首页 > 最新文献

Neuropsychologia最新文献

英文 中文
Openness to experience and cognitive functioning and decline in older adults: The mediating role of cognitive reserve 老年人对经验和认知功能的开放性与衰退:认知储备的中介作用
IF 2.6 3区 心理学 Q3 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2023-09-09 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2023.108655
Teresa Montoliu , Mariola Zapater-Fajarí , Vanesa Hidalgo , Alicia Salvador

Objective

Openness to experience has been consistently associated with better cognitive functioning in older people, but its association with cognitive decline is less clear. Cognitive reserve has been proposed as a mechanism underlying this relationship, but previous studies have reported mixed findings, possibly due to the different ways of conceptualizing cognitive reserve. We aimed to analyze the potential mediating role of cognitive reserve in the association between openness and cognitive functioning and decline in healthy older people.

Method

In Wave 1 and at the four-year follow-up (Wave 2), 87 healthy older people (49.4% women; M age = 65.08, SD = 4.54) completed a neuropsychological battery to assess cognitive functioning and a questionnaire to assess cognitive reserve. Openness was measured with the NEO- Five-Factor Inventory. Mediation models were proposed to investigate the relationship between openness and cognitive function or decline through cognitive reserve or its change.

Results

Cognitive reserve mediated the openness-cognitive functioning association. Thus, individuals with higher openness showed greater cognitive reserve, and this greater cognitive reserve was associated with better cognitive functioning. Moreover, greater cognitive reserve at baseline also mediated the association between higher openness and slower cognitive decline. However, change in cognitive reserve did not mediate the association between openness and change in cognitive functioning.

Conclusions

Cognitive reserve is a mechanism underlying the association between openness and cognitive functioning and decline. These findings support the differential preservation hypothesis, suggesting that healthy older adults who engage in more cognitively stimulating activities would show less age-related cognitive decline.

客观经验的开放性一直与老年人更好的认知功能有关,但其与认知能力下降的关系尚不清楚。认知储备被认为是这种关系的一种机制,但之前的研究报告了喜忧参半的结果,可能是由于认知储备的概念化方式不同。我们旨在分析认知储备在健康老年人的开放性、认知功能和衰退之间的潜在中介作用。方法在第1波和四年随访(第2波)中,87名健康老年人(49.4%为女性;M年龄=65.08,SD=4.54)完成了评估认知功能的神经心理测验和评估认知储备的问卷。开放性是用近地天体五因素清单来衡量的。提出了中介模型,通过认知储备或其变化来研究开放性与认知功能或衰退之间的关系。结果认知储备介导开放性认知功能联想。因此,具有更高开放性的个体表现出更大的认知储备,这种更大的感知储备与更好的认知功能有关。此外,基线时更大的认知储备也介导了更高的开放性和更慢的认知下降之间的联系。然而,认知储备的变化并不能调节开放性和认知功能变化之间的联系。结论认知储备是开放性与认知功能和衰退之间联系的机制。这些发现支持差异保存假说,表明参与更多认知刺激活动的健康老年人与年龄相关的认知能力下降较少。
{"title":"Openness to experience and cognitive functioning and decline in older adults: The mediating role of cognitive reserve","authors":"Teresa Montoliu ,&nbsp;Mariola Zapater-Fajarí ,&nbsp;Vanesa Hidalgo ,&nbsp;Alicia Salvador","doi":"10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2023.108655","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2023.108655","url":null,"abstract":"<div><h3>Objective</h3><p>Openness to experience has been consistently associated with better cognitive functioning in older people, but its association with cognitive decline is less clear. Cognitive reserve has been proposed as a mechanism underlying this relationship, but previous studies have reported mixed findings, possibly due to the different ways of conceptualizing cognitive reserve. We aimed to analyze the potential mediating role of cognitive reserve in the association between openness and cognitive functioning and decline in healthy older people.</p></div><div><h3>Method</h3><p>In Wave 1 and at the four-year follow-up (Wave 2), 87 healthy older people (49.4% women; M age = 65.08, SD = 4.54) completed a neuropsychological battery to assess cognitive functioning and a questionnaire to assess cognitive reserve. Openness was measured with the NEO- Five-Factor Inventory. Mediation models were proposed to investigate the relationship between openness and cognitive function or decline through cognitive reserve or its change.</p></div><div><h3>Results</h3><p>Cognitive reserve mediated the openness-cognitive functioning association. Thus, individuals with higher openness showed greater cognitive reserve, and this greater cognitive reserve was associated with better cognitive functioning. Moreover, greater cognitive reserve at baseline also mediated the association between higher openness and slower cognitive decline. However, change in cognitive reserve did not mediate the association between openness and change in cognitive functioning.</p></div><div><h3>Conclusions</h3><p>Cognitive reserve is a mechanism underlying the association between openness and cognitive functioning and decline. These findings support the differential preservation hypothesis, suggesting that healthy older adults who engage in more cognitively stimulating activities would show less age-related cognitive decline.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":19279,"journal":{"name":"Neuropsychologia","volume":"188 ","pages":"Article 108655"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2023-09-09","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10140272","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Processing objects of perceptual expertise: Differential interhemispheric transmission efficiency but similar transmission direction advantages 感知专业知识的处理对象:半球间传输效率不同但传输方向相似的优势
IF 2.6 3区 心理学 Q3 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2023-09-09 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2023.108568
Simin Wan , Yini Sun , Qing Ye , Yu Gu , Werner Sommer , Xiaohua Cao

Faces and Chinese characters are both objects of perceptual expertise. In this study, we investigated the characteristics of interhemispheric transmission times (IHTTs) in both transmission direction and transmission efficiency during the processing of objects of perceptual expertise. A total of 112 participants engaged in a divided visual field paradigm for faces, Chinese characters, and houses in both upright and inverted orientations. The N170 amplitudes elicited by the objects of perceptual expertise (faces and Chinese characters) involved in this study were larger than those elicited by the non-perceptual expertise objects (houses). We used the latencies of the N170 component of the event-related potential (ERP) recorded in the left and right hemispheres to calculate the IHTTs. For all objects, the N170-related IHTTs from the right to the left hemispheres were shorter than those in the opposite direction. Essentially, the N170-related IHTTs for faces were shorter, that is, more efficient than those for Chinese characters and houses. This result indicates that the IHTTs during perceptual expertise and non-perceptual expertise object processing share a common transmission direction advantage, but transmission efficiency is face-specific.

面孔和汉字都是感性专门知识的对象。在本研究中,我们研究了在处理感知专业知识对象过程中,半球间传输时间(IHTT)在传输方向和传输效率方面的特征。共有112名参与者参与了一个垂直和倒置方向的人脸、汉字和房屋的分视野范式。本研究涉及的感知专业对象(人脸和汉字)引发的N170振幅大于非感知专业对象的N170波幅。我们使用左右半球记录的事件相关电位(ERP)N170成分的潜伏期来计算IHTT。对于所有物体,从右到左半球的N170相关IHTT都比相反方向的短。从本质上讲,与N170相关的人脸IHTT更短,也就是说,比汉字和房屋的IHTT更有效。这一结果表明,在感知专业知识和非感知专业知识对象处理过程中,IHTT具有共同的传输方向优势,但传输效率是面向特定人脸的。
{"title":"Processing objects of perceptual expertise: Differential interhemispheric transmission efficiency but similar transmission direction advantages","authors":"Simin Wan ,&nbsp;Yini Sun ,&nbsp;Qing Ye ,&nbsp;Yu Gu ,&nbsp;Werner Sommer ,&nbsp;Xiaohua Cao","doi":"10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2023.108568","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2023.108568","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Faces and Chinese characters are both objects of perceptual expertise. In this study, we investigated the characteristics of interhemispheric transmission times (IHTTs) in both transmission direction and transmission efficiency during the processing of objects of perceptual expertise. A total of 112 participants engaged in a divided visual field paradigm for faces, Chinese characters, and houses in both upright and inverted orientations. The N170 amplitudes elicited by the objects of perceptual expertise (faces and Chinese characters) involved in this study were larger than those elicited by the non-perceptual expertise objects (houses). We used the latencies of the N170 component of the event-related potential (ERP) recorded in the left and right hemispheres to calculate the IHTTs. For all objects, the N170-related IHTTs from the right to the left hemispheres were shorter than those in the opposite direction. Essentially, the N170-related IHTTs for faces were shorter, that is, more efficient than those for Chinese characters and houses. This result indicates that the IHTTs during perceptual expertise and non-perceptual expertise object processing share a common transmission direction advantage, but transmission efficiency is face-specific.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":19279,"journal":{"name":"Neuropsychologia","volume":"188 ","pages":"Article 108568"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2023-09-09","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10136819","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Long-term phonemic representations become audiovisual by mid-childhood 长期的音位表征在儿童中期就变成了视听的。
IF 2.6 3区 心理学 Q3 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2023-09-09 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2023.108633
Natalya Kaganovich , Jennifer Schumaker , Sharon Christ

In earlier work with adults, we showed that long-term phonemic representations are audiovisual, meaning that they contain information on typical mouth shape during articulation. Many aspects of audiovisual processing have a prolonged developmental course, often not reaching maturity until late adolescence. In this study, we examined the status of phonemic representations in two groups of children – 8-9-year-olds and 11-12-year-olds. We used the same audiovisual oddball paradigm as in the earlier study with adults (Kaganovich and Christ, 2021). On each trial, participants saw a face and heard one of two vowels. One vowel occurred frequently (standard), while another occurred rarely (deviant). In one condition (neutral), the face had a closed, non-articulating mouth. In the other condition (audiovisual violation), the mouth shape matched the frequent vowel. Although stimuli were audiovisual in both conditions, we hypothesized that identical auditory changes would be perceived differently by participants. Namely, in the neutral condition, deviants violated only the audiovisual pattern specific to each experimental block. By contrast, in the audiovisual violation condition, deviants additionally violated long-term representations for how a speaker's mouth looks during articulation. We compared the amplitude of MMN and P3 components elicited by deviants in the two conditions. In the 11-12-year-old group, the pattern of neural responses was similar to that in adults – namely, they had a larger MMN component in the audiovisual compared to neutral condition, with no major difference in the P3 amplitude. In contrast, in the 8-9-year-old group, we saw a posterior MMN in the neutral condition only and a larger P3 in the audiovisual violation compared to the neutral condition. The larger P3 in the audiovisual violation condition suggests that younger children did perceive deviants as being more attention-grabbing when they violated the typical combination of sound and mouth shape. Yet, at this age, the earlier, more automatic stages of phonemic processing indexed by the MMN component may not yet encode visual speech elements the same way they do in older children and adults. We conclude that phonemic representations do not become audiovisual until 11–12 years of age.

在早期对成年人的研究中,我们发现长期的音位表征是视听的,这意味着它们包含了发音过程中典型口型的信息。视听加工的许多方面都有一个漫长的发展过程,通常要到青春期晚期才能成熟。在这项研究中,我们检查了两组儿童(8-9岁和11-12岁)的音位表征状况。我们使用了与早期成人研究相同的视听怪人范式(Kaganovich和Christ,2021)。在每次试验中,参与者看到一张脸,听到两个元音中的一个。一个元音经常出现(标准),而另一个元音很少出现(异常)。在一种情况下(中性),面部有一个闭合的、没有关节的嘴巴。在另一种情况下(视听违规),口型与频繁元音匹配。尽管在这两种情况下刺激都是视听的,但我们假设相同的听觉变化会被参与者感知到不同。也就是说,在中性条件下,偏离者只违反了每个实验块特有的视听模式。相比之下,在视听侵犯条件下,越轨者还侵犯了说话者在发音过程中口腔外观的长期表征。我们比较了两种条件下偏差引起的MMN和P3分量的振幅。在11-12岁的组中,神经反应的模式与成年人相似,即与中性条件相比,他们在视听条件下的MMN成分更大,P3振幅没有重大差异。相反,在8-9岁的组中,与中性条件相比,我们只在中性条件下看到了后部MMN,在视听侵犯中看到了更大的P3。视听侵犯条件下较大的P3表明,年龄较小的儿童在侵犯声音和口型的典型组合时,确实认为越轨者更能吸引注意力。然而,在这个年龄段,由MMN成分索引的更早、更自动的音素处理阶段可能还没有像在年龄较大的儿童和成年人中那样对视觉语音元素进行编码。我们得出的结论是,音位表征直到11-12岁才变成视听的。
{"title":"Long-term phonemic representations become audiovisual by mid-childhood","authors":"Natalya Kaganovich ,&nbsp;Jennifer Schumaker ,&nbsp;Sharon Christ","doi":"10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2023.108633","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2023.108633","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>In earlier work with adults, we showed that long-term phonemic representations are audiovisual, meaning that they contain information on typical mouth shape during articulation. Many aspects of audiovisual processing have a prolonged developmental course, often not reaching maturity until late adolescence. In this study, we examined the status of phonemic representations in two groups of children – 8-9-year-olds and 11-12-year-olds. We used the same audiovisual oddball paradigm<span> as in the earlier study with adults (Kaganovich and Christ, 2021). On each trial, participants saw a face and heard one of two vowels. One vowel occurred frequently (standard), while another occurred rarely (deviant). In one condition (neutral), the face had a closed, non-articulating mouth. In the other condition (audiovisual violation), the mouth shape matched the frequent vowel. Although stimuli were audiovisual in both conditions, we hypothesized that identical auditory changes would be perceived differently by participants. Namely, in the neutral condition, deviants violated only the audiovisual pattern specific to each experimental block. By contrast, in the audiovisual violation condition, deviants additionally violated long-term representations for how a speaker's mouth looks during articulation. We compared the amplitude of MMN and P3 components elicited by deviants in the two conditions. In the 11-12-year-old group, the pattern of neural responses was similar to that in adults – namely, they had a larger MMN component in the audiovisual compared to neutral condition, with no major difference in the P3 amplitude. In contrast, in the 8-9-year-old group, we saw a posterior MMN in the neutral condition only and a larger P3 in the audiovisual violation compared to the neutral condition. The larger P3 in the audiovisual violation condition suggests that younger children did perceive deviants as being more attention-grabbing when they violated the typical combination of sound and mouth shape. Yet, at this age, the earlier, more automatic stages of phonemic processing indexed by the MMN component may not yet encode visual speech elements the same way they do in older children and adults. We conclude that phonemic representations do not become audiovisual until 11–12 years of age.</span></p></div>","PeriodicalId":19279,"journal":{"name":"Neuropsychologia","volume":"188 ","pages":"Article 108633"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2023-09-09","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10530328/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10140250","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Narrative simulation of social experiences in naturalistic context – A neurocinematic approach 自然主义背景下社会体验的叙事模拟——一种神经网络方法
IF 2.6 3区 心理学 Q3 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2023-09-09 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2023.108654
Pia Tikka , Mauri Kaipainen , Juha Salmi

Narratives may be regarded as simulations of everyday social situations. They are key to studying the human mind in socio-culturally determined contexts as they allow anchoring to the common ground of embodied and environmentally-engaged cognition. Here we review recent findings from naturalistic neuroscience on neural functions in conditions that mimic lifelike situations. We will focus particularly on neurocinematics, a research field that applies mediated narratives as stimuli for neuroimaging experiments. During the last two decades, this paradigm has contributed to an accumulation of insights about the neural underpinnings of behavior and sense-making in various narratively contextualized situations particularly pertaining to socio-emotional encounters. One of the key questions in neurocinematics is, how do intersubjectively synchronized brain activations relate to subjective experiences? Another question we address is how to bring natural contexts into experimental studies. Seeking to respond to both questions, we suggest neurocinematic studies to examine three manifestations of the same phenomenon side-by-side: subjective experiences of narrative situations, unfolding of narrative stimulus structure, and neural processes that co-constitute the experience. This approach facilitates identifying experientially meaningful activity patterns in the brain and points out what they may mean in relation to shared and communicable contents. Via rich-featured and temporally contextualized narrative stimuli, neurocinematics attempts to contribute to emerging holistic theories of neural dynamics and connectomics explaining typical and atypical interindividual variability.

叙事可以被看作是对日常社会状况的模拟。它们是在社会文化决定的背景下研究人类思维的关键,因为它们可以锚定具体化和环境参与认知的共同点。在这里,我们回顾了自然主义神经科学在模拟逼真情况下的神经功能的最新发现。我们将特别关注神经运动学,这是一个应用中介叙事作为神经成像实验刺激的研究领域。在过去的二十年里,这种范式有助于积累对各种叙事情境下行为和意义形成的神经基础的见解,特别是与社会情感遭遇有关的情况。神经学家的一个关键问题是,主体间同步的大脑激活如何与主观体验相关?我们要解决的另一个问题是如何将自然环境纳入实验研究。为了回答这两个问题,我们建议进行神经系统学研究,同时检查同一现象的三种表现:叙事情境的主观体验、叙事刺激结构的展开以及共同构成体验的神经过程。这种方法有助于识别大脑中有经验意义的活动模式,并指出它们在共享和可传播内容方面可能意味着什么。通过丰富的特征和时间语境化的叙事刺激,神经运动学试图为新兴的神经动力学和连接组学的整体理论做出贡献,解释典型和非典型的个体间变异性。
{"title":"Narrative simulation of social experiences in naturalistic context – A neurocinematic approach","authors":"Pia Tikka ,&nbsp;Mauri Kaipainen ,&nbsp;Juha Salmi","doi":"10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2023.108654","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2023.108654","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Narratives may be regarded as simulations of everyday social situations. They are key to studying the human mind in socio-culturally determined contexts as they allow anchoring to the common ground of embodied and environmentally-engaged cognition. Here we review recent findings from naturalistic neuroscience on neural functions in conditions that mimic lifelike situations. We will focus particularly on neurocinematics, a research field that applies mediated narratives as stimuli for neuroimaging experiments. During the last two decades, this paradigm has contributed to an accumulation of insights about the neural underpinnings of behavior and sense-making in various narratively contextualized situations particularly pertaining to socio-emotional encounters. One of the key questions in neurocinematics is, how do intersubjectively synchronized brain activations relate to subjective experiences? Another question we address is how to bring natural contexts into experimental studies. Seeking to respond to both questions, we suggest neurocinematic studies to examine three manifestations of the same phenomenon side-by-side: subjective experiences of narrative situations, unfolding of narrative stimulus structure, and neural processes that co-constitute the experience. This approach facilitates identifying experientially meaningful activity patterns in the brain and points out what they may mean in relation to shared and communicable contents. Via rich-featured and temporally contextualized narrative stimuli, neurocinematics attempts to contribute to emerging holistic theories of neural dynamics and connectomics explaining typical and atypical interindividual variability.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":19279,"journal":{"name":"Neuropsychologia","volume":"188 ","pages":"Article 108654"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2023-09-09","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10140274","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 3
An fMRI study of inflectional encoding in spoken word production: Role of domain-general inhibition 屈折编码在口语产生中的功能磁共振成像研究:域一般抑制的作用
IF 2.6 3区 心理学 Q3 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2023-09-09 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2023.108653
João Ferreira , Ardi Roelofs , Guilherme Blazquez Freches , Vitória Piai

A major issue concerning inflectional encoding in spoken word production is whether or not regular forms (e.g., past tense walked) are encoded by rule application and irregular forms (e.g., swam) by retrieval from associative memory and inhibition of the regular rule. We used functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) to examine the involvement of domain-general inhibition, thought to be underpinned by right inferior frontal gyrus (IFG), right pre-supplementary motor area (SMA), and right basal ganglia. Participants were presented with infinitive verbs that take either regular or irregular past tense. They switched between producing the past tense of these regular and irregular verbs in one block, and between inflecting or reading these infinitive verbs aloud in another block. As concerns corticobasal areas, compared to reading, inflecting activated left IFG and left preSMA/SMA. Regulars yielded higher activation than irregulars in these frontal areas, both on switch and repeat trials, which did not differ in activation. Switching between inflecting and reading activated left preSMA/SMA. These results indicate that inflectional encoding, and switching between inflecting and reading, engage frontal areas in the left hemisphere, including left preSMA/SMA for both and left IFG for inflecting, without recruiting the domain-general inhibition circuitry in the right hemisphere. We advance an account of inflectional encoding in spoken word production that assumes a distinction between regulars and irregulars, but without engaging domain-general inhibition.

口语产生中的屈折编码的一个主要问题是规则形式(如过去时wald)是否通过规则应用进行编码,不规则形式(例如swim)是否通过从联想记忆中检索和抑制规则进行编码。我们使用功能性磁共振成像(fMRI)来检查域普遍抑制的参与,该抑制被认为是由右额下回(IFG)、右补充运动前区(SMA)和右基底神经节支持的。向参与者展示了采用规则或不规则过去时的不定式动词。他们在一个块中产生这些规则动词和不规则动词的过去时之间切换,在另一块中屈折或大声朗读这些不定式动词之间切换。关于皮质基底区,与阅读相比,屈折激活了左侧IFG和左侧前SMA/SMA。在这些额叶区域,无论是在转换试验还是重复试验中,规则区的激活率都高于非规则区,这在激活率上没有差异。在屈折和阅读之间切换激活了左前SMA/SMA。这些结果表明,屈折编码以及屈折和阅读之间的切换涉及左半球的额叶区域,包括两者的左前SMA/SMA和屈折的左IFG,而没有募集右半球的域通用抑制电路。我们提出了一种对口语单词产生中屈折编码的描述,该描述假设了正则和非正则之间的区别,但没有涉及领域一般抑制。
{"title":"An fMRI study of inflectional encoding in spoken word production: Role of domain-general inhibition","authors":"João Ferreira ,&nbsp;Ardi Roelofs ,&nbsp;Guilherme Blazquez Freches ,&nbsp;Vitória Piai","doi":"10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2023.108653","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2023.108653","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>A major issue concerning inflectional encoding in spoken word production is whether or not regular forms (e.g., past tense <em>walked</em>) are encoded by rule application and irregular forms (e.g., <em>swam</em><span>) by retrieval from associative memory<span> and inhibition of the regular rule. We used functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) to examine the involvement of domain-general inhibition, thought to be underpinned by right inferior frontal gyrus (IFG), right pre-supplementary motor area (SMA), and right basal ganglia. Participants were presented with infinitive verbs that take either regular or irregular past tense. They switched between producing the past tense of these regular and irregular verbs in one block, and between inflecting or reading these infinitive verbs aloud in another block. As concerns corticobasal areas, compared to reading, inflecting activated left IFG and left preSMA/SMA. Regulars yielded higher activation than irregulars in these frontal areas, both on switch and repeat trials, which did not differ in activation. Switching between inflecting and reading activated left preSMA/SMA. These results indicate that inflectional encoding, and switching between inflecting and reading, engage frontal areas in the left hemisphere, including left preSMA/SMA for both and left IFG for inflecting, without recruiting the domain-general inhibition circuitry in the right hemisphere. We advance an account of inflectional encoding in spoken word production that assumes a distinction between regulars and irregulars, but without engaging domain-general inhibition.</span></span></p></div>","PeriodicalId":19279,"journal":{"name":"Neuropsychologia","volume":"188 ","pages":"Article 108653"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2023-09-09","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10493977","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Hemispheric contributions toward interoception and emotion recognition in left-vs right-semantic dementia 左右语义痴呆患者半球对互觉和情绪识别的贡献
IF 2.6 3区 心理学 Q3 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2023-09-09 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2023.108628
Jessica L. Hazelton , Emma Devenney , Rebekah Ahmed , James Burrell , Yun Hwang , Olivier Piguet , Fiona Kumfor

Background

The hemispheric contributions toward interoception, the perception of internal bodily cues, and emotion recognition remains unclear. Semantic dementia cases with either left-dominant (i.e., left-SD) or right-dominant (i.e., right-SD) anterior temporal lobe atrophy experience emotion recognition difficulties, however, little is known about interoception in these syndromes. Here, we hypothesised that right-SD would show worse interoception and emotion recognition due to right-dominant atrophy.

Methods

Thirty-five participants (8 left-SD; 6 right-SD; 21 controls) completed a monitoring task. Participants pressed a button when they: (1) felt their heartbeat, without pulse measurement (Interoception); or (2) heard a recorded heartbeat (Exteroception-control). Simultaneous ECG was recorded. Accuracy was calculated by comparing the event frequency (i.e., heartbeat or sound) to response frequency. Emotion recognition was assessed via the Facial Affect Selection Task. Voxel-based morphometry analyses identified neural correlates of interoception and emotion recognition.

Results

Right-SD showed worse interoception than controls and left-SD (both p's < 0.001). Both patient groups showed worse emotion recognition than controls (right-SD: p < .001; left-SD: p = .018), and right-SD showed worse emotion recognition than left-SD (p = .003). Regression analyses revealed that worse emotion recognition was predicted by right-SD (p = .002), left-SD (p = .005), and impaired interoception (p = .004). Interoception and emotion were associated with the integrity of right-lateralised structures including the insula, temporal pole, thalamus, superior temporal gyrus, and hippocampus.

Conclusion

Our study provides the first evidence for impaired interoception in right-SD, suggesting that impaired emotion recognition in this syndrome is driven by inaccurate internal monitoring. Further we identified a common neurobiological basis for interoception and emotion in the right hemisphere.

背景半球对内感受、对身体内部线索的感知和情绪识别的贡献尚不清楚。患有左显性(即左SD)或右显性(即右SD)前颞叶萎缩的语义性痴呆病例会出现情绪识别困难,然而,对这些综合征中的内感受知之甚少。在这里,我们假设由于右显性萎缩,右SD会表现出更差的内感受和情绪识别。方法35名受试者(左侧SD 8名,右侧SD 6名,对照组21名)完成监测任务。参与者按下按钮时:(1)感觉到自己的心跳,没有脉搏测量(Interception);或(2)听到记录的心跳(外部感受控制)。同时记录心电图。通过将事件频率(即心跳或声音)与响应频率进行比较来计算准确性。情绪识别通过面部情感选择任务进行评估。基于体素的形态计量学分析确定了交互感受和情绪识别的神经相关性。结果右SD表现出比对照组和左SD更差的互感(均p<0.001)。两组患者的情绪识别能力都比对照组差(右SD:p<0.001;左SD:p=.018),右SD表现得比左SD差(p=.003)。回归分析显示,右SD预测情绪识别能力较差(p=0.002),左侧SD(p=0.005)和感觉间受损(p=0.004)。感觉间和情绪与右侧结构的完整性有关,包括岛叶、颞极、丘脑、颞上回和海马。结论我们的研究为右SD患者的内感受受损提供了第一个证据,表明该综合征的情绪识别受损是由不准确的内部监测引起的。此外,我们确定了右半球内感受和情绪的共同神经生物学基础。
{"title":"Hemispheric contributions toward interoception and emotion recognition in left-vs right-semantic dementia","authors":"Jessica L. Hazelton ,&nbsp;Emma Devenney ,&nbsp;Rebekah Ahmed ,&nbsp;James Burrell ,&nbsp;Yun Hwang ,&nbsp;Olivier Piguet ,&nbsp;Fiona Kumfor","doi":"10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2023.108628","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2023.108628","url":null,"abstract":"<div><h3>Background</h3><p>The hemispheric contributions toward interoception, the perception of internal bodily cues, and emotion recognition remains unclear. Semantic dementia cases with either left-dominant (i.e., left-SD) or right-dominant (i.e., right-SD) anterior temporal lobe atrophy experience emotion recognition difficulties, however, little is known about interoception in these syndromes. Here, we hypothesised that right-SD would show worse interoception and emotion recognition due to right-dominant atrophy.</p></div><div><h3>Methods</h3><p>Thirty-five participants (8 left-SD; 6 right-SD; 21 controls) completed a monitoring task. Participants pressed a button when they: (1) felt their heartbeat, without pulse measurement (Interoception); or (2) heard a recorded heartbeat (Exteroception-control). Simultaneous ECG was recorded. Accuracy was calculated by comparing the event frequency (i.e., heartbeat or sound) to response frequency. Emotion recognition was assessed via the Facial Affect Selection Task. Voxel-based morphometry analyses identified neural correlates of interoception and emotion recognition.</p></div><div><h3>Results</h3><p>Right-SD showed worse interoception than controls and left-SD (both <em>p's</em> &lt; 0.001). Both patient groups showed worse emotion recognition than controls (right-SD: <em>p</em> &lt; .001; left-SD: <em>p</em> = .018), and right-SD showed worse emotion recognition than left-SD (<em>p</em> = .003). Regression analyses revealed that worse emotion recognition was predicted by right-SD (<em>p</em> = .002), left-SD (<em>p</em> = .005), and impaired interoception (<em>p</em> = .004). Interoception and emotion were associated with the integrity of right-lateralised structures including the insula, temporal pole, thalamus, superior temporal gyrus, and hippocampus.</p></div><div><h3>Conclusion</h3><p>Our study provides the first evidence for impaired interoception in right-SD, suggesting that impaired emotion recognition in this syndrome is driven by inaccurate internal monitoring. Further we identified a common neurobiological basis for interoception and emotion in the right hemisphere.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":19279,"journal":{"name":"Neuropsychologia","volume":"188 ","pages":"Article 108628"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2023-09-09","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10130241","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Dichotic-listening performance after complete callosotomy: No relief from left-ear extinction by selective attention 胼胝体完全切除术后的二分听表现:选择性注意对左耳消光没有缓解作用
IF 2.6 3区 心理学 Q3 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2023-09-09 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2023.108627
René Westerhausen , Mara Fabri , Markus Hausmann

The surgical section of the corpus callosum (callosotomy) has been frequently demonstrated to result in a left-ear extinction in dichotic listening. That is, callosotomy patients report the left-ear stimulus below chance level, resulting in substantially enhanced right-ear advantage (REA) compared with controls. A small number of previous studies also suggest that callosotomy patients can overcome left-ear extinction when the instruction encourages to attend selectively to the left-ear stimulus. In the present case study, we re-examine the role of selective attention in dichotic listening in two patients with complete callosotomy and 40 age- and sex-matched controls. We used the standardised Bergen dichotic-listening paradigm which uses stop-consonant-vowel syllables as stimulus material and includes both a free-report and selective-attention condition. As was predicted, both patients showed a clear left-ear extinction. However, contrasting the earlier reports, we did not find any evidence for a relief from this extinction by selectively attending to the left-ear stimulus. We conclude that previous demonstrations of an attention-improved left-ear recall in callosotomy patients may be attributed to the use of suboptimal dichotic paradigms or residual callosal connectivity, rather than representing a genuine effect of attention.

胼胝体的外科切除术(胼胝体切开术)经常被证明会导致双耳听力中的左耳消失。也就是说,胼胝体切开术患者报告的左耳刺激低于机会水平,与对照组相比,右耳优势(REA)显著增强。先前的少量研究也表明,当指导鼓励选择性地注意左耳刺激时,胼胝体切开术患者可以克服左耳消光。在本案例研究中,我们在两名完全胼胝体切除患者和40名年龄和性别匹配的对照组中,重新审视了选择性注意力在二分听中的作用。我们使用了标准化的卑尔根二分听范式,该范式使用停止辅音元音音节作为刺激材料,包括自由报告和选择性注意条件。正如预测的那样,两名患者都表现出明显的左耳消失。然而,与早期的报告相比,我们没有发现任何证据表明通过选择性地处理左耳刺激可以缓解这种灭绝。我们的结论是,先前在胼胝体切开术患者中,注意力改善左耳回忆的证明可能归因于使用了次优的二分法范式或残留的胼胝体连接,而不是代表注意力的真正影响。
{"title":"Dichotic-listening performance after complete callosotomy: No relief from left-ear extinction by selective attention","authors":"René Westerhausen ,&nbsp;Mara Fabri ,&nbsp;Markus Hausmann","doi":"10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2023.108627","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2023.108627","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>The surgical section of the corpus callosum (callosotomy) has been frequently demonstrated to result in a left-ear extinction in dichotic listening. That is, callosotomy patients report the left-ear stimulus below chance level, resulting in substantially enhanced right-ear advantage (REA) compared with controls. A small number of previous studies also suggest that callosotomy patients can overcome left-ear extinction when the instruction encourages to attend selectively to the left-ear stimulus. In the present case study, we re-examine the role of selective attention in dichotic listening in two patients with complete callosotomy and 40 age- and sex-matched controls. We used the standardised Bergen dichotic-listening paradigm which uses stop-consonant-vowel syllables as stimulus material and includes both a free-report and selective-attention condition. As was predicted, both patients showed a clear left-ear extinction. However, contrasting the earlier reports, we did not find any evidence for a relief from this extinction by selectively attending to the left-ear stimulus. We conclude that previous demonstrations of an attention-improved left-ear recall in callosotomy patients may be attributed to the use of suboptimal dichotic paradigms or residual callosal connectivity, rather than representing a genuine effect of attention.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":19279,"journal":{"name":"Neuropsychologia","volume":"188 ","pages":"Article 108627"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2023-09-09","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10138909","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Auditory cortical responses to abrupt lateralization shifts do not reflect the activity of hemifield-specific units involved in opponent coding of auditory space 听觉皮层对突然偏侧移位的反应不能反映参与听觉空间对手编码的半场特异性单元的活动
IF 2.6 3区 心理学 Q3 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2023-09-09 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2023.108629
Barkın İlhan , Saliha Kurt , Pekcan Ungan

Recent studies show that the classical model based on axonal delay-lines may not explain interaural time difference (ITD) based spatial coding in humans. Instead, a population-code model called “opponent channels model” (OCM) has been suggested. This model comprises two competing channels respectively for the two auditory hemifields, each with a sigmoidal tuning curve. Event-related potentials (ERPs) to ITD-changes are used in some studies to test the predictions of this model by considering the sounds before and after the change as adaptor and probe stimuli, respectively. It is assumed in these studies that the former stimulus causes adaptation of the neurons selective to its side, and that the ERP N1–P2 response to the ITD-change is the specific response of the neurons with selectivity to the side of probe sound. However, these ERP components are known as a global, non-specific acoustic change complex of cortical origin evoked by any change in the auditory environment. It probably does not genuinely reflect the activity of some stimulus-specific neuronal units that have escaped the refractory effect of the preceding adaptor, which means a violation of the crucial assumption in an adaptor-probe paradigm. To assess this viewpoint, we conducted two experiments. In the first one, we recorded ERPs to abrupt lateralization shifts of click trains having various pre- and post-shift ITDs within the physiological range of 600μs to +600μs. Magnitudes of the ERP components P1, N1, and P2 to these ITD-shifts did not comply with the additive behavior of partial probe responses presumed for an adaptor-probe paradigm, casting doubt on the accuracy of testing sensory coding models by using ERPs to abrupt lateralization changes. Findings of the second experiment, involving ERPs to conjoint outwards/transverse shift stimuli also supported this conclusion.

最近的研究表明,基于轴突延迟线的经典模型可能无法解释人类基于耳间时间差(ITD)的空间编码。相反,有人提出了一种称为“对手渠道模型”(OCM)的人口编码模型。该模型包括分别用于两个听觉半场的两个竞争通道,每个通道具有S形调谐曲线。ITD变化的事件相关电位(ERPs)在一些研究中被用来测试该模型的预测,方法是将变化前后的声音分别视为适配器和探针刺激。在这些研究中,假设前一种刺激导致神经元对其一侧的选择性适应,并且ERP N1–P2对ITD变化的反应是神经元对探测音一侧的选择性反应。然而,这些ERP成分被称为由听觉环境的任何变化引起的皮层起源的全局非特异性声学变化复合体。它可能并没有真正反映出一些刺激特异性神经元单元的活动,这些神经元单元逃脱了前一个适配器的难治性影响,这意味着违反了适配器-探针范式中的关键假设。为了评估这一观点,我们进行了两个实验。在第一个实验中,我们记录了在−600μs至+600μs的生理范围内,具有各种移位前和移位后ITD的点击序列的突然偏侧移位的ERPs。ERP成分P1、N1和P2对这些ITD变化的幅度不符合适配器探针范式假设的部分探针反应的加性行为,这让人怀疑通过使用ERP对突然的偏侧变化测试感觉编码模型的准确性。第二个实验的结果,涉及ERPs结合向外/横向移位刺激也支持这一结论。
{"title":"Auditory cortical responses to abrupt lateralization shifts do not reflect the activity of hemifield-specific units involved in opponent coding of auditory space","authors":"Barkın İlhan ,&nbsp;Saliha Kurt ,&nbsp;Pekcan Ungan","doi":"10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2023.108629","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2023.108629","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p><span>Recent studies show that the classical model based on axonal delay-lines may not explain interaural time difference<span> (ITD) based spatial coding in humans. Instead, a population-code model called “opponent channels model” (OCM) has been suggested. This model comprises two competing channels respectively for the two auditory hemifields, each with a sigmoidal tuning curve. Event-related potentials (ERPs) to ITD-changes are used in some studies to test the predictions of this model by considering the sounds before and after the change as adaptor and probe stimuli, respectively. It is assumed in these studies that the former stimulus causes adaptation of the neurons selective to its side, and that the ERP N1–P2 response to the ITD-change is the specific response of the neurons with selectivity to the side of probe sound. However, these ERP components are known as a global, non-specific acoustic change complex of cortical origin evoked by any change in the auditory environment. It probably does not genuinely reflect the activity of some stimulus-specific neuronal units that have escaped the refractory effect of the preceding adaptor, which means a violation of the crucial assumption in an adaptor-probe paradigm. To assess this viewpoint, we conducted two experiments. In the first one, we recorded ERPs to abrupt lateralization shifts of click trains having various pre- and post-shift ITDs within the physiological range of </span></span><span><math><mo>−</mo><mn>600</mn><mspace></mspace><mi>μ</mi><mi>s</mi></math></span> to <span><math><mo>+</mo><mn>600</mn><mspace></mspace><mi>μ</mi><mi>s</mi></math></span><span>. Magnitudes of the ERP components P1, N1, and P2 to these ITD-shifts did not comply with the additive behavior of partial probe responses presumed for an adaptor-probe paradigm, casting doubt on the accuracy of testing sensory coding models by using ERPs to abrupt lateralization changes. Findings of the second experiment, involving ERPs to conjoint outwards/transverse shift stimuli also supported this conclusion.</span></p></div>","PeriodicalId":19279,"journal":{"name":"Neuropsychologia","volume":"188 ","pages":"Article 108629"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2023-09-09","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10493469","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
A calculus of probability or belief? Neural underpinnings of social decision-making in a card game 概率演算还是信仰演算?纸牌游戏中社会决策的神经基础
IF 2.6 3区 心理学 Q3 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2023-09-09 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2023.108635
Yibei Chen , Sungbin Youk , Paula T. Wang , Paola Pinti , René Weber

For decades, the prefrontal cortex (PFC) has been the focus of social neuroscience research, specifically regarding its role in competitive social decision-making. However, the distinct contributions of PFC subregions when making strategic decisions involving multiple types of information (social, non-social, and mixed information) remain unclear. This study investigates decision-making strategies (pure probability calculation vs. mentalizing) and their neural representations using functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) data collected during a two-person card game. We observed individual differences in information processing strategy, indicating that some participants relied more on probability than others. Overall, the use of pure probability decreased over time in favor of other types of information (e.g., mixed information), with this effect being more pronounced within-round trials than across rounds. In the brain, (1) the lateral PFC activates when decisions are driven by probability calculations; (2) the right lateral PFC responds to trial difficulty; and (3) the anterior medial PFC is engaged when decision-making involves mentalizing. Furthermore, neural synchrony, which reflects the real-time interplay between individuals' cognitive processes, did not consistently contribute to correct decisions and fluctuated throughout the experiment, suggesting a hierarchical mentalizing mechanism at work.

几十年来,前额叶皮层(PFC)一直是社会神经科学研究的焦点,特别是关于其在竞争性社会决策中的作用。然而,在做出涉及多种类型信息(社会信息、非社会信息和混合信息)的战略决策时,PFC次区域的不同贡献仍不清楚。本研究使用在双人纸牌游戏中收集的功能性近红外光谱(fNIRS)数据,研究了决策策略(纯概率计算与心理化)及其神经表示。我们观察到信息处理策略的个体差异,表明一些参与者比其他参与者更依赖概率。总体而言,纯概率的使用随着时间的推移而减少,有利于其他类型的信息(例如,混合信息),这种影响在轮次试验中比跨轮次试验更明显。在大脑中,(1)当决策由概率计算驱动时,横向PFC激活;(2) 右侧PFC对试验难度作出反应;以及(3)当决策涉及心理化时,前-中PFC参与。此外,反映个体认知过程之间实时相互作用的神经同步性并没有始终如一地有助于做出正确的决定,并且在整个实验过程中有所波动,这表明存在一种分层的心理机制。
{"title":"A calculus of probability or belief? Neural underpinnings of social decision-making in a card game","authors":"Yibei Chen ,&nbsp;Sungbin Youk ,&nbsp;Paula T. Wang ,&nbsp;Paola Pinti ,&nbsp;René Weber","doi":"10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2023.108635","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2023.108635","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>For decades, the prefrontal cortex<span><span> (PFC) has been the focus of social neuroscience<span> research, specifically regarding its role in competitive social decision-making. However, the distinct contributions of PFC subregions when making strategic decisions involving multiple types of information (social, non-social, and mixed information) remain unclear. This study investigates decision-making strategies (pure probability calculation vs. mentalizing) and their neural representations using functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) data collected during a two-person card game. We observed individual differences in information processing strategy, indicating that some participants relied more on probability than others. Overall, the use of pure probability decreased over time in favor of other types of information (e.g., mixed information), with this effect being more pronounced within-round trials than across rounds. In the brain, (1) the lateral PFC activates when decisions are driven by probability calculations; (2) the right lateral PFC responds to trial difficulty; and (3) the anterior </span></span>medial PFC<span> is engaged when decision-making involves mentalizing. Furthermore, neural synchrony, which reflects the real-time interplay between individuals' cognitive processes, did not consistently contribute to correct decisions and fluctuated throughout the experiment, suggesting a hierarchical mentalizing mechanism at work.</span></span></p></div>","PeriodicalId":19279,"journal":{"name":"Neuropsychologia","volume":"188 ","pages":"Article 108635"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2023-09-09","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10493483","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Neural processing of observed performance-based errors and rewards in the context of friends and unfamiliar peers across adolescence 青少年时期在朋友和不熟悉的同龄人的背景下观察到的基于表现的错误和奖励的神经处理
IF 2.6 3区 心理学 Q3 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2023-09-09 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2023.108619
Iris J. Koele , Jorien van Hoorn , Ellen R.A. de Bruijn , Berna Güroğlu

Adolescence is characterized by changes in performance monitoring, whereby action outcomes are monitored to subsequently adapt behavior and optimize performance. Observation of performance-based outcomes (i.e., errors and rewards) received by others forms the basis of observational learning. Adolescence is also a period of increasing importance of peers, especially friends, and observing peers forms a crucial aspect of learning in the social context of the classroom. However, to our knowledge, no developmental fMRI studies have examined the neural mechanisms underlying observed performance monitoring of errors and rewards in the context of peers. The current fMRI study investigated the neural correlates of observing performance-based errors and rewards of peers in adolescents aged 9–16 years (N = 80). In the scanner, participants observed either their best friend or an unfamiliar peer play a shooting game resulting in performance-dependent rewards (based on hits) or losses (based on misses, i. e, errors), where outcomes affected both the player and the observing participant. Findings showed higher activation in the bilateral striatum and bilateral anterior insula when adolescents observed peers (i.e., best friend and unfamiliar peer) receive performance-based rewards compared to losses. This might reflect the heightened salience of observed reward processing in the peer context in adolescence. Our results further revealed lower activation in the left temporoparietal junction (TPJ) while adolescents observed the performance-based outcomes (rewards and losses) for their best friend than for an unfamiliar peer. Considering that observation of others’ performance-based errors and rewards forms the basis of observational learning, this study provides a crucial first step in understanding and potentially improving adolescent observational learning in the peer context.

青春期的特点是表现监控的变化,通过监控行动结果来调整行为并优化表现。观察他人获得的基于绩效的结果(即错误和奖励)构成了观察学习的基础。青春期也是同龄人,尤其是朋友越来越重要的时期,在课堂的社会背景下,观察同龄人是学习的一个重要方面。然而,据我们所知,没有任何发展性功能磁共振成像研究检查了在同龄人的背景下观察到的错误和奖励绩效监测的神经机制。目前的功能磁共振成像研究调查了9-16岁青少年(N=80)观察基于表现的错误和同伴奖励的神经相关性。在扫描仪中,参与者观察到他们最好的朋友或不熟悉的同伴玩射击游戏,结果会产生依赖于表现的奖励(基于命中率)或损失(基于失误,即失误),结果会影响玩家和观察参与者。研究结果显示,与损失相比,当青少年观察到同龄人(即最好的朋友和不熟悉的同龄人)获得基于绩效的奖励时,双侧纹状体和双侧前脑岛的激活率更高。这可能反映了在青春期同伴环境中观察到的奖励处理的显著性增强。我们的研究结果进一步揭示了左颞顶叶交界处(TPJ)的激活率较低,而青少年观察到他们最好的朋友比不熟悉的同龄人表现出的结果(奖励和损失)。考虑到对他人基于表现的错误和奖励的观察构成了观察学习的基础,本研究为理解和潜在地改善青少年在同伴环境中的观察学习提供了至关重要的第一步。
{"title":"Neural processing of observed performance-based errors and rewards in the context of friends and unfamiliar peers across adolescence","authors":"Iris J. Koele ,&nbsp;Jorien van Hoorn ,&nbsp;Ellen R.A. de Bruijn ,&nbsp;Berna Güroğlu","doi":"10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2023.108619","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2023.108619","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p><span><span>Adolescence is characterized by changes in performance monitoring, whereby action outcomes are monitored to subsequently adapt behavior and optimize performance. Observation of performance-based outcomes (i.e., errors and rewards) received by others forms the basis of </span>observational learning. Adolescence is also a period of increasing importance of peers, especially friends, and observing peers forms a crucial aspect of learning in the social context of the classroom. However, to our knowledge, no developmental fMRI studies have examined the neural mechanisms underlying observed performance monitoring of errors and rewards in the context of peers. The current fMRI study investigated the neural correlates of observing performance-based errors and rewards of peers in adolescents aged 9–16 years (</span><em>N</em><span> = 80). In the scanner, participants observed either their best friend or an unfamiliar peer play a shooting game resulting in performance-dependent rewards (based on hits) or losses (based on misses, i. e, errors), where outcomes affected both the player and the observing participant. Findings showed higher activation in the bilateral striatum and bilateral anterior insula<span> when adolescents observed peers (i.e., best friend and unfamiliar peer) receive performance-based rewards compared to losses. This might reflect the heightened salience of observed reward processing in the peer context in adolescence. Our results further revealed lower activation in the left temporoparietal junction (TPJ) while adolescents observed the performance-based outcomes (rewards and losses) for their best friend than for an unfamiliar peer. Considering that observation of others’ performance-based errors and rewards forms the basis of observational learning, this study provides a crucial first step in understanding and potentially improving adolescent observational learning in the peer context.</span></span></p></div>","PeriodicalId":19279,"journal":{"name":"Neuropsychologia","volume":"188 ","pages":"Article 108619"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2023-09-09","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10512548","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Neuropsychologia
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1