首页 > 最新文献

Neuropsychologia最新文献

英文 中文
Revisiting which language declines more in Spanish-English bilinguals with Alzheimer's disease: Longitudinal decline patterns on the multilingual naming test 重新审视患有阿尔茨海默病的西英双语患者哪种语言衰退得更严重:多语言命名测试的纵向下降模式。
IF 2 3区 心理学 Q3 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-07-04 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2024.108948

Theories of bilingual language production predict that bilinguals with Alzheimer's disease (AD) should exhibit one of two decline patterns. Either parallel decline of both languages (if decline reflects damage to semantic representations that are accessed by both languages), or asymmetrical decline, with greater decline of the nondominant language (if decline reflects reduced ability to resolve competition from the dominant language with disease progression). Only two previous studies examined decline longitudinally with one showing parallel, and the other asymmetrical, decline. We examined decline over 2–7 years (3.9 on average) in Spanish-English bilinguals (N = 23). Logistic regression revealed a parallel decline pattern at one year from baseline, but an asymmetrical decline pattern over the longer decline period, with greater decline of the nondominant language (when calculating predicted probabilities of a correct response). The asymmetrical decline pattern was significantly greater for the nondominant language only when including item-difficulty in the model. Exploratory analyses across dominance groups looking at proportional decline relative to initial naming accuracy further suggested that decline of the nondominant language may be more precipitous if that language was acquired later in life, but the critical interaction needed to support this possibility was not statistically significant in a logistic regression analysis. These results suggest that accessibility of the nondominant language may initially be more resilient in early versus more advanced AD, and that AD affects shared semantic representations before executive control declines to a point where the ability to name pictures in single-language testing block is disrupted. Additional work is needed to determine if asymmetrical decline patterns are magnified by late age of acquisition of the nondominant language, and if more subtle impairments to executive control underlie impairments to language switching that occur in the earliest stages of AD (even preclinically).

双语语言生成理论预测,患有阿尔茨海默病(AD)的双语患者应表现出两种衰退模式之一。一种是两种语言并行衰退(如果衰退反映了两种语言都能使用的语义表征受损),另一种是不对称衰退,非优势语言衰退程度更大(如果衰退反映了随着病情发展,解决优势语言竞争的能力下降)。此前只有两项研究对语言能力的衰退进行了纵向研究,一项研究显示了平行衰退,另一项研究则显示了非对称衰退。我们对西班牙语和英语双语者(23 人)2-7 年(平均 3.9 年)的语言能力下降情况进行了研究。逻辑回归结果表明,从基线开始一年的下降模式是平行的,但在更长的下降期内,下降模式是不对称的,非主导语言的下降幅度更大(在计算预测的正确反应概率时)。只有在将项目难度纳入模型时,非优势语言的非对称下降模式才会明显加剧。根据与初始命名准确率成比例的下降情况对各优势组进行的探索性分析进一步表明,如果非优势语言是在晚年才获得的,那么该语言的下降可能会更快,但在逻辑回归分析中,支持这种可能性所需的关键交互作用在统计上并不显著。这些结果表明,与晚期注意力缺失症相比,早期注意力缺失症患者非优势语言的可及性最初可能更有弹性,而且注意力缺失症会影响共有语义表征,然后执行控制能力才会下降,以至于在单一语言测试区块中命名图片的能力受到破坏。我们还需要做更多的工作,以确定非对称下降模式是否会因掌握非优势语言的年龄较晚而放大,以及执行控制能力是否会出现更微妙的损伤,从而导致 AD 早期阶段(甚至是临床前期)出现的语言转换障碍。
{"title":"Revisiting which language declines more in Spanish-English bilinguals with Alzheimer's disease: Longitudinal decline patterns on the multilingual naming test","authors":"","doi":"10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2024.108948","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2024.108948","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Theories of bilingual language production predict that bilinguals with Alzheimer's disease (AD) should exhibit one of two decline patterns. Either parallel decline of both languages (if decline reflects damage to semantic representations that are accessed by both languages), or asymmetrical decline, with greater decline of the nondominant language (if decline reflects reduced ability to resolve competition from the dominant language with disease progression). Only two previous studies examined decline longitudinally with one showing parallel, and the other asymmetrical, decline. We examined decline over 2–7 years (3.9 on average) in Spanish-English bilinguals (<em>N</em> = 23). Logistic regression revealed a parallel decline pattern at one year from baseline, but an asymmetrical decline pattern over the longer decline period, with greater decline of the nondominant language (when calculating predicted probabilities of a correct response). The asymmetrical decline pattern was significantly greater for the nondominant language only when including item-difficulty in the model. Exploratory analyses across dominance groups looking at proportional decline relative to initial naming accuracy further suggested that decline of the nondominant language may be more precipitous if that language was acquired later in life, but the critical interaction needed to support this possibility was not statistically significant in a logistic regression analysis. These results suggest that accessibility of the nondominant language may initially be more resilient in early versus more advanced AD, and that AD affects shared semantic representations before executive control declines to a point where the ability to name pictures in single-language testing block is disrupted. Additional work is needed to determine if asymmetrical decline patterns are magnified by late age of acquisition of the nondominant language, and if more subtle impairments to executive control underlie impairments to language switching that occur in the earliest stages of AD (even preclinically).</p></div>","PeriodicalId":19279,"journal":{"name":"Neuropsychologia","volume":"202 ","pages":"Article 108948"},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2024-07-04","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0028393224001635/pdfft?md5=8c567ff2c8b57fe72f541a7733692105&pid=1-s2.0-S0028393224001635-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141545106","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Neural bases of reading fluency: A systematic review and meta-analysis 阅读流畅性的神经基础:系统回顾和荟萃分析。
IF 2 3区 心理学 Q3 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-07-02 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2024.108947
Marissa M. Lee , Catherine J. Stoodley

Reading fluency, the ability to read quickly and accurately, is a critical marker of successful reading and is notoriously difficult to improve in reading disabled populations. Despite its importance to functional literacy, fluency is a relatively under-studied aspect of reading, and the neural correlates of reading fluency are not well understood. Here, we review the literature of the neural correlates of reading fluency as well as rapid automatized naming (RAN), a task that is robustly related to reading fluency. In a qualitative review of the neuroimaging literature, we evaluated structural and functional MRI studies of reading fluency in readers from a range of skill levels. This was followed by a quantitative activation likelihood estimate (ALE) meta-analysis of fMRI studies of reading speed and RAN measures. We anticipated that reading speed, relative to untimed reading and reading-related tasks, would harness ventral reading pathways that are thought to enable the fast, visual recognition of words. The qualitative review showed that speeded reading taps the entire canonical reading network. The meta-analysis indicated a stronger role of the ventral reading pathway in rapid reading and rapid naming. Both reviews identified regions outside the canonical reading network that contribute to reading fluency, such as the bilateral insula and superior parietal lobule. We suggest that fluent reading engages both domain-specific reading pathways as well as domain-general regions that support overall task performance and discuss future avenues of research to expand our understanding of the neural bases of fluent reading.

阅读流畅性,即快速、准确阅读的能力,是成功阅读的重要标志,但在阅读障碍人群中却很难提高。尽管阅读流畅性对功能性读写能力非常重要,但对阅读流畅性的研究却相对不足,人们对阅读流畅性的神经相关因素也不甚了解。在此,我们回顾了有关阅读流畅性以及快速自动命名(RAN)的神经相关性的文献,快速自动命名是一项与阅读流畅性密切相关的任务。在对神经成像文献的定性回顾中,我们评估了对不同技能水平读者的阅读流畅性进行的结构性和功能性 MRI 研究。随后,我们对阅读速度和 RAN 测量的 fMRI 研究进行了定量激活似然估计 (ALE) 元分析。我们预计,相对于无时间限制的阅读和与阅读相关的任务而言,阅读速度将利用腹侧阅读通路,而这种通路被认为能够快速、直观地识别单词。定性研究表明,速读可以利用整个典型阅读网络。荟萃分析表明,腹侧阅读通路在快速阅读和快速命名中的作用更大。两篇综述都发现了典型阅读网络以外的区域对流畅阅读的贡献,如双侧岛叶和顶叶上部。我们认为,流畅阅读既涉及特定领域的阅读通路,也涉及支持整体任务表现的一般领域区域,并讨论了未来的研究途径,以拓展我们对流畅阅读神经基础的理解。
{"title":"Neural bases of reading fluency: A systematic review and meta-analysis","authors":"Marissa M. Lee ,&nbsp;Catherine J. Stoodley","doi":"10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2024.108947","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2024.108947","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Reading fluency, the ability to read quickly and accurately, is a critical marker of successful reading and is notoriously difficult to improve in reading disabled populations. Despite its importance to functional literacy, fluency is a relatively under-studied aspect of reading, and the neural correlates of reading fluency are not well understood. Here, we review the literature of the neural correlates of reading fluency as well as rapid automatized naming (RAN), a task that is robustly related to reading fluency. In a qualitative review of the neuroimaging literature, we evaluated structural and functional MRI studies of reading fluency in readers from a range of skill levels. This was followed by a quantitative activation likelihood estimate (ALE) meta-analysis of fMRI studies of reading speed and RAN measures. We anticipated that reading speed, relative to untimed reading and reading-related tasks, would harness ventral reading pathways that are thought to enable the fast, visual recognition of words. The qualitative review showed that speeded reading taps the entire canonical reading network. The meta-analysis indicated a stronger role of the ventral reading pathway in rapid reading and rapid naming. Both reviews identified regions outside the canonical reading network that contribute to reading fluency, such as the bilateral insula and superior parietal lobule. We suggest that fluent reading engages both domain-specific reading pathways as well as domain-general regions that support overall task performance and discuss future avenues of research to expand our understanding of the neural bases of fluent reading.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":19279,"journal":{"name":"Neuropsychologia","volume":"202 ","pages":"Article 108947"},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2024-07-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141534943","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Functional connectivity during facial and vocal emotion recognition: Preliminary evidence for dissociations in developmental change by nonverbal modality 面部和声音情绪识别过程中的功能连接:非语言模式发展变化差异的初步证据。
IF 2 3区 心理学 Q3 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-06-28 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2024.108946
M. Morningstar , C. Hughes , R.C. French , C. Grannis , W.I. Mattson , E.E. Nelson

The developmental trajectory of emotion recognition (ER) skills is thought to vary by nonverbal modality, with vocal ER becoming mature later than facial ER. To investigate potential neural mechanisms contributing to this dissociation at a behavioural level, the current study examined whether youth's neural functional connectivity during vocal and facial ER tasks showed differential developmental change across time. Youth ages 8–19 (n = 41) completed facial and vocal ER tasks while undergoing functional magnetic resonance imaging, at two timepoints (1 year apart; n = 36 for behavioural data, n = 28 for neural data). Partial least squares analyses revealed that functional connectivity during ER is both distinguishable by modality (with different patterns of connectivity for facial vs. vocal ER) and across time—with changes in connectivity being particularly pronounced for vocal ER. ER accuracy was greater for faces than voices, and positively associated with age; although task performance did not change appreciably across a 1-year period, changes in latent functional connectivity patterns across time predicted participants' ER accuracy at Time 2. Taken together, these results suggest that vocal and facial ER are supported by distinguishable neural correlates that may undergo different developmental trajectories. Our findings are also preliminary evidence that changes in network integration may support the development of ER skills in childhood and adolescence.

情绪识别(ER)技能的发展轨迹被认为因非语言模式而异,声乐ER的成熟晚于面部ER。为了从行为层面研究造成这种差异的潜在神经机制,本研究考察了青少年在完成发声和面部情绪识别任务时的神经功能连接是否会随着时间的推移而出现不同的发展变化。8-19 岁的青少年(n = 41)在两个时间点(相隔 1 年;行为数据 n = 36,神经数据 n = 28)完成面部和发声 ER 任务,同时接受功能磁共振成像。偏最小二乘法分析表明,ER过程中的功能连通性既可按模式区分(面部ER和声音ER的连通性模式不同),也可跨时间区分--声音ER的连通性变化尤为明显。面孔ER的准确性高于声音ER,而且与年龄呈正相关;虽然任务表现在1年时间内没有明显变化,但潜伏功能连接模式在不同时间的变化预测了参与者在时间2时的ER准确性。综上所述,这些结果表明,发声和面部ER由不同的神经相关因素支持,这些神经相关因素可能经历不同的发展轨迹。我们的研究结果还初步证明,网络整合的变化可能会支持儿童和青少年ER技能的发展。
{"title":"Functional connectivity during facial and vocal emotion recognition: Preliminary evidence for dissociations in developmental change by nonverbal modality","authors":"M. Morningstar ,&nbsp;C. Hughes ,&nbsp;R.C. French ,&nbsp;C. Grannis ,&nbsp;W.I. Mattson ,&nbsp;E.E. Nelson","doi":"10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2024.108946","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2024.108946","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>The developmental trajectory of emotion recognition (ER) skills is thought to vary by nonverbal modality, with vocal ER becoming mature later than facial ER. To investigate potential neural mechanisms contributing to this dissociation at a behavioural level, the current study examined whether youth's neural functional connectivity during vocal and facial ER tasks showed differential developmental change across time. Youth ages 8–19 (<em>n</em> = 41) completed facial and vocal ER tasks while undergoing functional magnetic resonance imaging, at two timepoints (1 year apart; <em>n</em> = 36 for behavioural data, <em>n</em> = 28 for neural data). Partial least squares analyses revealed that functional connectivity during ER is both distinguishable by modality (with different patterns of connectivity for facial vs. vocal ER) and across time—with changes in connectivity being particularly pronounced for vocal ER. ER accuracy was greater for faces than voices, and positively associated with age; although task performance did not change appreciably across a 1-year period, changes in latent functional connectivity patterns across time predicted participants' ER accuracy at Time 2. Taken together, these results suggest that vocal and facial ER are supported by distinguishable neural correlates that may undergo different developmental trajectories. Our findings are also preliminary evidence that changes in network integration may support the development of ER skills in childhood and adolescence.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":19279,"journal":{"name":"Neuropsychologia","volume":"202 ","pages":"Article 108946"},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2024-06-28","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0028393224001611/pdfft?md5=29f9534acae8a01ea67d262188082a19&pid=1-s2.0-S0028393224001611-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141469673","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
No evidence for top-down expertise effects on action perception in sprinters using static images 没有证据表明自上而下的专业知识会对使用静态图像的短跑运动员的动作感知产生影响。
IF 2 3区 心理学 Q3 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-06-27 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2024.108945
Róisín Elaine Harrison , Martin Giesel , Constanze Hesse

Athletes have been found to demonstrate a superior ability to detect subtle variations in dynamic displays (e.g., point-light displays and videos) depicting expert actions compared to non-athletes. The current study aimed to determine whether this advantage also exists when dynamic information is unavailable (i.e., using static images). Using a staircase procedure, two frames from a video depicting an athlete either walking (everyday action) or performing a sprint start (expert action) were presented, and athletes (sprinters) and non-athletes were asked to indicate whether the images were identical or different. We examined whether presenting the images sequentially (temporal task) or simultaneously (spatial task) influenced participants' discrimination performance. We predicted that the sprinters would outperform the non-sprinters in the spatial task as body postures could be compared directly but not in the temporal task due to larger representational momentum effects for athletes. Contrary to our hypotheses, the sprinters and non-sprinters performed similarly in all tasks and conditions. In line with the prediction that representational momentum may impair performance, participants’ thresholds were lower for the spatial than the temporal task. However, post-hoc analysis suggested that this effect is likely to be better explained by a task order effect whereby participants who completed the temporal task first exhibited an advantage in the spatial task, while there were no performance differences for participants who completed the opposite task order. In sum, our results provide no evidence for the idea that motor expertise affects action perception (i.e., perceptual resonance) in a simple psychophysical task employing static images.

研究发现,与非运动员相比,运动员对描绘专家动作的动态显示(如点光源显示和视频)中细微变化的检测能力更强。本研究旨在确定在没有动态信息的情况下(即使用静态图像)是否也存在这种优势。我们采用阶梯式程序,从视频中展示了运动员行走(日常动作)或短跑起跑(专业动作)的两个画面,并要求运动员(短跑运动员)和非运动员指出这两个画面是相同还是不同。我们研究了顺序(时间任务)或同时(空间任务)呈现图像是否会影响参与者的辨别能力。我们预测短跑运动员在空间任务中的表现会优于非短跑运动员,因为身体姿势可以直接进行比较,但在时间任务中,由于运动员的表象动量效应较大,所以短跑运动员的表现不会优于非短跑运动员。与我们的假设相反,短跑运动员和非短跑运动员在所有任务和条件中的表现相似。与 "表象动量可能会影响表现 "的预测一致,参与者在空间任务中的阈值低于时间任务。然而,事后分析表明,任务顺序效应更能解释这种效应,即首先完成时间任务的参与者在空间任务中表现出优势,而完成相反任务顺序的参与者则没有表现差异。总之,我们的研究结果没有为运动专长影响静态图像简单心理物理任务中的动作感知(即感知共振)这一观点提供证据。
{"title":"No evidence for top-down expertise effects on action perception in sprinters using static images","authors":"Róisín Elaine Harrison ,&nbsp;Martin Giesel ,&nbsp;Constanze Hesse","doi":"10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2024.108945","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2024.108945","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Athletes have been found to demonstrate a superior ability to detect subtle variations in dynamic displays (e.g., point-light displays and videos) depicting expert actions compared to non-athletes. The current study aimed to determine whether this advantage also exists when dynamic information is unavailable (i.e., using static images). Using a staircase procedure, two frames from a video depicting an athlete either walking (everyday action) or performing a sprint start (expert action) were presented, and athletes (sprinters) and non-athletes were asked to indicate whether the images were identical or different. We examined whether presenting the images sequentially (temporal task) or simultaneously (spatial task) influenced participants' discrimination performance. We predicted that the sprinters would outperform the non-sprinters in the spatial task as body postures could be compared directly but not in the temporal task due to larger representational momentum effects for athletes. Contrary to our hypotheses, the sprinters and non-sprinters performed similarly in all tasks and conditions. In line with the prediction that representational momentum may impair performance, participants’ thresholds were lower for the spatial than the temporal task. However, post-hoc analysis suggested that this effect is likely to be better explained by a task order effect whereby participants who completed the temporal task first exhibited an advantage in the spatial task, while there were no performance differences for participants who completed the opposite task order. In sum, our results provide no evidence for the idea that motor expertise affects action perception (i.e., perceptual resonance) in a simple psychophysical task employing static images.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":19279,"journal":{"name":"Neuropsychologia","volume":"202 ","pages":"Article 108945"},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2024-06-27","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S002839322400160X/pdfft?md5=0c987b9d93fb2f7bf6dbe31fcf1dd090&pid=1-s2.0-S002839322400160X-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141469714","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Effects of attentional instructions on the behavioral and neural mechanisms of speech auditory feedback control 注意指令对言语听觉反馈控制的行为和神经机制的影响
IF 2 3区 心理学 Q3 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-06-24 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2024.108944
Yilun Zhang , Kimaya Sarmukadam , Yuan Wang , Roozbeh Behroozmand

The present study investigated how instructions for paying attention to auditory feedback may affect speech error detection and sensorimotor control. Electroencephalography (EEG) and speech signals were recorded from 21 neurologically intact adult subjects while they produced the speech vowel sound /a/ and received randomized ±100 cents pitch-shift alterations in their real-time auditory feedback. Subjects were instructed to pay attention to their auditory feedback and press a button to indicate whether they detected a pitch-shift stimulus during trials. Data for this group was compared with 22 matched subjects who completed the same speech task under altered auditory feedback condition without attentional instructions. Results revealed a significantly smaller magnitude of speech compensations in the attentional-instruction vs. no-instruction group and a positive linear association between the magnitude of compensations and P2 event-related potential (ERP) amplitudes. In addition, we found that the amplitude of P2 ERP component was significantly larger in the attentional-instruction vs. no-instruction group. Source localization analysis showed that this effect was accounted for by significantly stronger neural activities in the right hemisphere insula, precentral gyrus, postcentral gyrus, transverse temporal gyrus, and superior temporal gyrus in the attentional-instruction group. These findings suggest that attentional instructions may enhance speech auditory feedback error detection, and subsequently improve sensorimotor control via generating more stable speech outputs (i.e., smaller compensations) in response to pitch-shift alterations. Our data are informative for advancing theoretical models and motivating targeted interventions with a focus on the role of attentional instructions for improving treatment outcomes in patients with motor speech disorders.

本研究探讨了注意听觉反馈的指令会如何影响语音错误检测和感觉运动控制。本研究记录了 21 名神经系统完好的成年受试者的脑电图(EEG)和语音信号,当时他们正在发出语音元音 /a/,并在实时听觉反馈中接收随机的 ±100 分音高偏移变化。受试者被要求注意他们的听觉反馈,并在试验过程中按下按钮以指示他们是否检测到音调偏移刺激。我们将这组受试者的数据与 22 名匹配受试者的数据进行了比较,后者在听觉反馈改变的条件下完成了相同的语音任务,但没有注意力指示。结果显示,有注意指令组与无注意指令组的语音补偿幅度明显较小,而且补偿幅度与 P2 事件相关电位(ERP)振幅之间呈正线性关系。此外,我们还发现,注意指导组与无指导组相比,P2 ERP 分量的振幅明显更大。源定位分析表明,这一效应是由注意指导组右半球岛叶、中央前回、中央后回、颞横回和颞上回明显更强的神经活动造成的。这些研究结果表明,注意指令可能会增强语音听觉反馈错误检测,进而通过产生更稳定的语音输出(即更小的补偿)来应对音高偏移的改变,从而改善感觉运动控制。我们的数据对推进理论模型和有针对性的干预措施具有参考价值,重点是注意指令对改善运动性言语障碍患者治疗效果的作用。
{"title":"Effects of attentional instructions on the behavioral and neural mechanisms of speech auditory feedback control","authors":"Yilun Zhang ,&nbsp;Kimaya Sarmukadam ,&nbsp;Yuan Wang ,&nbsp;Roozbeh Behroozmand","doi":"10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2024.108944","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2024.108944","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>The present study investigated how instructions for paying attention to auditory feedback may affect speech error detection and sensorimotor control. Electroencephalography (EEG) and speech signals were recorded from 21 neurologically intact adult subjects while they produced the speech vowel sound /a/ and received randomized ±100 cents pitch-shift alterations in their real-time auditory feedback. Subjects were instructed to pay attention to their auditory feedback and press a button to indicate whether they detected a pitch-shift stimulus during trials. Data for this group was compared with 22 matched subjects who completed the same speech task under altered auditory feedback condition without attentional instructions. Results revealed a significantly smaller magnitude of speech compensations in the attentional-instruction <em>vs.</em> no-instruction group and a positive linear association between the magnitude of compensations and P2 event-related potential (ERP) amplitudes. In addition, we found that the amplitude of P2 ERP component was significantly larger in the attentional-instruction <em>vs.</em> no-instruction group. Source localization analysis showed that this effect was accounted for by significantly stronger neural activities in the right hemisphere insula, precentral gyrus, postcentral gyrus, transverse temporal gyrus, and superior temporal gyrus in the attentional-instruction group. These findings suggest that attentional instructions may enhance speech auditory feedback error detection, and subsequently improve sensorimotor control via generating more stable speech outputs (i.e., smaller compensations) in response to pitch-shift alterations. Our data are informative for advancing theoretical models and motivating targeted interventions with a focus on the role of attentional instructions for improving treatment outcomes in patients with motor speech disorders.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":19279,"journal":{"name":"Neuropsychologia","volume":"201 ","pages":"Article 108944"},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2024-06-24","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0028393224001593/pdfft?md5=8c90fe0883a323db38a9728f9a9ab1c8&pid=1-s2.0-S0028393224001593-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141458345","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The human brain deals with violating general color or depth knowledge in different time courses 人脑会在不同的时间进程中处理违反一般颜色或深度知识的情况。
IF 2 3区 心理学 Q3 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-06-21 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2024.108941
Xiaoyu Tang , Shilong Yu , Shigeko Takahashi , Jiajia Yang , Yoshimichi Ejima , Yulin Gao , Qiong Wu , Jinglong Wu

Utilizing the high temporal resolution of event-related potentials (ERPs), we compared the time course of processing incongruent color versus 3D-depth information. Participants were asked to judge whether the food color (color condition) or 3D structure (3D-depth condition) was congruent or incongruent with their previous knowledge and experience. The behavioral results showed that the reaction times in the congruent 3D-depth condition were slower than those in the congruent color condition. The reaction times in the incongruent 3D-depth condition were slower than those in the incongruent color condition. The ERP results showed that incongruent color stimuli induced a larger N270, larger P300, and smaller N400 components in the fronto-central region than the congruent color stimuli. Incongruent 3D-depth stimuli induced a smaller N1 in the occipital region, larger P300 and smaller N400 in the parietal-occipital region than congruent 3D-depth stimuli. The time–frequency analysis found that incongruent color stimuli induced a larger theta band (360–580 ms) activation in the fronto-central region than congruent color stimuli. Incongruent 3D-depth stimuli induced larger alpha and beta bands (240–350 ms) activation in the parietal region than congruent 3D-depth stimuli. Our results suggest that the human brain deals with violating general color or depth knowledge in different time courses. We speculate that the depth perception conflict was dominated by solving the problem with visual processing, whereas the color perception conflict was dominated by solving the problem with semantic violation.

利用事件相关电位(ERPs)的高时间分辨率,我们比较了处理不一致颜色和三维深度信息的时间过程。我们要求受试者判断食物的颜色(颜色条件)或三维结构(三维深度条件)与他们之前的知识和经验是一致还是不一致。行为结果显示,三维深度一致条件下的反应时间比颜色一致条件下的反应时间慢。三维深度不一致条件下的反应时间比颜色不一致条件下的反应时间慢。ERP结果显示,与同色刺激相比,不一致颜色刺激在前中央区诱发了更大的N270、更大的P300和更小的N400成分。与一致的三维深度刺激相比,不一致的三维深度刺激在枕叶区诱发了较小的 N1,在顶叶-枕叶区诱发了较大的 P300 和较小的 N400。时频分析发现,与颜色一致的刺激相比,不一致的颜色刺激在前中央区诱发了较大的θ波段(360-580 ms)激活。与一致的三维深度刺激相比,不一致的三维深度刺激在顶叶区诱发了更大的α和β波段(240-350 毫秒)激活。我们的研究结果表明,人脑会在不同的时间过程中处理违反一般颜色或深度知识的情况。我们推测,深度知觉冲突主要是通过视觉处理解决问题,而颜色知觉冲突主要是通过语义违规解决问题。
{"title":"The human brain deals with violating general color or depth knowledge in different time courses","authors":"Xiaoyu Tang ,&nbsp;Shilong Yu ,&nbsp;Shigeko Takahashi ,&nbsp;Jiajia Yang ,&nbsp;Yoshimichi Ejima ,&nbsp;Yulin Gao ,&nbsp;Qiong Wu ,&nbsp;Jinglong Wu","doi":"10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2024.108941","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2024.108941","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Utilizing the high temporal resolution of event-related potentials (ERPs), we compared the time course of processing incongruent color versus 3D-depth information. Participants were asked to judge whether the food color (color condition) or 3D structure (3D-depth condition) was congruent or incongruent with their previous knowledge and experience. The behavioral results showed that the reaction times in the congruent 3D-depth condition were slower than those in the congruent color condition. The reaction times in the incongruent 3D-depth condition were slower than those in the incongruent color condition. The ERP results showed that incongruent color stimuli induced a larger N270, larger P300, and smaller N400 components in the fronto-central region than the congruent color stimuli. Incongruent 3D-depth stimuli induced a smaller N1 in the occipital region, larger P300 and smaller N400 in the parietal-occipital region than congruent 3D-depth stimuli. The time–frequency analysis found that incongruent color stimuli induced a larger theta band (360–580 ms) activation in the fronto-central region than congruent color stimuli. Incongruent 3D-depth stimuli induced larger alpha and beta bands (240–350 ms) activation in the parietal region than congruent 3D-depth stimuli. Our results suggest that the human brain deals with violating general color or depth knowledge in different time courses. We speculate that the depth perception conflict was dominated by solving the problem with visual processing, whereas the color perception conflict was dominated by solving the problem with semantic violation.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":19279,"journal":{"name":"Neuropsychologia","volume":"201 ","pages":"Article 108941"},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2024-06-21","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141440765","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Integrity of autobiographical memory and episodic future thinking in older adults varies with cognitive functioning 老年人自传体记忆和外显未来思维的完整性随认知功能而变化。
IF 2 3区 心理学 Q3 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-06-20 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2024.108943
Audrey Li-Chay-Chung , Faryn Starrs , Jennifer D. Ryan , Morgan Barense , Rosanna K. Olsen , Donna Rose Addis

Research has documented changes in autobiographical memory and episodic future thinking in mild cognitive impairment (MCI) and Alzheimer's disease (AD). However, cognitive decline occurs gradually and recent findings suggest that subtle alterations in autobiographical cognition may be evident earlier in the trajectory towards dementia, before AD-related symptoms emerge or a clinical diagnosis has been given. The current study used the Autobiographical Interview to examine the episodic and semantic content of autobiographical past and future events generated by older adults (N = 38) of varying cognitive functioning who were grouped into High (N = 20) and Low Cognition (N = 18) groups based on their Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) scores. Participants described 12 past and 12 future autobiographical events, and transcripts were scored to quantify the numbers of internal (episodic) or external (non-episodic, including semantic) details. Although the Low Cognition group exhibited a differential reduction for internal details comprising both past and future events, they did not show the expected overproduction of external details relative to the High Cognition group. Multilevel modelling demonstrated that on trials lower in episodic content, semantic content was significantly increased in both groups. Although suggestive of a compensatory mechanism, the magnitude of this inverse relationship did not differ across groups or interact with MoCA scores. This finding indicates that external detail production may be underpinned by mechanisms not affected by cognitive decline, such as narrative style and the ability to contextualize one's past and future events in relation to broader autobiographical knowledge.

研究记录了轻度认知障碍(MCI)和阿尔茨海默病(AD)患者自传体记忆和外显未来思维的变化。然而,认知能力的衰退是逐渐发生的,最近的研究结果表明,自传体认知的细微变化可能在老年痴呆症相关症状出现或临床诊断之前的早期就已显现。目前的研究使用自传体访谈法来检查不同认知功能的老年人(38 人)所产生的自传体过去和未来事件的情节和语义内容,这些老年人根据蒙特利尔认知评估(MoCA)得分被分为高认知(20 人)和低认知(18 人)两组。参与者描述了 12 个过去和 12 个未来的自传事件,并对记录誊本进行评分,以量化内部(情节性)或外部(非情节性,包括语义)细节的数量。虽然低认知度组在过去和未来事件的内部细节方面表现出不同程度的减少,但与高认知度组相比,他们并没有表现出预期的外部细节过多。多层次建模表明,在情节内容较少的试验中,两组的语义内容都显著增加。虽然这表明了一种补偿机制,但这种反向关系的程度在不同组之间并无差异,也不会与 MoCA 分数产生交互作用。这一发现表明,外部细节的产生可能受到不受认知能力下降影响的机制的支持,如叙事风格和将自己过去和未来的事件与更广泛的自传知识联系起来的能力。
{"title":"Integrity of autobiographical memory and episodic future thinking in older adults varies with cognitive functioning","authors":"Audrey Li-Chay-Chung ,&nbsp;Faryn Starrs ,&nbsp;Jennifer D. Ryan ,&nbsp;Morgan Barense ,&nbsp;Rosanna K. Olsen ,&nbsp;Donna Rose Addis","doi":"10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2024.108943","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2024.108943","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Research has documented changes in autobiographical memory and episodic future thinking in mild cognitive impairment (MCI) and Alzheimer's disease (AD). However, cognitive decline occurs gradually and recent findings suggest that subtle alterations in autobiographical cognition may be evident earlier in the trajectory towards dementia, before AD-related symptoms emerge or a clinical diagnosis has been given. The current study used the Autobiographical Interview to examine the episodic and semantic content of autobiographical past and future events generated by older adults (<em>N</em> = 38) of varying cognitive functioning who were grouped into High (<em>N</em> = 20) and Low Cognition (<em>N</em> = 18) groups based on their Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) scores. Participants described 12 past and 12 future autobiographical events, and transcripts were scored to quantify the numbers of internal (episodic) or external (non-episodic, including semantic) details. Although the Low Cognition group exhibited a differential reduction for internal details comprising both past and future events, they did not show the expected overproduction of external details relative to the High Cognition group. Multilevel modelling demonstrated that on trials lower in episodic content, semantic content was significantly increased in both groups. Although suggestive of a compensatory mechanism, the magnitude of this inverse relationship did not differ across groups or interact with MoCA scores. This finding indicates that external detail production may be underpinned by mechanisms not affected by cognitive decline, such as narrative style and the ability to contextualize one's past and future events in relation to broader autobiographical knowledge.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":19279,"journal":{"name":"Neuropsychologia","volume":"201 ","pages":"Article 108943"},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2024-06-20","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141440764","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Post-stroke urinary incontinence is associated with behavior control deficits and overactive bladder 中风后尿失禁与行为控制缺陷和膀胱过度活跃有关。
IF 2 3区 心理学 Q3 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-06-19 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2024.108942
Michitaka Funayama , Akihiro Koreki , Taketo Takata , Yoshitaka Nakagawa , Masaru Mimura

Background

Although urinary incontinence in stroke survivors can substantially impact the patient's quality of life, the underlying neuropsychological mechanisms and its neural basis have not been adequately investigated. Therefore, we investigated this topic via neuropsychological assessment and neuroimaging in a cross-sectional study.

Methods

We recruited 71 individuals with cerebrovascular disease. The relationship between urinary incontinence and neuropsychological indices was investigated using simple linear regression analysis or Mann-Whitney U test, along with other explanatory variables, e.g., severity of overactive bladder. Variables with a p-value of <0.1 in the simple regression analysis were entered in the final multiple linear regression model to control for potential confounding factors. To carry out an in-depth examination of the neuroanatomical substrate for urinary incontinence, voxel-based lesion-behavior mapping was performed using MRIcron software.

Results

Behavioral control deficits and severity of overactive bladder were closely related to severity of urinary incontinence. The voxel-based lesion-behavior mapping suggests a potential role for ventromedial prefrontal cortex lesioning in the severity of urinary incontinence, although this association is not statistically significant.

Conclusions

Post-stroke urinary incontinence is closely related to two factors: neurogenic overactive bladder, a physiological disinhibition of micturition reflex, and cognitive dysfunction, characterized by behavior control deficits.

背景:尽管中风幸存者的尿失禁会严重影响患者的生活质量,但其潜在的神经心理学机制及其神经基础尚未得到充分研究。因此,我们在一项横断面研究中通过神经心理学评估和神经影像学检查对这一问题进行了调查:方法:我们招募了 71 名脑血管疾病患者。方法:我们招募了 71 名脑血管疾病患者,使用简单线性回归分析或 Mann-Whitney U 检验以及其他解释变量(如膀胱过度活动的严重程度)来研究尿失禁与神经心理学指标之间的关系。结果显示,P 值为 0 的变量为膀胱过度活动的严重程度:行为控制缺陷和膀胱过度活动症的严重程度与尿失禁的严重程度密切相关。基于体素的病变-行为映射表明,室内侧前额叶皮层病变对尿失禁的严重程度具有潜在作用,尽管这种关联在统计学上并不显著:结论:中风后尿失禁与两个因素密切相关:一是神经源性膀胱过度活动,这是一种生理性排尿反射抑制;二是认知功能障碍,其特征是行为控制缺陷。
{"title":"Post-stroke urinary incontinence is associated with behavior control deficits and overactive bladder","authors":"Michitaka Funayama ,&nbsp;Akihiro Koreki ,&nbsp;Taketo Takata ,&nbsp;Yoshitaka Nakagawa ,&nbsp;Masaru Mimura","doi":"10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2024.108942","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2024.108942","url":null,"abstract":"<div><h3>Background</h3><p>Although urinary incontinence in stroke survivors can substantially impact the patient's quality of life, the underlying neuropsychological mechanisms and its neural basis have not been adequately investigated. Therefore, we investigated this topic via neuropsychological assessment and neuroimaging in a cross-sectional study.</p></div><div><h3>Methods</h3><p>We recruited 71 individuals with cerebrovascular disease. The relationship between urinary incontinence and neuropsychological indices was investigated using simple linear regression analysis or Mann-Whitney <em>U</em> test, along with other explanatory variables, e.g., severity of overactive bladder. Variables with a <em>p</em>-value of &lt;0.1 in the simple regression analysis were entered in the final multiple linear regression model to control for potential confounding factors. To carry out an in-depth examination of the neuroanatomical substrate for urinary incontinence, voxel-based lesion-behavior mapping was performed using MRIcron software.</p></div><div><h3>Results</h3><p>Behavioral control deficits and severity of overactive bladder were closely related to severity of urinary incontinence. The voxel-based lesion-behavior mapping suggests a potential role for ventromedial prefrontal cortex lesioning in the severity of urinary incontinence, although this association is not statistically significant.</p></div><div><h3>Conclusions</h3><p>Post-stroke urinary incontinence is closely related to two factors: neurogenic overactive bladder, a physiological disinhibition of micturition reflex, and cognitive dysfunction, characterized by behavior control deficits.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":19279,"journal":{"name":"Neuropsychologia","volume":"201 ","pages":"Article 108942"},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2024-06-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141437260","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The primacy of experience in language processing: Semantic priming is driven primarily by experiential similarity 经验在语言处理中的首要地位:语义引申主要由经验相似性驱动
IF 2 3区 心理学 Q3 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-06-18 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2024.108939
Leonardo Fernandino , Lisa L. Conant

The organization of semantic memory, including memory for word meanings, has long been a central question in cognitive science. Although there is general agreement that word meaning representations must make contact with sensory-motor and affective experiences in a non-arbitrary fashion, the nature of this relationship remains controversial. One prominent view proposes that word meanings are represented directly in terms of their experiential content (i.e., sensory-motor and affective representations). Opponents of this view argue that the representation of word meanings reflects primarily taxonomic structure, that is, their relationships to natural categories. In addition, the recent success of language models based on word co-occurrence (i.e., distributional) information in emulating human linguistic behavior has led to proposals that this kind of information may play an important role in the representation of lexical concepts. We used a semantic priming paradigm designed for representational similarity analysis (RSA) to quantitatively assess how well each of these theories explains the representational similarity pattern for a large set of words. Crucially, we used partial correlation RSA to account for intercorrelations between model predictions, which allowed us to assess, for the first time, the unique effect of each model. Semantic priming was driven primarily by experiential similarity between prime and target, with no evidence of an independent effect of distributional or taxonomic similarity. Furthermore, only the experiential models accounted for unique variance in priming after partialling out explicit similarity ratings. These results support experiential accounts of semantic representation and indicate that, despite their good performance at some linguistic tasks, the distributional models evaluated here do not encode the same kind of information used by the human semantic system.

长期以来,语义记忆(包括词义记忆)的组织一直是认知科学的核心问题。尽管人们普遍认为词义表征必须以一种非任意的方式与感觉-运动和情感体验相联系,但这种关系的性质仍然存在争议。一种著名的观点认为,词义表征是直接以其经验内容(即感觉-运动和情感表征)为基础的。反对这一观点的人则认为,词义的表征主要反映的是分类结构,即词义与自然范畴的关系。此外,最近基于词的共现(即分布)信息的语言模型在模拟人类语言行为方面取得了成功,因此有人提出这类信息可能在词义概念的表征中扮演重要角色。我们使用了一种为表征相似性分析(RSA)而设计的语义引理范式,来定量评估这些理论中的每一种在多大程度上解释了大量词语的表征相似性模式。最重要的是,我们使用了部分相关 RSA 来解释模型预测之间的相互关系,这使我们第一次能够评估每个模型的独特效果。语义引物主要是由引物和目标之间的经验相似性驱动的,没有证据表明分布或分类相似性有独立的影响。此外,在剔除明确的相似性评级后,只有经验模型能解释引物的独特差异。这些结果支持语义表征的经验描述,并表明尽管分布模型在某些语言任务中表现出色,但本文评估的分布模型并没有编码人类语义系统所使用的同类信息。
{"title":"The primacy of experience in language processing: Semantic priming is driven primarily by experiential similarity","authors":"Leonardo Fernandino ,&nbsp;Lisa L. Conant","doi":"10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2024.108939","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2024.108939","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>The organization of semantic memory, including memory for word meanings, has long been a central question in cognitive science. Although there is general agreement that word meaning representations must make contact with sensory-motor and affective experiences in a non-arbitrary fashion, the nature of this relationship remains controversial. One prominent view proposes that word meanings are represented directly in terms of their experiential content (i.e., sensory-motor and affective representations). Opponents of this view argue that the representation of word meanings reflects primarily taxonomic structure, that is, their relationships to natural categories. In addition, the recent success of language models based on word co-occurrence (i.e., distributional) information in emulating human linguistic behavior has led to proposals that this kind of information may play an important role in the representation of lexical concepts. We used a semantic priming paradigm designed for representational similarity analysis (RSA) to quantitatively assess how well each of these theories explains the representational similarity pattern for a large set of words. Crucially, we used partial correlation RSA to account for intercorrelations between model predictions, which allowed us to assess, for the first time, the unique effect of each model. Semantic priming was driven primarily by experiential similarity between prime and target, with no evidence of an independent effect of distributional or taxonomic similarity. Furthermore, only the experiential models accounted for unique variance in priming after partialling out explicit similarity ratings. These results support experiential accounts of semantic representation and indicate that, despite their good performance at some linguistic tasks, the distributional models evaluated here do not encode the same kind of information used by the human semantic system.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":19279,"journal":{"name":"Neuropsychologia","volume":"201 ","pages":"Article 108939"},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2024-06-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141427339","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Rational adaptation in word production: Strong conceptual ability reduces the effect of lexical impairments on verb retrieval in aphasia 词汇生成中的合理适应:强大的概念能力可减少词汇障碍对失语症患者动词检索的影响。
IF 2.6 3区 心理学 Q3 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-06-15 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2024.108938
Haley C. Dresang , Tessa Warren , William D. Hula , Michael Walsh Dickey

Language users rely on both linguistic and conceptual processing abilities to efficiently comprehend or produce language. According to the principle of rational adaptation, the degree to which a cognitive system relies on one process vs. another can change under different conditions or disease states with the goal of optimizing behavior. In this study, we investigated rational adaptation in reliance on linguistic versus conceptual processing in aphasia, an acquired disorder of language. In individuals living with aphasia, verb-retrieval impairments are a pervasive deficit that negatively impacts communicative function. As such, we examined evidence of adaptation in verb production, using parallel measures to index impairment in two of verb naming's critical subcomponents: conceptual and linguistic processing. These component processes were evaluated using a standardized assessment battery designed to contrast non-linguistic (picture input) and linguistic (word input) tasks of conceptual action knowledge. The results indicate that non-linguistic conceptual action processing can be impaired in people with aphasia and contributes to verb-retrieval impairments. Furthermore, relatively unimpaired conceptual action processing can ameliorate the influence of linguistic processing deficits on verb-retrieval impairments. These findings are consistent with rational adaptation accounts, indicating that conceptual processing plays a key role in language function and can be leveraged in rehabilitation to improve verb retrieval in adults with chronic aphasia.

语言使用者依靠语言和概念处理能力来有效地理解或产生语言。根据理性适应原则,在不同的条件或疾病状态下,认知系统对一种处理过程与另一种处理过程的依赖程度会发生变化,从而达到优化行为的目的。在本研究中,我们调查了失语症(一种后天性语言障碍)患者对语言处理与概念处理依赖程度的理性适应。在失语症患者中,动词检索障碍是一种普遍存在的缺陷,对交流功能产生了负面影响。因此,我们使用平行测量法来评估动词命名的两个关键子组件:概念和语言处理的障碍,从而检查动词生成的适应性证据。我们使用标准化的评估系统对这两个部分进行了评估,该系统旨在对比非语言(图片输入)和语言(单词输入)的概念动作知识任务。结果表明,失语症患者的非语言概念动作处理能力可能会受损,并导致动词检索能力受损。此外,相对未受损的概念动作加工可以改善语言加工缺陷对动词检索障碍的影响。这些发现与理性适应的观点一致,表明概念加工在语言功能中起着关键作用,可以在康复治疗中利用概念加工来改善成年慢性失语症患者的动词检索能力。
{"title":"Rational adaptation in word production: Strong conceptual ability reduces the effect of lexical impairments on verb retrieval in aphasia","authors":"Haley C. Dresang ,&nbsp;Tessa Warren ,&nbsp;William D. Hula ,&nbsp;Michael Walsh Dickey","doi":"10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2024.108938","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2024.108938","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Language users rely on both linguistic and conceptual processing abilities to efficiently comprehend or produce language. According to the principle of rational adaptation, the degree to which a cognitive system relies on one process vs. another can change under different conditions or disease states with the goal of optimizing behavior. In this study, we investigated rational adaptation in reliance on linguistic versus conceptual processing in aphasia, an acquired disorder of language. In individuals living with aphasia, verb-retrieval impairments are a pervasive deficit that negatively impacts communicative function. As such, we examined evidence of adaptation in verb production, using parallel measures to index impairment in two of verb naming's critical subcomponents: conceptual and linguistic processing. These component processes were evaluated using a standardized assessment battery designed to contrast non-linguistic (picture input) and linguistic (word input) tasks of conceptual action knowledge. The results indicate that non-linguistic conceptual action processing can be impaired in people with aphasia and contributes to verb-retrieval impairments. Furthermore, relatively unimpaired conceptual action processing can ameliorate the influence of linguistic processing deficits on verb-retrieval impairments. These findings are consistent with rational adaptation accounts, indicating that conceptual processing plays a key role in language function and can be leveraged in rehabilitation to improve verb retrieval in adults with chronic aphasia.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":19279,"journal":{"name":"Neuropsychologia","volume":"201 ","pages":"Article 108938"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-06-15","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141331509","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Neuropsychologia
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1