming scores in the dominant language. When controlling for differences in age and education level, monolinguals named more pictures than bilinguals even in their dominant language, a difference that was numerically smaller for English-dominant bilinguals. However, two bilinguals who stated that they prefer to be tested in English scored much higher in Spanish.</p><p><strong>Conclusions: </strong>Older bilinguals name fewer pictures than demographically matched monolinguals even when bilinguals are tested in their dominant language and especially if they report many years of immersion in their nondominant language. The bilingual disadvantage can be magnified if self-reported language preference is used to determine the language of testing. Accurate interpretation of bilingual picture naming scores requires a thorough language history and objective assessment in both languages, which can be done in relatively little time using rapid administration procedures. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":19205,"journal":{"name":"Neuropsychology","volume":" ","pages":"653-664"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141590854","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Negative global metacognitive biases are associated with depressive and posttraumatic stress disorder symptoms and improve with targeted or game-based cognitive training. 消极的全球元认知偏差与抑郁和创伤后应激障碍症状有关,通过有针对性的或基于游戏的认知训练,这些偏差会得到改善。
IF 2.6 3区 心理学 Q3 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-10-01 Epub Date: 2024-08-29 DOI: 10.1037/neu0000967
Sam Agnoli, Henry Mahncke, Sarah-Jane Grant, Zachary T Goodman, William P Milberg, Michael Esterman, Joseph DeGutis

Objective: Metacognition is disrupted in several clinical populations. One aspect of metacognition, global metacognitive bias (difference between objective and self-reported abilities), has shown to be particularly relevant to clinical functioning. However, previous studies of global metacognitive biases in populations with elevated depressive/posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) symptoms have not measured objective and self-reported abilities relative to normative samples, limiting the quantification of biases. Additionally, few studies have examined whether cognitive interventions can improve metacognitive biases or how this relates to depressive/PTSD symptom severity.

Method: A total of 84 participants with mild traumatic brain injury (77% veterans) performed PTSD and depression assessments along with self-reported and objective measures of global cognition. Age-adjusted norm-based z scores were used for self-reported and objective cognition, and bias was calculated by subtracting objective minus self-report scores. Participants then received 13 weeks of targeted cognitive training or entertainment games training (both providing performance feedback). Participants were assessed at baseline, immediately posttraining, and 3 months posttraining.

Results: We found large negative metacognitive biases in those with clinically significant severity of depressive symptoms (z score difference = -1.77), PTSD symptoms (-1.47), and depressive + PTSD symptoms (-2.29). Metacognitive biases improved after both targeted and entertainment training and was associated with reductions in depressive/PTSD symptom severity (r = -.41/-.42, respectively), led by the entertainment training group (r = -.54/-.46, respectively).

Conclusions: These findings show that clinically significant severity of depressive/PTSD symptoms is associated with substantial negative global metacognitive biases and preliminarily suggests that cognitive training may improve these biases and depressive/PTSD symptom severity. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

目的元认知在一些临床人群中受到干扰。元认知的一个方面,即整体元认知偏差(客观能力与自我报告能力之间的差异),已被证明与临床功能特别相关。然而,以往对抑郁/创伤后应激障碍(PTSD)症状加重人群的整体元认知偏差的研究并没有测量相对于常模样本的客观能力和自我报告能力,从而限制了对偏差的量化。此外,很少有研究探讨认知干预是否能改善元认知偏差,或这与抑郁/创伤后应激障碍症状严重程度的关系:共有 84 名轻度脑外伤患者(77% 为退伍军人)进行了创伤后应激障碍和抑郁评估,以及自我报告和客观的全球认知测量。自我报告和客观认知均采用年龄调整后的常模z分数,偏差则通过客观分数减去自我报告分数来计算。然后,参与者接受为期 13 周的定向认知训练或娱乐游戏训练(两者都提供成绩反馈)。分别在基线、训练后立即和训练后 3 个月对参与者进行评估:我们发现,在抑郁症状(z分数差异=-1.77)、创伤后应激障碍症状(-1.47)和抑郁+创伤后应激障碍症状(-2.29)具有临床显著严重性的人群中,元认知偏差较大。元认知偏差在定向训练和娱乐训练后都有所改善,并与抑郁/创伤后应激障碍症状严重程度的降低相关(r = -.41/-.42, respectively),其中娱乐训练组的改善幅度最大(r = -.54/-.46, respectively):这些研究结果表明,具有临床意义的抑郁/创伤后应激障碍症状的严重程度与大量负面的全球元认知偏差有关,并初步表明认知训练可改善这些偏差和抑郁/创伤后应激障碍症状的严重程度。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"Negative global metacognitive biases are associated with depressive and posttraumatic stress disorder symptoms and improve with targeted or game-based cognitive training.","authors":"Sam Agnoli, Henry Mahncke, Sarah-Jane Grant, Zachary T Goodman, William P Milberg, Michael Esterman, Joseph DeGutis","doi":"10.1037/neu0000967","DOIUrl":"10.1037/neu0000967","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Objective: </strong>Metacognition is disrupted in several clinical populations. One aspect of metacognition, global metacognitive bias (difference between objective and self-reported abilities), has shown to be particularly relevant to clinical functioning. However, previous studies of global metacognitive biases in populations with elevated depressive/posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) symptoms have not measured objective and self-reported abilities relative to normative samples, limiting the quantification of biases. Additionally, few studies have examined whether cognitive interventions can improve metacognitive biases or how this relates to depressive/PTSD symptom severity.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>A total of 84 participants with mild traumatic brain injury (77% veterans) performed PTSD and depression assessments along with self-reported and objective measures of global cognition. Age-adjusted norm-based z scores were used for self-reported and objective cognition, and bias was calculated by subtracting objective minus self-report scores. Participants then received 13 weeks of targeted cognitive training or entertainment games training (both providing performance feedback). Participants were assessed at baseline, immediately posttraining, and 3 months posttraining.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>We found large negative metacognitive biases in those with clinically significant severity of depressive symptoms (z score difference = -1.77), PTSD symptoms (-1.47), and depressive + PTSD symptoms (-2.29). Metacognitive biases improved after both targeted and entertainment training and was associated with reductions in depressive/PTSD symptom severity (<i>r</i> = -.41/-.42, respectively), led by the entertainment training group (<i>r</i> = -.54/-.46, respectively).</p><p><strong>Conclusions: </strong>These findings show that clinically significant severity of depressive/PTSD symptoms is associated with substantial negative global metacognitive biases and preliminarily suggests that cognitive training may improve these biases and depressive/PTSD symptom severity. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":19205,"journal":{"name":"Neuropsychology","volume":" ","pages":"622-636"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142110011","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The memory binding test in a longitudinal study of cognitive aging and preclinical disease. 认知老化和临床前疾病纵向研究中的记忆结合测试。
IF 2.6 3区 心理学 Q3 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-09-01 Epub Date: 2024-07-08 DOI: 10.1037/neu0000952
Stephanie Aghamoosa, Katrina S Rbeiz, Olivia Horn, Kathryn E Thorn, Andreana Benitez

Objective: The Memory Binding Test (MBT) shows promise in detecting early cognitive changes associated with Alzheimer's disease (AD). This study assesses the psychometric properties (i.e., construct and criterion validity, test-retest reliability) of the MBT and its sensitivity to incipient disease and incident cognitive impairment.

Method: One hundred forty-nine cognitively unimpaired adults ages 45-85 completed the MBT and neuropsychological tests at baseline; 132 returned for 2-year follow-up. Based on neuroradiological ratings of amyloid positron emission tomography and MRI markers at baseline, they were categorized as healthy (n = 94) or having preclinical disease (n = 55, either on the AD continuum or having non-AD pathologic change). Construct validity was assessed by the associations between MBT scores, demographics, and neuropsychological scores within the healthy group. Criterion validity was assessed by testing how MBT scores correlate with AD biomarkers, differ and discriminate between groups at baseline, and predict incident cognitive impairment.

Results: MBT scores decreased with age and were strongly associated with memory and global cognition. MBT scores were largely not associated with amyloid, hippocampal volume, or AD signature cortical volume but related to white matter lesion volume in those with preclinical disease. The preclinical groups performed worse on MBT immediate free recall at baseline than the healthy group, but no scores predicted incident cognitive impairment at follow-up. Most scores demonstrated modest test-retest reliability.

Conclusions: This study demonstrates that the MBT has adequate construct validity in cognitively unimpaired adults, moderate sensitivity to preclinical disease cross-sectionally, and limited prognostic utility. Careful consideration of demographic influences on score interpretation remains necessary. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

目的记忆束缚测试(MBT)有望检测出与阿尔茨海默病(AD)相关的早期认知变化。本研究评估了记忆绑定测试的心理计量特性(即构建和标准效度、重测可靠性)及其对初发疾病和偶发认知障碍的敏感性:149名年龄在45-85岁之间、认知能力未受损的成年人完成了基线MBT和神经心理学测试;132人返回进行为期2年的随访。根据基线时淀粉样蛋白正电子发射断层扫描和磁共振成像标记物的神经放射学评级,他们被分为健康人(94 人)或临床前疾病患者(55 人,处于注意力缺失症连续体或无注意力缺失症病理改变)。结构效度通过健康组中 MBT 分数、人口统计学和神经心理学分数之间的关联进行评估。标准有效性是通过测试 MBT 分数与注意力缺失症生物标志物的相关性、基线时各组之间的差异和区别以及预测认知障碍事件来评估的:结果:MBT 分数随着年龄的增长而降低,并与记忆和整体认知密切相关。MBT评分与淀粉样蛋白、海马体积或AD标志性皮质体积基本无关,但与临床前疾病患者的白质病变体积有关。与健康组相比,临床前疾病组在基线MBT即时自由回忆中的表现更差,但没有得分能预测随访时出现的认知障碍。大多数分数显示出适度的测试-再测试可靠性:这项研究表明,MBT 在认知功能未受损的成年人中具有充分的构建有效性,对临床前疾病的横断面敏感性适中,但预后效用有限。仍有必要仔细考虑人口统计学对分数解释的影响。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"The memory binding test in a longitudinal study of cognitive aging and preclinical disease.","authors":"Stephanie Aghamoosa, Katrina S Rbeiz, Olivia Horn, Kathryn E Thorn, Andreana Benitez","doi":"10.1037/neu0000952","DOIUrl":"10.1037/neu0000952","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Objective: </strong>The Memory Binding Test (MBT) shows promise in detecting early cognitive changes associated with Alzheimer's disease (AD). This study assesses the psychometric properties (i.e., construct and criterion validity, test-retest reliability) of the MBT and its sensitivity to incipient disease and incident cognitive impairment.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>One hundred forty-nine cognitively unimpaired adults ages 45-85 completed the MBT and neuropsychological tests at baseline; 132 returned for 2-year follow-up. Based on neuroradiological ratings of amyloid positron emission tomography and MRI markers at baseline, they were categorized as healthy (<i>n</i> = 94) or having preclinical disease (<i>n</i> = 55, either on the AD continuum or having non-AD pathologic change). Construct validity was assessed by the associations between MBT scores, demographics, and neuropsychological scores within the healthy group. Criterion validity was assessed by testing how MBT scores correlate with AD biomarkers, differ and discriminate between groups at baseline, and predict incident cognitive impairment.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>MBT scores decreased with age and were strongly associated with memory and global cognition. MBT scores were largely not associated with amyloid, hippocampal volume, or AD signature cortical volume but related to white matter lesion volume in those with preclinical disease. The preclinical groups performed worse on MBT immediate free recall at baseline than the healthy group, but no scores predicted incident cognitive impairment at follow-up. Most scores demonstrated modest test-retest reliability.</p><p><strong>Conclusions: </strong>This study demonstrates that the MBT has adequate construct validity in cognitively unimpaired adults, moderate sensitivity to preclinical disease cross-sectionally, and limited prognostic utility. Careful consideration of demographic influences on score interpretation remains necessary. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":19205,"journal":{"name":"Neuropsychology","volume":" ","pages":"570-588"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141559340","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Neuropsychology
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1