首页 > 最新文献

Neuropsychologia最新文献

英文 中文
Speech comprehension across time, space, frequency, and age: MEG-MVPA classification of intertrial phase coherence 跨时间、空间、频率和年龄的语音理解:序列间相位连贯的MEG-MVPA分类
IF 2.6 3区 心理学 Q3 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2023-09-09 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2023.108602
Mads Jensen , Rasha Hyder , Britta U. Westner , Andreas Højlund , Yury Shtyrov

Language is a key part of human cognition, essential for our well-being at all stages of our lives. Whereas many neurocognitive abilities decline with age, for language the picture is much less clear, and how exactly speech comprehension changes with ageing is still unknown. To investigate this, we employed magnetoencephalography (MEG) and recorded neuromagnetic brain responses to auditory linguistic stimuli in healthy participants of younger and older age using a passive task-free paradigm and a range of different linguistic stimulus contrasts, which enabled us to assess neural processing of spoken language at multiple levels (lexical, semantic, morphosyntactic). Using machine learning-based classification algorithms to scrutinise intertrial phase coherence of MEG responses in cortical source space, we found that patterns of oscillatory neural activity diverged between younger and older participants across several frequency bands (alpha, beta, gamma) for all tested linguistic information types. The results suggest multiple age-related changes in the brain's neurolinguistic circuits, which may be due to both healthy ageing in general and compensatory processes in particular.

语言是人类认知的重要组成部分,对我们生命各个阶段的幸福至关重要。尽管许多神经认知能力随着年龄的增长而下降,但对于语言来说,情况就不那么清楚了,言语理解究竟是如何随着年龄的变化的,目前还不得而知。为了研究这一点,我们使用脑磁图(MEG),并使用被动无任务范式和一系列不同的语言刺激对比记录了年轻和老年健康参与者对听觉语言刺激的神经磁脑反应,这使我们能够在多个层面(词汇、语义、形态句法)评估口语的神经处理。使用基于机器学习的分类算法来仔细检查皮层源空间中MEG反应的序列间相位一致性,我们发现,对于所有测试的语言信息类型,年轻和老年参与者的振荡神经活动模式在几个频带(α、β、γ)上存在差异。研究结果表明,大脑的神经语言回路发生了多种与年龄相关的变化,这可能是由于一般的健康衰老,尤其是代偿过程。
{"title":"Speech comprehension across time, space, frequency, and age: MEG-MVPA classification of intertrial phase coherence","authors":"Mads Jensen ,&nbsp;Rasha Hyder ,&nbsp;Britta U. Westner ,&nbsp;Andreas Højlund ,&nbsp;Yury Shtyrov","doi":"10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2023.108602","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2023.108602","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Language is a key part of human cognition, essential for our well-being at all stages of our lives. Whereas many neurocognitive abilities decline with age, for language the picture is much less clear, and how exactly speech comprehension changes with ageing is still unknown. To investigate this, we employed magnetoencephalography (MEG) and recorded neuromagnetic brain responses to auditory linguistic stimuli in healthy participants of younger and older age using a passive task-free paradigm and a range of different linguistic stimulus contrasts, which enabled us to assess neural processing of spoken language at multiple levels (lexical, semantic, morphosyntactic). Using machine learning-based classification algorithms to scrutinise intertrial phase coherence of MEG responses in cortical source space, we found that patterns of oscillatory neural activity diverged between younger and older participants across several frequency bands (alpha, beta, gamma) for all tested linguistic information types. The results suggest multiple age-related changes in the brain's neurolinguistic circuits, which may be due to both healthy ageing in general and compensatory processes in particular.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":19279,"journal":{"name":"Neuropsychologia","volume":"188 ","pages":"Article 108602"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2023-09-09","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10195237","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The effect of context congruency on fMRI repetition suppression for objects 上下文一致性对物体fMRI重复抑制的影响
IF 2.6 3区 心理学 Q3 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2023-09-09 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2023.108603
Chenglin Li , Linda Ficco , Sabrina Trapp , Sophie-Marie Rostalski , Lukas Korn , Gyula Kovács

The recognition of objects is strongly facilitated when they are presented in the context of other objects (Biederman, 1972). Such contexts facilitate perception and induce expectations of context-congruent objects (Trapp and Bar, 2015). The neural mechanisms underlying these facilitatory effects of context on object processing, however, are not yet fully understood. In the present study, we investigate how context-induced expectations affect subsequent object processing. We used functional magnetic resonance imaging and measured repetition suppression as a proxy for prediction error processing. Participants viewed pairs of alternating or repeated object images which were preceded by context-congruent, context-incongruent or neutral cues. We found a stronger repetition suppression in congruent as compared to incongruent or neutral cues in the object sensitive lateral occipital cortex. Interestingly, this stronger effect was driven by enhanced responses to alternating stimulus pairs in the congruent contexts, rather than by suppressed responses to repeated stimulus pairs, which emphasizes the contribution of surprise-related response enhancement for the context modulation on RS when expectations are violated. In addition, in the congruent condition, we discovered significant functional connectivity between object-responsive and frontal cortical regions, as well as between object-responsive regions and the fusiform gyrus. Our findings indicate that prediction errors, reflected in enhanced brain responses to violated contextual expectations, underlie the facilitating effect of context during object perception.

当物体在其他物体的背景下呈现时,会大大促进物体的识别(Biederman,1972)。这种情境有助于感知并诱导对情境一致对象的期望(Trapp和Bar,2015)。然而,上下文对对象处理的这些促进作用的神经机制尚不完全清楚。在本研究中,我们研究了上下文诱导的期望如何影响后续的对象处理。我们使用功能磁共振成像和测量的重复抑制作为预测误差处理的代理。参与者观看交替或重复的对象图像对,这些图像之前有上下文一致、上下文不一致或中性提示。我们发现,在对物体敏感的枕外侧皮层,与不一致或中性线索相比,一致线索的重复抑制更强。有趣的是,这种更强的效应是由一致情境中对交替刺激对的增强反应驱动的,而不是由对重复刺激对的抑制反应驱动的。重复刺激对强调了当期望被违反时,与惊喜相关的反应增强对RS的情境调节的贡献。此外,在一致条件下,我们发现物体反应区和额叶皮层区域之间以及物体反应区与梭状回之间存在显著的功能连接。我们的研究结果表明,预测错误反映在大脑对违反上下文预期的反应增强上,是上下文在物体感知过程中的促进作用的基础。
{"title":"The effect of context congruency on fMRI repetition suppression for objects","authors":"Chenglin Li ,&nbsp;Linda Ficco ,&nbsp;Sabrina Trapp ,&nbsp;Sophie-Marie Rostalski ,&nbsp;Lukas Korn ,&nbsp;Gyula Kovács","doi":"10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2023.108603","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2023.108603","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>The recognition of objects is strongly facilitated when they are presented in the context of other objects (Biederman, 1972). Such contexts facilitate perception and induce expectations of context-congruent objects (Trapp and Bar, 2015). The neural mechanisms underlying these facilitatory effects of context on object processing, however, are not yet fully understood. In the present study, we investigate how context-induced expectations affect subsequent object processing. We used functional magnetic resonance imaging and measured repetition suppression as a proxy for prediction error processing. Participants viewed pairs of alternating or repeated object images which were preceded by context-congruent, context-incongruent or neutral cues. We found a stronger repetition suppression in congruent as compared to incongruent or neutral cues in the object sensitive lateral occipital cortex. Interestingly, this stronger effect was driven by <em>enhanced</em> responses to alternating stimulus pairs in the congruent contexts, rather than by suppressed responses to repeated stimulus pairs, which emphasizes the contribution of surprise-related response enhancement for the context modulation on RS when expectations are violated. In addition, in the congruent condition, we discovered significant functional connectivity between object-responsive and frontal cortical regions, as well as between object-responsive regions and the fusiform gyrus. Our findings indicate that prediction errors, reflected in enhanced brain responses to violated contextual expectations, underlie the facilitating effect of context during object perception.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":19279,"journal":{"name":"Neuropsychologia","volume":"188 ","pages":"Article 108603"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2023-09-09","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10195238","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Cognitive processing stages in mental rotation – How can cognitive modelling inform HsMM-EEG models? 心理旋转中的认知处理阶段——认知建模如何为HsMM EEG模型提供信息?
IF 2.6 3区 心理学 Q3 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2023-09-09 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2023.108615
Linda Heimisch , Kai Preuss , Nele Russwinkel

The aspiration for insight into human cognitive processing has traditionally driven research in cognitive science. With methods such as the Hidden semi-Markov Model-Electroencephalography (HsMM-EEG) method, new approaches have been developed that help to understand the temporal structure of cognition by identifying temporally discrete processing stages. However, it remains challenging to assign concrete functional contributions by specific processing stages to the overall cognitive process. In this paper, we address this challenge by linking HsMM-EEG3 with cognitive modelling, with the aim of further validating the HsMM-EEG3 method and demonstrating the potential of cognitive models to facilitate functional interpretation of processing stages. For this purpose, we applied HsMM-EEG3 to data from a mental rotation task and developed an ACT-R cognitive model that is able to closely replicate human performance in this task. Applying HsMM-EEG3 to the mental rotation experiment data revealed a strong likelihood for 6 distinct stages of cognitive processing during trials, with an additional stage for non-rotated conditions. The cognitive model predicted intra-trial mental activity patterns that project well onto the processing stages, while explaining the additional stage as a marker of non-spatial shortcut use. Thereby, this combined methodology provided substantially more information than either method by itself and suggests conclusions for cognitive processing in general.

对深入了解人类认知过程的渴望传统上推动了认知科学的研究。通过隐半马尔可夫模型脑电图(HsMM-EG)方法等方法,已经开发出新的方法,通过识别时间离散的处理阶段来帮助理解认知的时间结构。然而,通过特定的处理阶段为整个认知过程分配具体的功能贡献仍然具有挑战性。在本文中,我们通过将HsMM-EEG3与认知建模联系起来来应对这一挑战,目的是进一步验证HsMM-EEG 3方法,并展示认知模型促进加工阶段功能解释的潜力。为此,我们将HsMM-EEG3应用于心理旋转任务的数据,并开发了一个能够密切复制人类在该任务中表现的ACT-R认知模型。将HsMM-EEG3应用于心理旋转实验数据显示,在试验期间,认知处理的6个不同阶段的可能性很大,非旋转条件下还有一个额外的阶段。认知模型预测了试验中的心理活动模式,这些模式很好地投射到处理阶段,同时将附加阶段解释为非空间快捷方式使用的标志。因此,这种组合方法比任何一种方法本身提供了更多的信息,并为一般的认知处理提出了结论。
{"title":"Cognitive processing stages in mental rotation – How can cognitive modelling inform HsMM-EEG models?","authors":"Linda Heimisch ,&nbsp;Kai Preuss ,&nbsp;Nele Russwinkel","doi":"10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2023.108615","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2023.108615","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p><span><span>The aspiration for insight into human cognitive processing has traditionally driven research in </span>cognitive science. With methods such as the Hidden semi-Markov Model-Electroencephalography (HsMM-EEG) method, new approaches have been developed that help to understand the temporal structure of cognition by identifying temporally discrete processing stages. However, it remains challenging to assign concrete functional contributions by specific processing stages to the overall cognitive process. In this paper, we address this challenge by linking HsMM-EEG</span><sup>3</sup> with cognitive modelling, with the aim of further validating the HsMM-EEG<sup>3</sup> method and demonstrating the potential of cognitive models to facilitate functional interpretation of processing stages. For this purpose, we applied HsMM-EEG<sup>3</sup> to data from a mental rotation task and developed an ACT-R cognitive model that is able to closely replicate human performance in this task. Applying HsMM-EEG<sup>3</sup> to the mental rotation experiment data revealed a strong likelihood for 6 distinct stages of cognitive processing during trials, with an additional stage for non-rotated conditions. The cognitive model predicted intra-trial mental activity patterns that project well onto the processing stages, while explaining the additional stage as a marker of non-spatial shortcut use. Thereby, this combined methodology provided substantially more information than either method by itself and suggests conclusions for cognitive processing in general.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":19279,"journal":{"name":"Neuropsychologia","volume":"188 ","pages":"Article 108615"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2023-09-09","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10493482","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Neural basis of sound-symbolic pseudoword-shape correspondences 发音的神经基础符号伪词的形状对应。
IF 2.6 3区 心理学 Q3 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2023-09-09 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2023.108657
Deborah A. Barany , Simon Lacey , Kaitlyn L. Matthews , Lynne C. Nygaard , K. Sathian

Non-arbitrary mapping between the sound of a word and its meaning, termed sound symbolism, is commonly studied through crossmodal correspondences between sounds and visual shapes, e.g., auditory pseudowords, like ‘mohloh’ and ‘kehteh’, are matched to rounded and pointed visual shapes, respectively. Here, we used functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) during a crossmodal matching task to investigate the hypotheses that sound symbolism (1) involves language processing; (2) depends on multisensory integration; (3) reflects embodiment of speech in hand movements. These hypotheses lead to corresponding neuroanatomical predictions of crossmodal congruency effects in (1) the language network; (2) areas mediating multisensory processing, including visual and auditory cortex; (3) regions responsible for sensorimotor control of the hand and mouth. Right-handed participants (n = 22) encountered audiovisual stimuli comprising a simultaneously presented visual shape (rounded or pointed) and an auditory pseudoword (‘mohloh’ or ‘kehteh’) and indicated via a right-hand keypress whether the stimuli matched or not. Reaction times were faster for congruent than incongruent stimuli. Univariate analysis showed that activity was greater for the congruent compared to the incongruent condition in the left primary and association auditory cortex, and left anterior fusiform/parahippocampal gyri. Multivoxel pattern analysis revealed higher classification accuracy for the audiovisual stimuli when congruent than when incongruent, in the pars opercularis of the left inferior frontal (Broca's area), the left supramarginal, and the right mid-occipital gyri. These findings, considered in relation to the neuroanatomical predictions, support the first two hypotheses and suggest that sound symbolism involves both language processing and multisensory integration.

一个单词的声音与其含义之间的非任意映射,称为声音象征,通常通过声音和视觉形状之间的跨模态对应来研究,例如,听觉假名,如“mohloh”和“kehteh”,分别与圆形和尖头视觉形状相匹配。在这里,我们在跨模态匹配任务中使用了功能性磁共振成像(fMRI)来研究声音象征(1)涉及语言处理的假设;(2) 依赖于多感官的整合;(3) 反映了言语在手部动作中的体现。这些假设导致了对(1)语言网络中跨模态一致性效应的相应神经解剖学预测;(2) 介导多感官处理的区域,包括视觉和听觉皮层;(3) 负责手和嘴的感觉运动控制的区域。右手参与者(n=22)遇到视听刺激,包括同时呈现的视觉形状(圆形或尖头)和听觉假名(“hloh”或“kehteh”),并通过右手按键指示刺激是否匹配。一致刺激的反应时间比不一致刺激更快。单变量分析显示,与不一致条件相比,一致条件下左侧初级和关联听觉皮层以及左侧前梭形/海马旁回的活动更大。多体素模式分析显示,在左额下叶(布罗卡区)、左缘上叶和右枕中回的操纵部,一致时的视听刺激分类精度高于不一致时的分类精度。这些发现与神经解剖学预测有关,支持了前两个假设,并表明声音象征主义涉及语言处理和多感官整合。
{"title":"Neural basis of sound-symbolic pseudoword-shape correspondences","authors":"Deborah A. Barany ,&nbsp;Simon Lacey ,&nbsp;Kaitlyn L. Matthews ,&nbsp;Lynne C. Nygaard ,&nbsp;K. Sathian","doi":"10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2023.108657","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2023.108657","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p><span><span>Non-arbitrary mapping between the sound of a word and its meaning, termed sound symbolism, is commonly studied through crossmodal correspondences between sounds and visual shapes, e.g., auditory pseudowords, like ‘mohloh’ and ‘kehteh’, are matched to rounded and pointed visual shapes, respectively. Here, we used functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) during a crossmodal matching task to investigate the hypotheses that sound symbolism (1) involves language processing; (2) depends on multisensory integration; (3) reflects embodiment of speech in hand movements. These hypotheses lead to corresponding neuroanatomical predictions of crossmodal congruency effects in (1) the language network; (2) areas mediating multisensory processing, including visual and </span>auditory cortex<span>; (3) regions responsible for sensorimotor control of the hand and mouth. Right-handed participants (</span></span><em>n</em><span><span> = 22) encountered audiovisual stimuli comprising a simultaneously presented visual shape (rounded or pointed) and an auditory pseudoword (‘mohloh’ or ‘kehteh’) and indicated via a right-hand keypress whether the stimuli matched or not. Reaction times were faster for congruent than incongruent stimuli. Univariate analysis showed that activity was greater for the congruent compared to the incongruent condition in the left primary and association auditory cortex, and left anterior fusiform/parahippocampal </span>gyri. Multivoxel pattern analysis revealed higher classification accuracy for the audiovisual stimuli when congruent than when incongruent, in the pars opercularis of the left inferior frontal (Broca's area), the left supramarginal, and the right mid-occipital gyri. These findings, considered in relation to the neuroanatomical predictions, support the first two hypotheses and suggest that sound symbolism involves both language processing and multisensory integration.</span></p></div>","PeriodicalId":19279,"journal":{"name":"Neuropsychologia","volume":"188 ","pages":"Article 108657"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2023-09-09","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10529692/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10130293","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Openness to experience and cognitive functioning and decline in older adults: The mediating role of cognitive reserve 老年人对经验和认知功能的开放性与衰退:认知储备的中介作用
IF 2.6 3区 心理学 Q3 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2023-09-09 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2023.108655
Teresa Montoliu , Mariola Zapater-Fajarí , Vanesa Hidalgo , Alicia Salvador

Objective

Openness to experience has been consistently associated with better cognitive functioning in older people, but its association with cognitive decline is less clear. Cognitive reserve has been proposed as a mechanism underlying this relationship, but previous studies have reported mixed findings, possibly due to the different ways of conceptualizing cognitive reserve. We aimed to analyze the potential mediating role of cognitive reserve in the association between openness and cognitive functioning and decline in healthy older people.

Method

In Wave 1 and at the four-year follow-up (Wave 2), 87 healthy older people (49.4% women; M age = 65.08, SD = 4.54) completed a neuropsychological battery to assess cognitive functioning and a questionnaire to assess cognitive reserve. Openness was measured with the NEO- Five-Factor Inventory. Mediation models were proposed to investigate the relationship between openness and cognitive function or decline through cognitive reserve or its change.

Results

Cognitive reserve mediated the openness-cognitive functioning association. Thus, individuals with higher openness showed greater cognitive reserve, and this greater cognitive reserve was associated with better cognitive functioning. Moreover, greater cognitive reserve at baseline also mediated the association between higher openness and slower cognitive decline. However, change in cognitive reserve did not mediate the association between openness and change in cognitive functioning.

Conclusions

Cognitive reserve is a mechanism underlying the association between openness and cognitive functioning and decline. These findings support the differential preservation hypothesis, suggesting that healthy older adults who engage in more cognitively stimulating activities would show less age-related cognitive decline.

客观经验的开放性一直与老年人更好的认知功能有关,但其与认知能力下降的关系尚不清楚。认知储备被认为是这种关系的一种机制,但之前的研究报告了喜忧参半的结果,可能是由于认知储备的概念化方式不同。我们旨在分析认知储备在健康老年人的开放性、认知功能和衰退之间的潜在中介作用。方法在第1波和四年随访(第2波)中,87名健康老年人(49.4%为女性;M年龄=65.08,SD=4.54)完成了评估认知功能的神经心理测验和评估认知储备的问卷。开放性是用近地天体五因素清单来衡量的。提出了中介模型,通过认知储备或其变化来研究开放性与认知功能或衰退之间的关系。结果认知储备介导开放性认知功能联想。因此,具有更高开放性的个体表现出更大的认知储备,这种更大的感知储备与更好的认知功能有关。此外,基线时更大的认知储备也介导了更高的开放性和更慢的认知下降之间的联系。然而,认知储备的变化并不能调节开放性和认知功能变化之间的联系。结论认知储备是开放性与认知功能和衰退之间联系的机制。这些发现支持差异保存假说,表明参与更多认知刺激活动的健康老年人与年龄相关的认知能力下降较少。
{"title":"Openness to experience and cognitive functioning and decline in older adults: The mediating role of cognitive reserve","authors":"Teresa Montoliu ,&nbsp;Mariola Zapater-Fajarí ,&nbsp;Vanesa Hidalgo ,&nbsp;Alicia Salvador","doi":"10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2023.108655","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2023.108655","url":null,"abstract":"<div><h3>Objective</h3><p>Openness to experience has been consistently associated with better cognitive functioning in older people, but its association with cognitive decline is less clear. Cognitive reserve has been proposed as a mechanism underlying this relationship, but previous studies have reported mixed findings, possibly due to the different ways of conceptualizing cognitive reserve. We aimed to analyze the potential mediating role of cognitive reserve in the association between openness and cognitive functioning and decline in healthy older people.</p></div><div><h3>Method</h3><p>In Wave 1 and at the four-year follow-up (Wave 2), 87 healthy older people (49.4% women; M age = 65.08, SD = 4.54) completed a neuropsychological battery to assess cognitive functioning and a questionnaire to assess cognitive reserve. Openness was measured with the NEO- Five-Factor Inventory. Mediation models were proposed to investigate the relationship between openness and cognitive function or decline through cognitive reserve or its change.</p></div><div><h3>Results</h3><p>Cognitive reserve mediated the openness-cognitive functioning association. Thus, individuals with higher openness showed greater cognitive reserve, and this greater cognitive reserve was associated with better cognitive functioning. Moreover, greater cognitive reserve at baseline also mediated the association between higher openness and slower cognitive decline. However, change in cognitive reserve did not mediate the association between openness and change in cognitive functioning.</p></div><div><h3>Conclusions</h3><p>Cognitive reserve is a mechanism underlying the association between openness and cognitive functioning and decline. These findings support the differential preservation hypothesis, suggesting that healthy older adults who engage in more cognitively stimulating activities would show less age-related cognitive decline.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":19279,"journal":{"name":"Neuropsychologia","volume":"188 ","pages":"Article 108655"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2023-09-09","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10140272","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Processing objects of perceptual expertise: Differential interhemispheric transmission efficiency but similar transmission direction advantages 感知专业知识的处理对象:半球间传输效率不同但传输方向相似的优势
IF 2.6 3区 心理学 Q3 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2023-09-09 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2023.108568
Simin Wan , Yini Sun , Qing Ye , Yu Gu , Werner Sommer , Xiaohua Cao

Faces and Chinese characters are both objects of perceptual expertise. In this study, we investigated the characteristics of interhemispheric transmission times (IHTTs) in both transmission direction and transmission efficiency during the processing of objects of perceptual expertise. A total of 112 participants engaged in a divided visual field paradigm for faces, Chinese characters, and houses in both upright and inverted orientations. The N170 amplitudes elicited by the objects of perceptual expertise (faces and Chinese characters) involved in this study were larger than those elicited by the non-perceptual expertise objects (houses). We used the latencies of the N170 component of the event-related potential (ERP) recorded in the left and right hemispheres to calculate the IHTTs. For all objects, the N170-related IHTTs from the right to the left hemispheres were shorter than those in the opposite direction. Essentially, the N170-related IHTTs for faces were shorter, that is, more efficient than those for Chinese characters and houses. This result indicates that the IHTTs during perceptual expertise and non-perceptual expertise object processing share a common transmission direction advantage, but transmission efficiency is face-specific.

面孔和汉字都是感性专门知识的对象。在本研究中,我们研究了在处理感知专业知识对象过程中,半球间传输时间(IHTT)在传输方向和传输效率方面的特征。共有112名参与者参与了一个垂直和倒置方向的人脸、汉字和房屋的分视野范式。本研究涉及的感知专业对象(人脸和汉字)引发的N170振幅大于非感知专业对象的N170波幅。我们使用左右半球记录的事件相关电位(ERP)N170成分的潜伏期来计算IHTT。对于所有物体,从右到左半球的N170相关IHTT都比相反方向的短。从本质上讲,与N170相关的人脸IHTT更短,也就是说,比汉字和房屋的IHTT更有效。这一结果表明,在感知专业知识和非感知专业知识对象处理过程中,IHTT具有共同的传输方向优势,但传输效率是面向特定人脸的。
{"title":"Processing objects of perceptual expertise: Differential interhemispheric transmission efficiency but similar transmission direction advantages","authors":"Simin Wan ,&nbsp;Yini Sun ,&nbsp;Qing Ye ,&nbsp;Yu Gu ,&nbsp;Werner Sommer ,&nbsp;Xiaohua Cao","doi":"10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2023.108568","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2023.108568","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Faces and Chinese characters are both objects of perceptual expertise. In this study, we investigated the characteristics of interhemispheric transmission times (IHTTs) in both transmission direction and transmission efficiency during the processing of objects of perceptual expertise. A total of 112 participants engaged in a divided visual field paradigm for faces, Chinese characters, and houses in both upright and inverted orientations. The N170 amplitudes elicited by the objects of perceptual expertise (faces and Chinese characters) involved in this study were larger than those elicited by the non-perceptual expertise objects (houses). We used the latencies of the N170 component of the event-related potential (ERP) recorded in the left and right hemispheres to calculate the IHTTs. For all objects, the N170-related IHTTs from the right to the left hemispheres were shorter than those in the opposite direction. Essentially, the N170-related IHTTs for faces were shorter, that is, more efficient than those for Chinese characters and houses. This result indicates that the IHTTs during perceptual expertise and non-perceptual expertise object processing share a common transmission direction advantage, but transmission efficiency is face-specific.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":19279,"journal":{"name":"Neuropsychologia","volume":"188 ","pages":"Article 108568"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2023-09-09","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10136819","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Long-term phonemic representations become audiovisual by mid-childhood 长期的音位表征在儿童中期就变成了视听的。
IF 2.6 3区 心理学 Q3 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2023-09-09 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2023.108633
Natalya Kaganovich , Jennifer Schumaker , Sharon Christ

In earlier work with adults, we showed that long-term phonemic representations are audiovisual, meaning that they contain information on typical mouth shape during articulation. Many aspects of audiovisual processing have a prolonged developmental course, often not reaching maturity until late adolescence. In this study, we examined the status of phonemic representations in two groups of children – 8-9-year-olds and 11-12-year-olds. We used the same audiovisual oddball paradigm as in the earlier study with adults (Kaganovich and Christ, 2021). On each trial, participants saw a face and heard one of two vowels. One vowel occurred frequently (standard), while another occurred rarely (deviant). In one condition (neutral), the face had a closed, non-articulating mouth. In the other condition (audiovisual violation), the mouth shape matched the frequent vowel. Although stimuli were audiovisual in both conditions, we hypothesized that identical auditory changes would be perceived differently by participants. Namely, in the neutral condition, deviants violated only the audiovisual pattern specific to each experimental block. By contrast, in the audiovisual violation condition, deviants additionally violated long-term representations for how a speaker's mouth looks during articulation. We compared the amplitude of MMN and P3 components elicited by deviants in the two conditions. In the 11-12-year-old group, the pattern of neural responses was similar to that in adults – namely, they had a larger MMN component in the audiovisual compared to neutral condition, with no major difference in the P3 amplitude. In contrast, in the 8-9-year-old group, we saw a posterior MMN in the neutral condition only and a larger P3 in the audiovisual violation compared to the neutral condition. The larger P3 in the audiovisual violation condition suggests that younger children did perceive deviants as being more attention-grabbing when they violated the typical combination of sound and mouth shape. Yet, at this age, the earlier, more automatic stages of phonemic processing indexed by the MMN component may not yet encode visual speech elements the same way they do in older children and adults. We conclude that phonemic representations do not become audiovisual until 11–12 years of age.

在早期对成年人的研究中,我们发现长期的音位表征是视听的,这意味着它们包含了发音过程中典型口型的信息。视听加工的许多方面都有一个漫长的发展过程,通常要到青春期晚期才能成熟。在这项研究中,我们检查了两组儿童(8-9岁和11-12岁)的音位表征状况。我们使用了与早期成人研究相同的视听怪人范式(Kaganovich和Christ,2021)。在每次试验中,参与者看到一张脸,听到两个元音中的一个。一个元音经常出现(标准),而另一个元音很少出现(异常)。在一种情况下(中性),面部有一个闭合的、没有关节的嘴巴。在另一种情况下(视听违规),口型与频繁元音匹配。尽管在这两种情况下刺激都是视听的,但我们假设相同的听觉变化会被参与者感知到不同。也就是说,在中性条件下,偏离者只违反了每个实验块特有的视听模式。相比之下,在视听侵犯条件下,越轨者还侵犯了说话者在发音过程中口腔外观的长期表征。我们比较了两种条件下偏差引起的MMN和P3分量的振幅。在11-12岁的组中,神经反应的模式与成年人相似,即与中性条件相比,他们在视听条件下的MMN成分更大,P3振幅没有重大差异。相反,在8-9岁的组中,与中性条件相比,我们只在中性条件下看到了后部MMN,在视听侵犯中看到了更大的P3。视听侵犯条件下较大的P3表明,年龄较小的儿童在侵犯声音和口型的典型组合时,确实认为越轨者更能吸引注意力。然而,在这个年龄段,由MMN成分索引的更早、更自动的音素处理阶段可能还没有像在年龄较大的儿童和成年人中那样对视觉语音元素进行编码。我们得出的结论是,音位表征直到11-12岁才变成视听的。
{"title":"Long-term phonemic representations become audiovisual by mid-childhood","authors":"Natalya Kaganovich ,&nbsp;Jennifer Schumaker ,&nbsp;Sharon Christ","doi":"10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2023.108633","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2023.108633","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>In earlier work with adults, we showed that long-term phonemic representations are audiovisual, meaning that they contain information on typical mouth shape during articulation. Many aspects of audiovisual processing have a prolonged developmental course, often not reaching maturity until late adolescence. In this study, we examined the status of phonemic representations in two groups of children – 8-9-year-olds and 11-12-year-olds. We used the same audiovisual oddball paradigm<span> as in the earlier study with adults (Kaganovich and Christ, 2021). On each trial, participants saw a face and heard one of two vowels. One vowel occurred frequently (standard), while another occurred rarely (deviant). In one condition (neutral), the face had a closed, non-articulating mouth. In the other condition (audiovisual violation), the mouth shape matched the frequent vowel. Although stimuli were audiovisual in both conditions, we hypothesized that identical auditory changes would be perceived differently by participants. Namely, in the neutral condition, deviants violated only the audiovisual pattern specific to each experimental block. By contrast, in the audiovisual violation condition, deviants additionally violated long-term representations for how a speaker's mouth looks during articulation. We compared the amplitude of MMN and P3 components elicited by deviants in the two conditions. In the 11-12-year-old group, the pattern of neural responses was similar to that in adults – namely, they had a larger MMN component in the audiovisual compared to neutral condition, with no major difference in the P3 amplitude. In contrast, in the 8-9-year-old group, we saw a posterior MMN in the neutral condition only and a larger P3 in the audiovisual violation compared to the neutral condition. The larger P3 in the audiovisual violation condition suggests that younger children did perceive deviants as being more attention-grabbing when they violated the typical combination of sound and mouth shape. Yet, at this age, the earlier, more automatic stages of phonemic processing indexed by the MMN component may not yet encode visual speech elements the same way they do in older children and adults. We conclude that phonemic representations do not become audiovisual until 11–12 years of age.</span></p></div>","PeriodicalId":19279,"journal":{"name":"Neuropsychologia","volume":"188 ","pages":"Article 108633"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2023-09-09","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10530328/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10140250","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Narrative simulation of social experiences in naturalistic context – A neurocinematic approach 自然主义背景下社会体验的叙事模拟——一种神经网络方法
IF 2.6 3区 心理学 Q3 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2023-09-09 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2023.108654
Pia Tikka , Mauri Kaipainen , Juha Salmi

Narratives may be regarded as simulations of everyday social situations. They are key to studying the human mind in socio-culturally determined contexts as they allow anchoring to the common ground of embodied and environmentally-engaged cognition. Here we review recent findings from naturalistic neuroscience on neural functions in conditions that mimic lifelike situations. We will focus particularly on neurocinematics, a research field that applies mediated narratives as stimuli for neuroimaging experiments. During the last two decades, this paradigm has contributed to an accumulation of insights about the neural underpinnings of behavior and sense-making in various narratively contextualized situations particularly pertaining to socio-emotional encounters. One of the key questions in neurocinematics is, how do intersubjectively synchronized brain activations relate to subjective experiences? Another question we address is how to bring natural contexts into experimental studies. Seeking to respond to both questions, we suggest neurocinematic studies to examine three manifestations of the same phenomenon side-by-side: subjective experiences of narrative situations, unfolding of narrative stimulus structure, and neural processes that co-constitute the experience. This approach facilitates identifying experientially meaningful activity patterns in the brain and points out what they may mean in relation to shared and communicable contents. Via rich-featured and temporally contextualized narrative stimuli, neurocinematics attempts to contribute to emerging holistic theories of neural dynamics and connectomics explaining typical and atypical interindividual variability.

叙事可以被看作是对日常社会状况的模拟。它们是在社会文化决定的背景下研究人类思维的关键,因为它们可以锚定具体化和环境参与认知的共同点。在这里,我们回顾了自然主义神经科学在模拟逼真情况下的神经功能的最新发现。我们将特别关注神经运动学,这是一个应用中介叙事作为神经成像实验刺激的研究领域。在过去的二十年里,这种范式有助于积累对各种叙事情境下行为和意义形成的神经基础的见解,特别是与社会情感遭遇有关的情况。神经学家的一个关键问题是,主体间同步的大脑激活如何与主观体验相关?我们要解决的另一个问题是如何将自然环境纳入实验研究。为了回答这两个问题,我们建议进行神经系统学研究,同时检查同一现象的三种表现:叙事情境的主观体验、叙事刺激结构的展开以及共同构成体验的神经过程。这种方法有助于识别大脑中有经验意义的活动模式,并指出它们在共享和可传播内容方面可能意味着什么。通过丰富的特征和时间语境化的叙事刺激,神经运动学试图为新兴的神经动力学和连接组学的整体理论做出贡献,解释典型和非典型的个体间变异性。
{"title":"Narrative simulation of social experiences in naturalistic context – A neurocinematic approach","authors":"Pia Tikka ,&nbsp;Mauri Kaipainen ,&nbsp;Juha Salmi","doi":"10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2023.108654","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2023.108654","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Narratives may be regarded as simulations of everyday social situations. They are key to studying the human mind in socio-culturally determined contexts as they allow anchoring to the common ground of embodied and environmentally-engaged cognition. Here we review recent findings from naturalistic neuroscience on neural functions in conditions that mimic lifelike situations. We will focus particularly on neurocinematics, a research field that applies mediated narratives as stimuli for neuroimaging experiments. During the last two decades, this paradigm has contributed to an accumulation of insights about the neural underpinnings of behavior and sense-making in various narratively contextualized situations particularly pertaining to socio-emotional encounters. One of the key questions in neurocinematics is, how do intersubjectively synchronized brain activations relate to subjective experiences? Another question we address is how to bring natural contexts into experimental studies. Seeking to respond to both questions, we suggest neurocinematic studies to examine three manifestations of the same phenomenon side-by-side: subjective experiences of narrative situations, unfolding of narrative stimulus structure, and neural processes that co-constitute the experience. This approach facilitates identifying experientially meaningful activity patterns in the brain and points out what they may mean in relation to shared and communicable contents. Via rich-featured and temporally contextualized narrative stimuli, neurocinematics attempts to contribute to emerging holistic theories of neural dynamics and connectomics explaining typical and atypical interindividual variability.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":19279,"journal":{"name":"Neuropsychologia","volume":"188 ","pages":"Article 108654"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2023-09-09","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10140274","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 3
An fMRI study of inflectional encoding in spoken word production: Role of domain-general inhibition 屈折编码在口语产生中的功能磁共振成像研究:域一般抑制的作用
IF 2.6 3区 心理学 Q3 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2023-09-09 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2023.108653
João Ferreira , Ardi Roelofs , Guilherme Blazquez Freches , Vitória Piai

A major issue concerning inflectional encoding in spoken word production is whether or not regular forms (e.g., past tense walked) are encoded by rule application and irregular forms (e.g., swam) by retrieval from associative memory and inhibition of the regular rule. We used functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) to examine the involvement of domain-general inhibition, thought to be underpinned by right inferior frontal gyrus (IFG), right pre-supplementary motor area (SMA), and right basal ganglia. Participants were presented with infinitive verbs that take either regular or irregular past tense. They switched between producing the past tense of these regular and irregular verbs in one block, and between inflecting or reading these infinitive verbs aloud in another block. As concerns corticobasal areas, compared to reading, inflecting activated left IFG and left preSMA/SMA. Regulars yielded higher activation than irregulars in these frontal areas, both on switch and repeat trials, which did not differ in activation. Switching between inflecting and reading activated left preSMA/SMA. These results indicate that inflectional encoding, and switching between inflecting and reading, engage frontal areas in the left hemisphere, including left preSMA/SMA for both and left IFG for inflecting, without recruiting the domain-general inhibition circuitry in the right hemisphere. We advance an account of inflectional encoding in spoken word production that assumes a distinction between regulars and irregulars, but without engaging domain-general inhibition.

口语产生中的屈折编码的一个主要问题是规则形式(如过去时wald)是否通过规则应用进行编码,不规则形式(例如swim)是否通过从联想记忆中检索和抑制规则进行编码。我们使用功能性磁共振成像(fMRI)来检查域普遍抑制的参与,该抑制被认为是由右额下回(IFG)、右补充运动前区(SMA)和右基底神经节支持的。向参与者展示了采用规则或不规则过去时的不定式动词。他们在一个块中产生这些规则动词和不规则动词的过去时之间切换,在另一块中屈折或大声朗读这些不定式动词之间切换。关于皮质基底区,与阅读相比,屈折激活了左侧IFG和左侧前SMA/SMA。在这些额叶区域,无论是在转换试验还是重复试验中,规则区的激活率都高于非规则区,这在激活率上没有差异。在屈折和阅读之间切换激活了左前SMA/SMA。这些结果表明,屈折编码以及屈折和阅读之间的切换涉及左半球的额叶区域,包括两者的左前SMA/SMA和屈折的左IFG,而没有募集右半球的域通用抑制电路。我们提出了一种对口语单词产生中屈折编码的描述,该描述假设了正则和非正则之间的区别,但没有涉及领域一般抑制。
{"title":"An fMRI study of inflectional encoding in spoken word production: Role of domain-general inhibition","authors":"João Ferreira ,&nbsp;Ardi Roelofs ,&nbsp;Guilherme Blazquez Freches ,&nbsp;Vitória Piai","doi":"10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2023.108653","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2023.108653","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>A major issue concerning inflectional encoding in spoken word production is whether or not regular forms (e.g., past tense <em>walked</em>) are encoded by rule application and irregular forms (e.g., <em>swam</em><span>) by retrieval from associative memory<span> and inhibition of the regular rule. We used functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) to examine the involvement of domain-general inhibition, thought to be underpinned by right inferior frontal gyrus (IFG), right pre-supplementary motor area (SMA), and right basal ganglia. Participants were presented with infinitive verbs that take either regular or irregular past tense. They switched between producing the past tense of these regular and irregular verbs in one block, and between inflecting or reading these infinitive verbs aloud in another block. As concerns corticobasal areas, compared to reading, inflecting activated left IFG and left preSMA/SMA. Regulars yielded higher activation than irregulars in these frontal areas, both on switch and repeat trials, which did not differ in activation. Switching between inflecting and reading activated left preSMA/SMA. These results indicate that inflectional encoding, and switching between inflecting and reading, engage frontal areas in the left hemisphere, including left preSMA/SMA for both and left IFG for inflecting, without recruiting the domain-general inhibition circuitry in the right hemisphere. We advance an account of inflectional encoding in spoken word production that assumes a distinction between regulars and irregulars, but without engaging domain-general inhibition.</span></span></p></div>","PeriodicalId":19279,"journal":{"name":"Neuropsychologia","volume":"188 ","pages":"Article 108653"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2023-09-09","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10493977","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Hemispheric contributions toward interoception and emotion recognition in left-vs right-semantic dementia 左右语义痴呆患者半球对互觉和情绪识别的贡献
IF 2.6 3区 心理学 Q3 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2023-09-09 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2023.108628
Jessica L. Hazelton , Emma Devenney , Rebekah Ahmed , James Burrell , Yun Hwang , Olivier Piguet , Fiona Kumfor

Background

The hemispheric contributions toward interoception, the perception of internal bodily cues, and emotion recognition remains unclear. Semantic dementia cases with either left-dominant (i.e., left-SD) or right-dominant (i.e., right-SD) anterior temporal lobe atrophy experience emotion recognition difficulties, however, little is known about interoception in these syndromes. Here, we hypothesised that right-SD would show worse interoception and emotion recognition due to right-dominant atrophy.

Methods

Thirty-five participants (8 left-SD; 6 right-SD; 21 controls) completed a monitoring task. Participants pressed a button when they: (1) felt their heartbeat, without pulse measurement (Interoception); or (2) heard a recorded heartbeat (Exteroception-control). Simultaneous ECG was recorded. Accuracy was calculated by comparing the event frequency (i.e., heartbeat or sound) to response frequency. Emotion recognition was assessed via the Facial Affect Selection Task. Voxel-based morphometry analyses identified neural correlates of interoception and emotion recognition.

Results

Right-SD showed worse interoception than controls and left-SD (both p's < 0.001). Both patient groups showed worse emotion recognition than controls (right-SD: p < .001; left-SD: p = .018), and right-SD showed worse emotion recognition than left-SD (p = .003). Regression analyses revealed that worse emotion recognition was predicted by right-SD (p = .002), left-SD (p = .005), and impaired interoception (p = .004). Interoception and emotion were associated with the integrity of right-lateralised structures including the insula, temporal pole, thalamus, superior temporal gyrus, and hippocampus.

Conclusion

Our study provides the first evidence for impaired interoception in right-SD, suggesting that impaired emotion recognition in this syndrome is driven by inaccurate internal monitoring. Further we identified a common neurobiological basis for interoception and emotion in the right hemisphere.

背景半球对内感受、对身体内部线索的感知和情绪识别的贡献尚不清楚。患有左显性(即左SD)或右显性(即右SD)前颞叶萎缩的语义性痴呆病例会出现情绪识别困难,然而,对这些综合征中的内感受知之甚少。在这里,我们假设由于右显性萎缩,右SD会表现出更差的内感受和情绪识别。方法35名受试者(左侧SD 8名,右侧SD 6名,对照组21名)完成监测任务。参与者按下按钮时:(1)感觉到自己的心跳,没有脉搏测量(Interception);或(2)听到记录的心跳(外部感受控制)。同时记录心电图。通过将事件频率(即心跳或声音)与响应频率进行比较来计算准确性。情绪识别通过面部情感选择任务进行评估。基于体素的形态计量学分析确定了交互感受和情绪识别的神经相关性。结果右SD表现出比对照组和左SD更差的互感(均p<0.001)。两组患者的情绪识别能力都比对照组差(右SD:p<0.001;左SD:p=.018),右SD表现得比左SD差(p=.003)。回归分析显示,右SD预测情绪识别能力较差(p=0.002),左侧SD(p=0.005)和感觉间受损(p=0.004)。感觉间和情绪与右侧结构的完整性有关,包括岛叶、颞极、丘脑、颞上回和海马。结论我们的研究为右SD患者的内感受受损提供了第一个证据,表明该综合征的情绪识别受损是由不准确的内部监测引起的。此外,我们确定了右半球内感受和情绪的共同神经生物学基础。
{"title":"Hemispheric contributions toward interoception and emotion recognition in left-vs right-semantic dementia","authors":"Jessica L. Hazelton ,&nbsp;Emma Devenney ,&nbsp;Rebekah Ahmed ,&nbsp;James Burrell ,&nbsp;Yun Hwang ,&nbsp;Olivier Piguet ,&nbsp;Fiona Kumfor","doi":"10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2023.108628","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2023.108628","url":null,"abstract":"<div><h3>Background</h3><p>The hemispheric contributions toward interoception, the perception of internal bodily cues, and emotion recognition remains unclear. Semantic dementia cases with either left-dominant (i.e., left-SD) or right-dominant (i.e., right-SD) anterior temporal lobe atrophy experience emotion recognition difficulties, however, little is known about interoception in these syndromes. Here, we hypothesised that right-SD would show worse interoception and emotion recognition due to right-dominant atrophy.</p></div><div><h3>Methods</h3><p>Thirty-five participants (8 left-SD; 6 right-SD; 21 controls) completed a monitoring task. Participants pressed a button when they: (1) felt their heartbeat, without pulse measurement (Interoception); or (2) heard a recorded heartbeat (Exteroception-control). Simultaneous ECG was recorded. Accuracy was calculated by comparing the event frequency (i.e., heartbeat or sound) to response frequency. Emotion recognition was assessed via the Facial Affect Selection Task. Voxel-based morphometry analyses identified neural correlates of interoception and emotion recognition.</p></div><div><h3>Results</h3><p>Right-SD showed worse interoception than controls and left-SD (both <em>p's</em> &lt; 0.001). Both patient groups showed worse emotion recognition than controls (right-SD: <em>p</em> &lt; .001; left-SD: <em>p</em> = .018), and right-SD showed worse emotion recognition than left-SD (<em>p</em> = .003). Regression analyses revealed that worse emotion recognition was predicted by right-SD (<em>p</em> = .002), left-SD (<em>p</em> = .005), and impaired interoception (<em>p</em> = .004). Interoception and emotion were associated with the integrity of right-lateralised structures including the insula, temporal pole, thalamus, superior temporal gyrus, and hippocampus.</p></div><div><h3>Conclusion</h3><p>Our study provides the first evidence for impaired interoception in right-SD, suggesting that impaired emotion recognition in this syndrome is driven by inaccurate internal monitoring. Further we identified a common neurobiological basis for interoception and emotion in the right hemisphere.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":19279,"journal":{"name":"Neuropsychologia","volume":"188 ","pages":"Article 108628"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2023-09-09","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10130241","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
期刊
Neuropsychologia
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1