首页 > 最新文献

Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin最新文献

英文 中文
A Bias Toward Kindness Goals in Performance Feedback to Women (vs. Men). 对女性(与男性)的绩效反馈中对善良目标的偏见。
IF 4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2023-10-01 Epub Date: 2022-06-24 DOI: 10.1177/01461672221088402
Lily Jampol, Aneeta Rattan, Elizabeth Baily Wolf

While research has documented positivity biases in workplace feedback to women versus men, this phenomenon is not fully understood. We take a motivational perspective, theorizing that the gender stereotype of warmth shapes feedback givers' goals, amplifying the importance placed on kindness when giving critical feedback to a woman versus a man. We found support for this hypothesis in a survey of professionals giving real developmental feedback (Study 1, N = 4,842 raters evaluating N = 423 individuals) and five experiments with MBA students, lab participants, and managers (Studies 2-5, N = 1,589). Across studies, people prioritized the goal of kindness more when they gave, or anticipated giving, critical feedback to a woman versus a man. Studies 1, 3, and 5 suggest that this kindness bias relates to gendered positivity biases, and Studies 4a and 4b tested potential mechanisms and supported an indirect effect through warmth. We discuss implications for the study of motivation and workplace gender bias.

虽然研究表明,工作场所对女性和男性的反馈存在积极偏见,但这一现象尚未得到充分理解。我们从动机的角度出发,理论上认为,对温暖的性别刻板印象塑造了反馈者的目标,在对女性和男性进行批判性反馈时,强化了善良的重要性。我们在一项针对提供真实发展反馈的专业人士的调查中发现了对这一假设的支持(研究1,N=4842名评分者评估N=423名个人),以及对MBA学生、实验室参与者和管理人员的五项实验(研究2-5,N=1589)。在所有研究中,当人们向女性或男性提供或预期提供关键反馈时,他们更优先考虑善良的目标。研究1、3和5表明,这种善良偏见与性别积极偏见有关,研究4a和4b测试了潜在的机制,并支持通过温暖产生的间接影响。我们讨论了动机和工作场所性别偏见研究的意义。
{"title":"A Bias Toward Kindness Goals in Performance Feedback to Women (vs. Men).","authors":"Lily Jampol,&nbsp;Aneeta Rattan,&nbsp;Elizabeth Baily Wolf","doi":"10.1177/01461672221088402","DOIUrl":"10.1177/01461672221088402","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>While research has documented positivity biases in workplace feedback to women versus men, this phenomenon is not fully understood. We take a motivational perspective, theorizing that the gender stereotype of warmth shapes feedback givers' goals, amplifying the importance placed on kindness when giving critical feedback to a woman versus a man. We found support for this hypothesis in a survey of professionals giving real developmental feedback (Study 1, <i>N</i> = 4,842 raters evaluating <i>N</i> = 423 individuals) and five experiments with MBA students, lab participants, and managers (Studies 2-5, <i>N</i> = 1,589). Across studies, people prioritized the goal of kindness more when they gave, or anticipated giving, critical feedback to a woman versus a man. Studies 1, 3, and 5 suggest that this kindness bias relates to gendered positivity biases, and Studies 4a and 4b tested potential mechanisms and supported an indirect effect through warmth. We discuss implications for the study of motivation and workplace gender bias.</p>","PeriodicalId":19834,"journal":{"name":"Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin","volume":"49 10","pages":"1423-1438"},"PeriodicalIF":4.0,"publicationDate":"2023-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10495105","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Dealing With Increasing Negativity Toward Refugees: A Latent Growth Curve Study of Positive and Negative Intergroup Contact and Approach-Avoidance Tendencies. 应对对难民日益增长的消极情绪:积极和消极的群体间接触和回避倾向的潜在增长曲线研究。
IF 3.4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2023-10-01 Epub Date: 2022-07-20 DOI: 10.1177/01461672221110325
Sabahat C Bagci, Gülseli Baysu, Mustafa Tercan, Abbas Turnuklu

Despite increasing contact opportunities, prejudice toward refugees persists, especially in mass immigration contexts. We investigated changes in and associations between Turkish early adolescents' (N = 687, Mage = 11.11 years) positive and negative contact with Syrian refugees and their outgroup approach-avoidance tendencies over 15 months (three waves). Univariate growth curve models demonstrated a rise in outgroup negativity indicated by increasing negative contact and avoidance tendencies, and decreasing approach tendencies, while positive contact only slightly increased over time (nonsignificantly). Combined latent growth curve models showed that increasing positive contact buffered against increasing outgroup negativity in behavioral tendencies by predicting a less steep decline in approach and a less steep increase in avoidance. Increasing negative contact was positively associated with increasing outgroup negativity so that it predicted a more steep increase in avoidance. Findings underline the importance of early contact interventions that target the fast deterioration of positive intergroup interactions in increasingly hostile intergroup contexts.

尽管接触机会越来越多,但对难民的偏见依然存在,尤其是在大规模移民的情况下。我们调查了土耳其早期青少年(N=687,Mage=11.11岁)与叙利亚难民的积极和消极接触的变化及其在15个月内(三波)的群外回避倾向之间的关系。单变量增长曲线模型显示,群体外消极性增加,表现为消极接触和回避倾向增加,接近倾向减少,而积极接触仅随时间略有增加(无显著性)。组合潜在增长曲线模型表明,通过预测接近的下降幅度较小和回避的增加幅度较小,增加的积极接触可以缓冲行为倾向中增加的群体外消极情绪。消极接触的增加与群体外消极性的增加呈正相关,因此它预测了回避的急剧增加。研究结果强调了早期接触干预的重要性,该干预针对的是在日益敌对的群体间环境中积极的群体间互动的快速恶化。
{"title":"Dealing With Increasing Negativity Toward Refugees: A Latent Growth Curve Study of Positive and Negative Intergroup Contact and Approach-Avoidance Tendencies.","authors":"Sabahat C Bagci, Gülseli Baysu, Mustafa Tercan, Abbas Turnuklu","doi":"10.1177/01461672221110325","DOIUrl":"10.1177/01461672221110325","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Despite increasing contact opportunities, prejudice toward refugees persists, especially in mass immigration contexts. We investigated changes in and associations between Turkish early adolescents' (<i>N</i> = 687, <i>M</i><sub>age</sub> = 11.11 years) positive and negative contact with Syrian refugees and their outgroup approach-avoidance tendencies over 15 months (three waves). Univariate growth curve models demonstrated a rise in outgroup negativity indicated by increasing negative contact and avoidance tendencies, and decreasing approach tendencies, while positive contact only slightly increased over time (nonsignificantly). Combined latent growth curve models showed that increasing positive contact buffered against increasing outgroup negativity in behavioral tendencies by predicting a less steep decline in approach and a less steep increase in avoidance. Increasing negative contact was positively associated with increasing outgroup negativity so that it predicted a more steep increase in avoidance. Findings underline the importance of early contact interventions that target the fast deterioration of positive intergroup interactions in increasingly hostile intergroup contexts.</p>","PeriodicalId":19834,"journal":{"name":"Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin","volume":"49 10","pages":"1466-1478"},"PeriodicalIF":3.4,"publicationDate":"2023-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://ftp.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pub/pmc/oa_pdf/30/56/10.1177_01461672221110325.PMC10478330.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10220226","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
A Longitudinal Test of the Conservative-Liberal Well-Being Gap. 保守党和自由党幸福差距的纵向测试。
IF 4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2023-10-01 Epub Date: 2022-07-07 DOI: 10.1177/01461672221096587
Salvador Vargas Salfate, Sammyh S Khan, James H Liu, Homero Gil de Zúñiga
In this article, we test if conservatism predicts psychological well-being longitudinally. We based the study on previous findings showing that conservatives score higher on different measures of well-being, such as life satisfaction and happiness. Most explanations in the literature have assumed that conservatism antecedes well-being without considering the alternative—that well-being may predict conservatism. In Study 1, using multilevel cross-lagged panel models with a two-wave longitudinal sample consisting of data from 19 countries (N = 8,740), we found that conservatism did not predict well-being over time. We found similar results in Study 2 (N = 2,554), using random-intercept cross-lagged panel models with a four-wave longitudinal sample from Chile. We discuss the main implications of these results for the literature examining the association between conservatism and well-being.
在这篇文章中,我们测试了保守主义是否能纵向预测心理健康。我们的研究基于先前的发现,这些发现表明保守派在不同的幸福感指标上得分更高,比如生活满意度和幸福感。文献中的大多数解释都假设保守主义先于幸福,而没有考虑幸福可能预测保守主义的替代方案。在研究1中,我们使用多水平交叉滞后面板模型和由19个国家(N=8740)的数据组成的两波纵向样本,发现保守主义不能预测幸福感随时间的变化。我们在研究2中发现了类似的结果(N=2554),使用了智利四波纵向样本的随机截距交叉滞后面板模型。我们讨论了这些结果对研究保守主义和幸福感之间关系的文献的主要影响。
{"title":"A Longitudinal Test of the Conservative-Liberal Well-Being Gap.","authors":"Salvador Vargas Salfate,&nbsp;Sammyh S Khan,&nbsp;James H Liu,&nbsp;Homero Gil de Zúñiga","doi":"10.1177/01461672221096587","DOIUrl":"10.1177/01461672221096587","url":null,"abstract":"In this article, we test if conservatism predicts psychological well-being longitudinally. We based the study on previous findings showing that conservatives score higher on different measures of well-being, such as life satisfaction and happiness. Most explanations in the literature have assumed that conservatism antecedes well-being without considering the alternative—that well-being may predict conservatism. In Study 1, using multilevel cross-lagged panel models with a two-wave longitudinal sample consisting of data from 19 countries (N = 8,740), we found that conservatism did not predict well-being over time. We found similar results in Study 2 (N = 2,554), using random-intercept cross-lagged panel models with a four-wave longitudinal sample from Chile. We discuss the main implications of these results for the literature examining the association between conservatism and well-being.","PeriodicalId":19834,"journal":{"name":"Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin","volume":"49 10","pages":"1439-1453"},"PeriodicalIF":4.0,"publicationDate":"2023-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10478485","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
When Objective Ambivalence Predicts Subjective Ambivalence: An Affect-Cognition Matching Perspective. 当客观歧义预测主观歧义时:情感-认知匹配视角。
IF 4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2023-10-01 Epub Date: 2022-07-12 DOI: 10.1177/01461672221102015
Wei Jie Reiner Ng, Ya Hui Michelle See, Laura E Wallace

Understanding when people are likely to feel ambivalent is important, as ambivalence is associated with key attitude outcomes, such as attitude-behavior consistency. Interestingly, the presence of conflicting positive and negative reactions (objective ambivalence) is weakly related to feeling conflicted (subjective ambivalence). We tested a novel situation that can influence the correspondence between objective and subjective ambivalence: whether a message and a recipient's topic match in affective versus cognitive orientation. When a person encounters a message with an affective or cognitive match to the topic, conflicting reactions may be more accessible, increasing feelings of ambivalence. Across five studies, greater objective-subjective ambivalence correspondence occurred with an affective-cognitive match between message and topic orientation. Studies 4 and 5 also demonstrated that this primarily occurred when the message was counterattitudinal. This work contributes to the literature explaining the gap between measures of objective and subjective ambivalence as well as how messages can influence attitude strength properties.

了解人们何时可能感到矛盾很重要,因为矛盾与关键的态度结果有关,例如态度-行为的一致性。有趣的是,存在冲突的积极和消极反应(客观矛盾心理)与感到冲突(主观矛盾心理)的关系较弱。我们测试了一种可以影响客观矛盾心理和主观矛盾心理之间对应关系的新情况:信息和接收者的主题在情感取向和认知取向上是否匹配。当一个人遇到与主题情感或认知匹配的信息时,冲突的反应可能更容易获得,从而增加矛盾情绪。在五项研究中,信息和主题定向之间的情感认知匹配产生了更大的客观-主观矛盾对应关系。研究4和5还表明,这主要发生在信息是反态度的时候。这项工作有助于文献解释客观和主观矛盾心理测量之间的差距,以及信息如何影响态度强度属性。
{"title":"When Objective Ambivalence Predicts Subjective Ambivalence: An Affect-Cognition Matching Perspective.","authors":"Wei Jie Reiner Ng,&nbsp;Ya Hui Michelle See,&nbsp;Laura E Wallace","doi":"10.1177/01461672221102015","DOIUrl":"10.1177/01461672221102015","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Understanding when people are likely to feel ambivalent is important, as ambivalence is associated with key attitude outcomes, such as attitude-behavior consistency. Interestingly, the presence of conflicting positive and negative reactions (objective ambivalence) is weakly related to feeling conflicted (subjective ambivalence). We tested a novel situation that can influence the correspondence between objective and subjective ambivalence: whether a message and a recipient's topic match in affective versus cognitive orientation. When a person encounters a message with an affective or cognitive match to the topic, conflicting reactions may be more accessible, increasing feelings of ambivalence. Across five studies, greater objective-subjective ambivalence correspondence occurred with an affective-cognitive match between message and topic orientation. Studies 4 and 5 also demonstrated that this primarily occurred when the message was counterattitudinal. This work contributes to the literature explaining the gap between measures of objective and subjective ambivalence as well as how messages can influence attitude strength properties.</p>","PeriodicalId":19834,"journal":{"name":"Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin","volume":"49 10","pages":"1495-1510"},"PeriodicalIF":4.0,"publicationDate":"2023-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10131911","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Speak Up! Mistaken Beliefs About How Much to Talk in Conversations. 大声说!关于谈话内容的错误信念。
IF 4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2023-10-01 Epub Date: 2022-07-11 DOI: 10.1177/01461672221104927
Quinn Hirschi, Timothy D Wilson, Daniel T Gilbert

We hypothesized that people would exhibit a reticence bias, the incorrect belief that they will be more likable if they speak less than half the time in a conversation with a stranger, as well as halo ignorance, the belief that their speaking time should depend on their goal (e.g., to be liked vs. to be found interesting), when in fact, perceivers form global impressions of each other. In Studies 1 and 2, participants forecasted they should speak less than half the time when trying to be liked, but significantly more when trying to be interesting. In Study 3, we tested the accuracy of these forecasts by randomly assigning participants to speak for 30%, 40%, 50%, 60%, or 70% of the time in a dyadic conversation. Contrary to people's forecasts, they were more likable the more they spoke, and their partners formed global rather than differentiated impressions.

我们假设,人们会表现出沉默偏见,错误地认为,如果他们在与陌生人的对话中发言时间少于一半,他们会更讨人喜欢,还有光环无知,认为他们的发言时间应该取决于他们的目标(例如,被喜欢与被发现有趣),而事实上,感知者对彼此形成了全球印象。在研究1和2中,参与者预测,当他们试图被人喜欢时,他们说话的时间不到一半,但当他们试图变得有趣时,他们应该说得更多。在研究3中,我们通过随机分配参与者在二元对话中发言30%、40%、50%、60%或70%的时间来测试这些预测的准确性。与人们的预测相反,他们说话越多就越讨人喜欢,他们的伴侣形成了全球性而非差异化的印象。
{"title":"Speak Up! Mistaken Beliefs About How Much to Talk in Conversations.","authors":"Quinn Hirschi,&nbsp;Timothy D Wilson,&nbsp;Daniel T Gilbert","doi":"10.1177/01461672221104927","DOIUrl":"10.1177/01461672221104927","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>We hypothesized that people would exhibit a <i>reticence bias</i>, the incorrect belief that they will be more likable if they speak less than half the time in a conversation with a stranger, as well as <i>halo ignorance</i>, the belief that their speaking time should depend on their goal (e.g., to be liked vs. to be found interesting), when in fact, perceivers form global impressions of each other. In Studies 1 and 2, participants forecasted they should speak less than half the time when trying to be liked, but significantly more when trying to be interesting. In Study 3, we tested the accuracy of these forecasts by randomly assigning participants to speak for 30%, 40%, 50%, 60%, or 70% of the time in a dyadic conversation. Contrary to people's forecasts, they were more likable the more they spoke, and their partners formed global rather than differentiated impressions.</p>","PeriodicalId":19834,"journal":{"name":"Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin","volume":"49 10","pages":"1454-1465"},"PeriodicalIF":4.0,"publicationDate":"2023-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10179190","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 3
Do Agency and Communion Explain the Relationship Between Perceiver and Target Effects in Interpersonal Perception? A Meta-Analysis on Generalized Reciprocity. 代理和交流能解释人际感知中感知者和目标效应之间的关系吗?广义互易性的元分析。
IF 4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2023-10-01 Epub Date: 2022-07-12 DOI: 10.1177/01461672221107205
Titus Schauf, Michael Dufner, Steffen Nestler, Richard Rau

This meta-analysis examines generalized reciprocity, that is, the relationship between how people perceive others and how they are perceived by others. It tests the hypothesis that generalized reciprocity varies as a function of the content domain under investigation. Generalized reciprocity for attributes with primarily communal content (e.g., friendliness) was hypothesized to be more positive than generalized reciprocity for attributes with primarily agentic content (e.g., assertiveness). Sixty-four primary studies reporting correlations between perceiver and target effects with a total number of 17,561 participants were included in the analysis. Results of a multilevel meta-analytical random effects model showed that reciprocity correlations were slightly negative, but around zero, for primarily agentic attributes (r = -.05) and became more positive with increasing communal content (up to r = .18 for primarily communal attributes). Generalized reciprocity thus varied depending on the extent to which the regarded attribute is agentic versus communal.

这项荟萃分析考察了广义互惠,即人们如何感知他人和他人如何感知他们之间的关系。它检验了广义互易性作为所调查内容域的函数而变化的假设。假设主要具有公共内容(如友好)的属性的广义互惠比主要具有主体内容(如自信)的属性更积极。64项主要研究报告了感知者和目标效果之间的相关性,共有17561名参与者被纳入分析。多水平元分析随机效应模型的结果表明,对于主要的主体属性,互惠相关性略为负,但约为零(r=-0.05),并且随着公共内容的增加而变得更为正(对于主要的公共属性,可达r=.18)。因此,广义互惠因所考虑的属性是代理属性还是公共属性的程度而异。
{"title":"Do Agency and Communion Explain the Relationship Between Perceiver and Target Effects in Interpersonal Perception? A Meta-Analysis on Generalized Reciprocity.","authors":"Titus Schauf,&nbsp;Michael Dufner,&nbsp;Steffen Nestler,&nbsp;Richard Rau","doi":"10.1177/01461672221107205","DOIUrl":"10.1177/01461672221107205","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>This meta-analysis examines generalized reciprocity, that is, the relationship between how people perceive others and how they are perceived by others. It tests the hypothesis that generalized reciprocity varies as a function of the content domain under investigation. Generalized reciprocity for attributes with primarily communal content (e.g., friendliness) was hypothesized to be more positive than generalized reciprocity for attributes with primarily agentic content (e.g., assertiveness). Sixty-four primary studies reporting correlations between perceiver and target effects with a total number of 17,561 participants were included in the analysis. Results of a multilevel meta-analytical random effects model showed that reciprocity correlations were slightly negative, but around zero, for primarily agentic attributes (<i>r</i> = -.05) and became more positive with increasing communal content (up to <i>r</i> = .18 for primarily communal attributes). Generalized reciprocity thus varied depending on the extent to which the regarded attribute is agentic versus communal.</p>","PeriodicalId":19834,"journal":{"name":"Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin","volume":"49 10","pages":"1479-1494"},"PeriodicalIF":4.0,"publicationDate":"2023-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10140550","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Diversity of Group Memberships Predicts Well-Being: Cross-Sectional and Longitudinal Evidence. 群体成员的多样性预测幸福:横截面和纵向证据。
IF 4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2023-09-30 DOI: 10.1177/01461672231202278
Sarah J Charles, Clifford Stevenson, Juliet R H Wakefield, Emanuele Fino

Groups have their health and well-being impacted by satisfying their members' needs and providing resources to help cope with threats. Multiple group memberships serve to accumulate these benefits and also provide resilience to the effects of group loss. However, the additional well-being benefits of belonging to multiple different types of group remain to be determined. In a preregistered cross-sectional survey in Nottingham, England (Study 1, N = 328), we found that group-type diversity predicted well-being and that this effect was fully serially mediated by increased creative self-efficacy, then reduced loneliness. To confirm our hypothesis in a more robust sample we conducted longitudinal analyses on the English Longitudinal Study of Aging (ELSA) dataset (Study 2, N = 5,838) finding that group-type diversity at time one (T1) predicted well-being at T2 (4 years later), even when accounting for wellbeing and loneliness at T1. We discuss the implications for enhancing group-based health interventions.

团体通过满足成员的需求和提供资源来帮助应对威胁,从而影响他们的健康和福祉。多个团体成员资格有助于积累这些好处,并为团体损失的影响提供弹性。然而,属于多种不同类型群体的额外福利仍有待确定。在英国诺丁汉进行的一项预先登记的横断面调查中(研究1,N=328),我们发现群体类型的多样性可以预测幸福感,而这种影响完全是由创造力自我效能感的增加和孤独感的减少所连续介导的。为了在一个更稳健的样本中证实我们的假设,我们对英国老龄化纵向研究(ELSA)数据集(研究2,N=5838)进行了纵向分析,发现时间一(T1)的群体类型多样性预测了T2(4年后)的幸福感,即使考虑了T1的幸福感和孤独感。我们讨论了加强基于群体的健康干预的影响。
{"title":"Diversity of Group Memberships Predicts Well-Being: Cross-Sectional and Longitudinal Evidence.","authors":"Sarah J Charles, Clifford Stevenson, Juliet R H Wakefield, Emanuele Fino","doi":"10.1177/01461672231202278","DOIUrl":"10.1177/01461672231202278","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Groups have their health and well-being impacted by satisfying their members' needs and providing resources to help cope with threats. Multiple group memberships serve to accumulate these benefits and also provide resilience to the effects of group loss. However, the additional well-being benefits of belonging to multiple <i>different types of group</i> remain to be determined. In a preregistered cross-sectional survey in Nottingham, England (Study 1, <i>N</i> = 328), we found that group-type diversity predicted well-being and that this effect was fully serially mediated by increased creative self-efficacy, then reduced loneliness. To confirm our hypothesis in a more robust sample we conducted longitudinal analyses on the English Longitudinal Study of Aging (ELSA) dataset (Study 2, <i>N</i> = 5,838) finding that group-type diversity at time one (T1) predicted well-being at T2 (4 years later), even when accounting for wellbeing and loneliness at T1. We discuss the implications for enhancing group-based health interventions.</p>","PeriodicalId":19834,"journal":{"name":"Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin","volume":" ","pages":"1461672231202278"},"PeriodicalIF":4.0,"publicationDate":"2023-09-30","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41176868","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Revisiting the Relation Between Steroid Hormones and Unethicality in an Exploratory, Longitudinal Study With Female Participants. 在一项针对女性参与者的探索性纵向研究中,重新审视类固醇激素与不道德之间的关系。
IF 4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2023-09-25 DOI: 10.1177/01461672231199961
Julia Stern, Christoph Schild, Ingo Zettler

Research on the relation between hormones and unethical behaviors and tendencies has provided mixed results, hindering the understanding of the potential biological regulation of unethical behaviors and tendencies. We conducted an exploratory, longitudinal study (N = 257 women) allowing to estimate relations between, on the one hand, steroid hormones (testosterone, cortisol, estradiol, and progesterone) and conception probability and, on the other hand, a broad variety of measures related to unethicality (self-reported personality variables, cheating in committed relationships, self-serving economic dishonesty in a behavioral task, namely, the mind game). Contrary to theoretical assumptions of and results from some previous studies, we find no consistent relation between hormones and unethical behavior or tendencies in the majority of analyses. Yet, some small, exploratory associations emerged that call for (preregistered) replications, before more firm conclusions can be made.

对激素与不道德行为和倾向之间关系的研究结果喜忧参半,阻碍了对不道德行为与倾向的潜在生物学调控的理解。我们进行了一项探索性的纵向研究(N=257名女性),一方面估计类固醇激素(睾酮、皮质醇、雌二醇和孕酮)与受孕概率之间的关系,另一方面,与不道德有关的各种各样的衡量标准(自我报告的人格变量、承诺关系中的欺骗、行为任务中的自私经济不诚实,即心理游戏)。与之前一些研究的理论假设和结果相反,我们在大多数分析中发现激素与不道德行为或倾向之间没有一致的关系。然而,在得出更确切的结论之前,出现了一些小型的探索性协会,要求(预先注册)复制。
{"title":"Revisiting the Relation Between Steroid Hormones and Unethicality in an Exploratory, Longitudinal Study With Female Participants.","authors":"Julia Stern, Christoph Schild, Ingo Zettler","doi":"10.1177/01461672231199961","DOIUrl":"10.1177/01461672231199961","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Research on the relation between hormones and unethical behaviors and tendencies has provided mixed results, hindering the understanding of the potential biological regulation of unethical behaviors and tendencies. We conducted an exploratory, longitudinal study (<i>N</i> = 257 women) allowing to estimate relations between, on the one hand, steroid hormones (testosterone, cortisol, estradiol, and progesterone) and conception probability and, on the other hand, a broad variety of measures related to unethicality (self-reported personality variables, cheating in committed relationships, self-serving economic dishonesty in a behavioral task, namely, the mind game). Contrary to theoretical assumptions of and results from some previous studies, we find no consistent relation between hormones and unethical behavior or tendencies in the majority of analyses. Yet, some small, exploratory associations emerged that call for (preregistered) replications, before more firm conclusions can be made.</p>","PeriodicalId":19834,"journal":{"name":"Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin","volume":" ","pages":"1461672231199961"},"PeriodicalIF":4.0,"publicationDate":"2023-09-25","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41161603","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Give Me a Straight Answer: Response Ambiguity Diminishes Likability. 给我一个直截了当的答案:反应模棱两可会降低好感度。
IF 4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2023-09-22 DOI: 10.1177/01461672231199161
Deming Wang, Ignazio Ziano

Across nine experiments (eight preregistered) involving Western and Asian samples, we showed that people providing ambiguous (vs. specific) responses to questions in various social scenarios are seen as less likable. This is because, depending on the social context, response ambiguity may be interpreted as a way to conceal the truth and as a sign of social disinterest. Consequently, people reported lower inclination to befriend or date individuals who appeared to provide ambiguous responses. We also identified situations in which response ambiguity does not harm likability, such as when the questions are sensitive and the responder may need to "soften the blow." A final exploratory study showed that response ambiguity also impacts personality perceptions-individuals providing ambiguous responses are judged as less warm, less extraverted, less gullible, and more cautious. We discuss theoretical implications for the language psychology and person perception literatures and practical implications for impression management and formation.

在涉及西方和亚洲样本的九个实验(八个预先注册的)中,我们发现,在各种社会场景中,对问题做出模糊(相对于具体)回答的人被认为不太讨人喜欢。这是因为,根据社会背景,反应模糊可能被解释为一种隐瞒真相的方式,也是社会不感兴趣的表现。因此,人们报告说,与那些似乎提供模棱两可回答的人交朋友或约会的倾向较低。我们还确定了回答模糊不会损害讨人喜欢的情况,例如当问题很敏感,回答者可能需要“减轻打击”时。一项最终的探索性研究表明,回答模糊也会影响人格感知。提供模糊回答的人被认为不那么热情、不那么外向、不那么容易上当、更谨慎。我们讨论了语言心理学和人称知觉文献的理论意义,以及印象管理和形成的实践意义。
{"title":"Give Me a Straight Answer: Response Ambiguity Diminishes Likability.","authors":"Deming Wang, Ignazio Ziano","doi":"10.1177/01461672231199161","DOIUrl":"10.1177/01461672231199161","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Across nine experiments (eight preregistered) involving Western and Asian samples, we showed that people providing ambiguous (vs. specific) responses to questions in various social scenarios are seen as less likable. This is because, depending on the social context, response ambiguity may be interpreted as a way to conceal the truth and as a sign of social disinterest. Consequently, people reported lower inclination to befriend or date individuals who appeared to provide ambiguous responses. We also identified situations in which response ambiguity does not harm likability, such as when the questions are sensitive and the responder may need to \"soften the blow.\" A final exploratory study showed that response ambiguity also impacts personality perceptions-individuals providing ambiguous responses are judged as less warm, less extraverted, less gullible, and more cautious. We discuss theoretical implications for the language psychology and person perception literatures and practical implications for impression management and formation.</p>","PeriodicalId":19834,"journal":{"name":"Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin","volume":" ","pages":"1461672231199161"},"PeriodicalIF":4.0,"publicationDate":"2023-09-22","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41168046","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Empathic Conservatives and Moralizing Liberals: Political Intergroup Empathy Varies by Political Ideology and Is Explained by Moral Judgment. 移情的保守派和道德化的自由派:政治群体间的移情因政治意识形态而异,并可通过道德判断加以解释》(Political Intergroup Empathy Varies by Political Ideology and Is Explained by Moral Judgment.
IF 4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2023-09-15 DOI: 10.1177/01461672231198001
James P Casey, Eric J Vanman, Fiona Kate Barlow

Empathy has the potential to bridge political divides. Here, we examine barriers to cross-party empathy and explore when and why these differ for liberals and conservatives. In four studies, U.S. and U.K. participants (total N = 4,737) read hypothetical scenarios and extended less empathy to suffering political opponents than allies or neutral targets. These effects were strongly shown by liberals but were weaker among conservatives, such that conservatives consistently showed more empathy to liberals than liberals showed to conservatives. This asymmetry was partly explained by liberals' harsher moral judgments of outgroup members (Studies 1-4) and the fact that liberals saw conservatives as more harmful than conservatives saw liberals (Studies 3 and 4). The asymmetry persisted across changes in the U.S. government and was not explained by perceptions of political power (Studies 3 and 4). Implications and future directions are discussed.

移情具有弥合政治分歧的潜力。在此,我们研究了跨党派共情的障碍,并探讨了自由派和保守派在何时以及为何存在这些障碍。在四项研究中,美国和英国的参与者(总人数 = 4737 人)在阅读假设情景时,对遭受苦难的政治对手的移情程度低于对盟友或中立目标的移情程度。这些效应在自由主义者中表现得很明显,但在保守主义者中则较弱,因此保守主义者对自由主义者表现出的移情一直多于自由主义者对保守主义者表现出的移情。造成这种不对称现象的部分原因是,自由主义者对外群体成员的道德判断更为苛刻(研究 1-4),以及自由主义者眼中的保守主义者比保守主义者眼中的自由主义者更有害(研究 3 和 4)。这种不对称性在美国政府发生变化时依然存在,政治权力观念无法解释这种不对称性(研究 3 和 4)。本文讨论了研究的意义和未来发展方向。
{"title":"Empathic Conservatives and Moralizing Liberals: Political Intergroup Empathy Varies by Political Ideology and Is Explained by Moral Judgment.","authors":"James P Casey, Eric J Vanman, Fiona Kate Barlow","doi":"10.1177/01461672231198001","DOIUrl":"10.1177/01461672231198001","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Empathy has the potential to bridge political divides. Here, we examine barriers to cross-party empathy and explore when and why these differ for liberals and conservatives. In four studies, U.S. and U.K. participants (total <i>N</i> = 4,737) read hypothetical scenarios and extended less empathy to suffering political opponents than allies or neutral targets. These effects were strongly shown by liberals but were weaker among conservatives, such that conservatives consistently showed more empathy to liberals than liberals showed to conservatives. This asymmetry was partly explained by liberals' harsher moral judgments of outgroup members (Studies 1-4) and the fact that liberals saw conservatives as more harmful than conservatives saw liberals (Studies 3 and 4). The asymmetry persisted across changes in the U.S. government and was not explained by perceptions of political power (Studies 3 and 4). Implications and future directions are discussed.</p>","PeriodicalId":19834,"journal":{"name":"Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin","volume":" ","pages":"1461672231198001"},"PeriodicalIF":4.0,"publicationDate":"2023-09-15","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10247595","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1