首页 > 最新文献

Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin最新文献

英文 中文
Mental Contrasting Strategies Promote the Pursuit of Difficult Goals: Japanese Cultural Context. 心理对比策略促进追求困难目标:日本文化背景。
IF 4 2区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-05-10 DOI: 10.1177/01461672241247481
Miki Toyama, Masato Nagamine, Li Tang

We examined whether mental contrasting inhibits the pursuit of difficult goals in an Eastern culture-Japan-rooted in self-improvement. Our pilot study found that, compared with American participants, Japanese participants did not perceive a difficult situation as a cue to abandon their goal and pursue alternative objectives. Studies 1a-1c found that mental contrasting encouraged Japanese participants to pursue difficult goals. When Japanese participants perceived their own goals as unattainable, they were more likely to pursue these goals if they mentally contrasted their desired future with the inhibiting reality than if they simply imagined their desired future. Study 2 showed that mental contrasting encouraged Japanese (but not American) participants to pursue difficult goals. Study 3 evidenced the causal effect of beliefs about difficulties on the impact of mental contrasting on motivation to pursue difficult goals. Culturally formed beliefs about difficulties underlie the effect of mental contrasting on difficult goal pursuit.

我们研究了在以自我完善为根基的东方文化--日本,心理对比是否会抑制对困难目标的追求。我们的试验性研究发现,与美国受试者相比,日本受试者不会将困难情况视为放弃目标、追求其他目标的线索。研究 1a-1c 发现,心理对比鼓励日本参与者追求困难目标。当日本受试者认为自己的目标无法实现时,如果他们在心理上将自己期望的未来与抑制性现实进行对比,他们就更有可能追求这些目标,而不是仅仅想象自己期望的未来。研究 2 表明,心理对比鼓励日本人(而不是美国人)追求困难的目标。研究 3 证明了困难信念对心理对比对追求困难目标的动机的影响的因果效应。文化形成的困难信念是心理对比对追求困难目标的影响的基础。
{"title":"Mental Contrasting Strategies Promote the Pursuit of Difficult Goals: Japanese Cultural Context.","authors":"Miki Toyama, Masato Nagamine, Li Tang","doi":"10.1177/01461672241247481","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/01461672241247481","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>We examined whether mental contrasting inhibits the pursuit of difficult goals in an Eastern culture-Japan-rooted in self-improvement. Our pilot study found that, compared with American participants, Japanese participants did not perceive a difficult situation as a cue to abandon their goal and pursue alternative objectives. Studies 1a-1c found that mental contrasting encouraged Japanese participants to pursue difficult goals. When Japanese participants perceived their own goals as unattainable, they were more likely to pursue these goals if they mentally contrasted their desired future with the inhibiting reality than if they simply imagined their desired future. Study 2 showed that mental contrasting encouraged Japanese (but not American) participants to pursue difficult goals. Study 3 evidenced the causal effect of beliefs about difficulties on the impact of mental contrasting on motivation to pursue difficult goals. Culturally formed beliefs about difficulties underlie the effect of mental contrasting on difficult goal pursuit.</p>","PeriodicalId":19834,"journal":{"name":"Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":4.0,"publicationDate":"2024-05-10","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140898939","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Global Ecology and Geography of Gender Equality. 全球生态学与性别平等地理学。
IF 4 2区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-05-10 DOI: 10.1177/01461672241237383
Evert Van de Vliert, Esther S Kluwer

Proximal socio-economic drivers of gender equality tend to obscure its remote ecological origins. General systems theory predicts that the greater annual variability in daylength, temperature, and daily precipitation at higher latitudes requires greater psychosocial flexibility. We extend this prediction to gender equality as a likely consequence. Accordingly, for 87 pre-industrial societies after 1500 CE, we find more gender equality in more variable habitats, and that this link is mediated by greater subsistence flexibility-foraging rather than raising plants and animals. Mutatis mutandis, these ecological predictors of global gender equality replicate in 175 modern countries after 2000 CE. Gender equality was, and still is, lowest around the Equator, higher toward the North and South Poles, and invariant in east-west direction. The geographical positioning of gender equality in pre-industrial times can predict over 40% of the opposite north-south gradients of gender equality in the opposite Northern and Southern Hemispheres today.

性别平等的社会经济驱动因素往往会掩盖其遥远的生态起源。一般系统理论预测,在高纬度地区,昼长、气温和日降水量的年变异性更大,这需要更大的社会心理灵活性。我们将这一预测延伸到性别平等上,认为这可能是一个结果。因此,在公元 1500 年后的 87 个前工业化社会中,我们发现在更多变的栖息地中性别更平等,而这种联系是由更大的生存灵活性--觅食而非饲养动植物--所促成的。同样,这些预测全球性别平等的生态因素在公元 2000 年后的 175 个现代国家也得到了验证。赤道附近的性别平等程度最低,南北两极的性别平等程度较高,东西方向的性别平等程度不变。前工业化时代性别平等的地理位置可以预测今天南北半球性别平等南北梯度相反情况的 40%以上。
{"title":"Global Ecology and Geography of Gender Equality.","authors":"Evert Van de Vliert, Esther S Kluwer","doi":"10.1177/01461672241237383","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/01461672241237383","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Proximal socio-economic drivers of gender equality tend to obscure its remote ecological origins. General systems theory predicts that the greater annual variability in daylength, temperature, and daily precipitation at higher latitudes requires greater psychosocial flexibility. We extend this prediction to gender equality as a likely consequence. Accordingly, for 87 pre-industrial societies after 1500 CE, we find more gender equality in more variable habitats, and that this link is mediated by greater subsistence flexibility-foraging rather than raising plants and animals. Mutatis mutandis, these ecological predictors of global gender equality replicate in 175 modern countries after 2000 CE. Gender equality was, and still is, lowest around the Equator, higher toward the North and South Poles, and invariant in east-west direction. The geographical positioning of gender equality in pre-industrial times can predict over 40% of the opposite north-south gradients of gender equality in the opposite Northern and Southern Hemispheres today.</p>","PeriodicalId":19834,"journal":{"name":"Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":4.0,"publicationDate":"2024-05-10","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140898936","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Mobilize Is a Verb: The Use of Verbs and Concrete Language Is Associated With Authors' and Readers' Perceptions of a Text's Action Orientation and Persuasiveness. 动员是一个动词:动词和具体语言的使用与作者和读者对文本的行动导向和说服力的看法有关。
IF 4 2区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-05-08 DOI: 10.1177/01461672241238418
Magdalena Formanowicz, Marta Beneda, Marta Witkowska, Jan Nikadon, Caterina Suitner

In three studies, we investigated the role of linguistic features characterizing texts aiming to mobilize others. In Study 1 (N = 728), participants produced a leaflet either mobilizing others to engage in an action or expressing their thoughts about that action, and evaluated how action-oriented their text was. Mobilizing texts included more verbs and concrete words, and the presence of these linguistic characteristics was positively linked to participants' evaluations of their messages as action-oriented. In Studies 2 and 3 (N = 557 and N = 556), independent groups of participants evaluated texts produced in Study 1. Readers' perceptions of texts as action-oriented were associated with the same linguistic features as in Study 1 and further positively linked to perceived message effectiveness (Study 2) and behavioral intention (Study 3). The studies reveal how encoding and decoding of verbs and concrete words serve as distinct persuasive tools in calls to action.

在三项研究中,我们调查了旨在动员他人的文本的语言特点的作用。在研究 1(N = 728)中,参与者制作了一份传单,或动员他人参与行动,或表达自己对该行动的想法,并对其文本的行动导向性进行评价。动员型文本包含更多的动词和具体词汇,这些语言特点的存在与参与者对其信息的行动导向性评价呈正相关。在研究 2 和研究 3 中(人数分别为 557 人和 556 人),独立的参与者小组对研究 1 中制作的文本进行了评估。读者对文本以行动为导向的看法与研究 1 中的语言特点相同,并进一步与感知到的信息有效性(研究 2)和行为意向(研究 3)正相关。这些研究揭示了动词和具体词语的编码和解码是如何在行动号召中发挥独特的说服作用的。
{"title":"<i>Mobilize</i> Is a Verb: The Use of Verbs and Concrete Language Is Associated With Authors' and Readers' Perceptions of a Text's Action Orientation and Persuasiveness.","authors":"Magdalena Formanowicz, Marta Beneda, Marta Witkowska, Jan Nikadon, Caterina Suitner","doi":"10.1177/01461672241238418","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/01461672241238418","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>In three studies, we investigated the role of linguistic features characterizing texts aiming to mobilize others. In Study 1 (<i>N</i> = 728), participants produced a leaflet either mobilizing others to engage in an action or expressing their thoughts about that action, and evaluated how action-oriented their text was. Mobilizing texts included more verbs and concrete words, and the presence of these linguistic characteristics was positively linked to participants' evaluations of their messages as action-oriented. In Studies 2 and 3 (<i>N</i> = 557 and <i>N</i> = 556), independent groups of participants evaluated texts produced in Study 1. Readers' perceptions of texts as action-oriented were associated with the same linguistic features as in Study 1 and further positively linked to perceived message effectiveness (Study 2) and behavioral intention (Study 3). The studies reveal how encoding and decoding of verbs and concrete words serve as distinct persuasive tools in calls to action.</p>","PeriodicalId":19834,"journal":{"name":"Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":4.0,"publicationDate":"2024-05-08","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140877035","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
On the Defensive: Identity, Language, and Partisan Reactions to Political Scandal. 防卫:身份、语言和党派对政治丑闻的反应》(On the Defensive: Identity, Language, and Partisan Reactions to Political Scandal.
IF 4 2区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-05-06 DOI: 10.1177/01461672241247084
Pierce D Ekstrom, Marti Hope Gonzales, Allison L Williams, Elliot Weiner, Rafael Aguilera

We investigated how individuals judge politicians embroiled in scandal. Drawing on social identity and realistic group conflict theory, we predicted that beyond an overall ingroup bias, partisans would be particularly forgiving of in-party politicians who denied or justified their misconduct rather than apologize for it. By insisting that they did nothing wrong, these politicians defend the public image of their party and signal their commitment to partisan goals. We find qualified support for this prediction across three experiments. Participants did not respond negatively to in-party politicians who apologized but did react more positively to those who denied or justified wrongdoing (relative to silence). These accounts worked only for in-party politicians and were more effective for those whose misconduct furthered their party's agenda or whose seat was high-status or pivotal for party goals. In intergroup contexts like politics, people may accept explanations for misconduct that they would otherwise find offensive.

我们研究了个人如何评判陷入丑闻的政治家。根据社会认同和现实群体冲突理论,我们预测,除了整体上的内群体偏见外,党派人士会特别宽容那些否认或为自己的不当行为辩解而不是道歉的党内政治家。通过坚称自己没有做错,这些政治家捍卫了本党的公众形象,并表明了他们对党派目标的承诺。我们在三个实验中都发现了对这一预测的支持。参与者没有对道歉的党内政治家做出负面反应,但对那些否认或辩解错误行为的政治家(相对于沉默)做出了更积极的反应。这些说法只对党内政治家有效,对那些不当行为促进了党的议程或其席位地位较高或对党的目标至关重要的政治家更为有效。在像政治这样的群体间环境中,人们可能会接受对不当行为的解释,否则他们会认为这些解释令人反感。
{"title":"On the Defensive: Identity, Language, and Partisan Reactions to Political Scandal.","authors":"Pierce D Ekstrom, Marti Hope Gonzales, Allison L Williams, Elliot Weiner, Rafael Aguilera","doi":"10.1177/01461672241247084","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/01461672241247084","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>We investigated how individuals judge politicians embroiled in scandal. Drawing on social identity and realistic group conflict theory, we predicted that beyond an overall ingroup bias, partisans would be particularly forgiving of in-party politicians who denied or justified their misconduct rather than apologize for it. By insisting that they did nothing wrong, these politicians defend the public image of their party and signal their commitment to partisan goals. We find qualified support for this prediction across three experiments. Participants did not respond <i>negatively</i> to in-party politicians who apologized but did react more positively to those who denied or justified wrongdoing (relative to silence). These accounts worked only for in-party politicians and were more effective for those whose misconduct furthered their party's agenda or whose seat was high-status or pivotal for party goals. In intergroup contexts like politics, people may accept explanations for misconduct that they would otherwise find offensive.</p>","PeriodicalId":19834,"journal":{"name":"Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":4.0,"publicationDate":"2024-05-06","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140870804","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Examining the Contribution of Physical Cues for Same- and Cross-Race Face Individuation. 研究物理线索对同种族和跨种族人脸识别的贡献
IF 4 2区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-05-01 Epub Date: 2023-01-03 DOI: 10.1177/01461672221141510
Joshua Correll, Debbie S Ma, David A Kenny, Tomás A Palma

Face individuation involves sensitivity to physical characteristics that provide information about identity. We examined whether Black and White American faces differ in terms of individuating information, and whether Black and White perceivers differentially weight information when judging same-race and cross-race faces. Study 1 analyzed 20 structural metrics (e.g., eye width, nose length) of 158 Black and White faces to determine which differentiate faces within each group. High-utility metrics (e.g., nose length, eye height, chin length) differentiated faces of both groups, low-utility metrics (e.g., face width, eye width, face length) offered less individuating information. Study 2 (N = 4,510) explored Black and White participants' sensitivity to variation on structural metrics using similarity ratings. High-utility metrics affected perceived dissimilarity more than low-utility metrics. This relationship was non-significantly stronger for same-race faces rather than cross-race faces. Perceivers also relied more on features that were racially stereotypic of the faces they were rating.

人脸识别涉及对提供身份信息的物理特征的敏感性。我们研究了美国黑人和白人面孔在个体化信息方面是否存在差异,以及黑人和白人感知者在判断同种族面孔和跨种族面孔时对信息的权重是否不同。研究 1 分析了 158 张黑人和白人面孔的 20 个结构指标(如眼睛宽度、鼻子长度),以确定哪些指标能区分每个群体中的面孔。高效用指标(如鼻长、眼高、下巴长)可区分两组人脸,低效用指标(如脸宽、眼宽、脸长)提供的个体化信息较少。研究 2(N=4,510)使用相似度评分法探讨了黑人和白人参与者对结构指标变化的敏感度。与低效用指标相比,高效用指标对感知相似性的影响更大。这种关系在同种族面孔上比跨种族面孔上更为明显。感知者也更依赖于他们所评定的人脸的种族刻板印象特征。
{"title":"Examining the Contribution of Physical Cues for Same- and Cross-Race Face Individuation.","authors":"Joshua Correll, Debbie S Ma, David A Kenny, Tomás A Palma","doi":"10.1177/01461672221141510","DOIUrl":"10.1177/01461672221141510","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Face individuation involves sensitivity to physical characteristics that provide information about identity. We examined whether Black and White American faces differ in terms of individuating information, and whether Black and White perceivers differentially weight information when judging same-race and cross-race faces. Study 1 analyzed 20 structural metrics (e.g., eye width, nose length) of 158 Black and White faces to determine which differentiate faces within each group. High-utility metrics (e.g., nose length, eye height, chin length) differentiated faces of both groups, low-utility metrics (e.g., face width, eye width, face length) offered less individuating information. Study 2 (<i>N</i> = 4,510) explored Black and White participants' sensitivity to variation on structural metrics using similarity ratings. High-utility metrics affected perceived dissimilarity more than low-utility metrics. This relationship was non-significantly stronger for same-race faces rather than cross-race faces. Perceivers also relied more on features that were racially stereotypic of the faces they were rating.</p>","PeriodicalId":19834,"journal":{"name":"Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":4.0,"publicationDate":"2024-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10826327","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
How Much Is Enough? The Relationship Between Prosocial Effort and Moral Character Judgments. 多少才算足够?亲社会努力与道德品格判断之间的关系》(The Relationship Between Prosocial Effort and Moral Character Judgments.
IF 4 2区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-05-01 Epub Date: 2022-12-28 DOI: 10.1177/01461672221135954
Zachariah Berry, Brian J Lucas

The amount of effort required to bring about a prosocial outcome can vary from low-handing a stranger the wallet she just dropped-to high-spending days tracking down the owner of a lost wallet. The goal of the current research is to characterize the relationship between prosocial effort and moral character judgments. Does more prosocial effort always lead to rosier moral character judgments? Across four studies (N = 1,658), we find that moral character judgments increase with prosocial effort to a point and then plateau. We find evidence that this pattern is produced, in part, by descriptive and prescriptive norms: exceeding descriptive norms increases moral character judgments, but exceeding prescriptive norms has the opposite effect, which leads to a tapering off of moral character judgments at higher levels of effort.

实现亲社会结果所需的努力程度各不相同,小到把陌生人刚掉的钱包交给她,大到花几天时间寻找丢失钱包的失主。当前研究的目标是描述亲社会努力与道德品格判断之间的关系。更多的亲社会努力是否总是会导致更美好的道德品质判断?通过四项研究(N = 1,658),我们发现道德品格判断会随着亲社会努力的增加而增加,达到一定程度后会趋于平稳。我们发现有证据表明,这种模式部分是由描述性规范和规定性规范产生的:超过描述性规范会增加道德品格判断,但超过规定性规范则会产生相反的效果,从而导致道德品格判断在较高的努力水平上逐渐减弱。
{"title":"How Much Is Enough? The Relationship Between Prosocial Effort and Moral Character Judgments.","authors":"Zachariah Berry, Brian J Lucas","doi":"10.1177/01461672221135954","DOIUrl":"10.1177/01461672221135954","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The amount of effort required to bring about a prosocial outcome can vary from low-handing a stranger the wallet she just dropped-to high-spending days tracking down the owner of a lost wallet. The goal of the current research is to characterize the relationship between prosocial effort and moral character judgments. Does more prosocial effort always lead to rosier moral character judgments? Across four studies (<i>N</i> = 1,658), we find that moral character judgments increase with prosocial effort to a point and then plateau. We find evidence that this pattern is produced, in part, by descriptive and prescriptive norms: exceeding descriptive norms increases moral character judgments, but exceeding prescriptive norms has the opposite effect, which leads to a tapering off of moral character judgments at higher levels of effort.</p>","PeriodicalId":19834,"journal":{"name":"Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":4.0,"publicationDate":"2024-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10444046","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Perceiving a Stable Self-Concept Enables the Experience of Meaning in Life. 感知稳定的自我概念,体验生命的意义。
IF 4 2区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-05-01 Epub Date: 2023-02-01 DOI: 10.1177/01461672221150234
Charles Chu, Brian S Lowery

We test the hypothesis that the perception of stability in one's self-concept (i.e., future self-continuity) enables the experience of meaning in life because perceiving a stable sense of self confers a sense of certainty to the self-concept. Study 1 provided initial evidence of the influence of future self-continuity on feelings of meaning in life (MIL) in a nationally representative sample. In Studies 2a and 2b, we manipulated future self-continuity by varying the expectedness of one's future self, demonstrating the causal influence of future self-continuity on self-certainty and feelings of MIL. Study 3 again manipulated future self-continuity, finding an indirect effect on feelings of meaning in life via self-certainty. Our findings thus suggest the experience of meaning in life arises from the perception of a stable sense of self. We discuss the implications for the antecedents and conceptualization of MIL as well as the nature of the self-concept.

我们检验了这样一个假设,即一个人对自我概念稳定性的感知(即未来自我连续性)能够让人体验到生命的意义,因为感知到稳定的自我感觉会赋予自我概念一种确定感。研究 1 提供了具有全国代表性样本的未来自我连续性对人生意义感(MIL)影响的初步证据。在研究 2a 和 2b 中,我们通过改变对未来自我的预期来操纵未来自我连续性,从而证明了未来自我连续性对自我确定性和生命意义感的因果影响。研究 3 再次操纵了未来自我连续性,发现它通过自我确定性对人生意义感产生了间接影响。因此,我们的研究结果表明,人生意义的体验源于对稳定的自我意识的感知。我们将讨论 MIL 的前因和概念化以及自我概念性质的影响。
{"title":"Perceiving a Stable Self-Concept Enables the Experience of Meaning in Life.","authors":"Charles Chu, Brian S Lowery","doi":"10.1177/01461672221150234","DOIUrl":"10.1177/01461672221150234","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>We test the hypothesis that the perception of stability in one's self-concept (i.e., future self-continuity) enables the experience of meaning in life because perceiving a stable sense of self confers a sense of certainty to the self-concept. Study 1 provided initial evidence of the influence of future self-continuity on feelings of meaning in life (MIL) in a nationally representative sample. In Studies 2a and 2b, we manipulated future self-continuity by varying the expectedness of one's future self, demonstrating the causal influence of future self-continuity on self-certainty and feelings of MIL. Study 3 again manipulated future self-continuity, finding an indirect effect on feelings of meaning in life via self-certainty. Our findings thus suggest the experience of meaning in life arises from the perception of a stable sense of self. We discuss the implications for the antecedents and conceptualization of MIL as well as the nature of the self-concept.</p>","PeriodicalId":19834,"journal":{"name":"Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":4.0,"publicationDate":"2024-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9143454","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The Impact of Awe on Existential Isolation: Evidence for Contrasting Pathways. 敬畏对存在孤独的影响:相反途径的证据。
IF 4 2区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-05-01 Epub Date: 2023-01-11 DOI: 10.1177/01461672221144597
Megan E Edwards, Peter J Helm, Steven Pratscher, B Ann Bettencourt, Jamie Arndt

We propose that awe has multifaceted relations with existential isolation, a feeling of separation between the self and others or the world. Three studies examined the relation between awe and existential isolation via feelings of small self (vastness, self-size, self-perspectives) and a sense of connectedness. Awe (vs. a control topic) was induced either using virtual reality (Study 1) or a recall task (Studies 2 and 3) and was indirectly associated with higher and lower levels of existential isolation through differing pathways. Awe was associated with lower feelings of existential isolation via an increased sense of vastness, which in turn predicted greater connectedness; whereas awe was associated with higher feelings of existential isolation via increased sense of feeling small, which in turn predicted lower connectedness. This work advances understanding of the complex nature of awe-revealing its competing effects on the self and the social connectedness pathways through which awe can influence existential isolation.

我们认为,敬畏感与存在孤独感(一种自我与他人或世界分离的感觉)有着多方面的关系。三项研究通过渺小的自我感觉(广阔感、自我尺寸、自我观点)和联系感来考察敬畏与生存隔离之间的关系。通过虚拟现实(研究 1)或回忆任务(研究 2 和研究 3)来诱发敬畏感(与对照主题相比),并通过不同的途径将敬畏感与较高或较低的存在孤独感间接联系起来。敬畏与较低的存在孤独感相关,因为存在孤独感会增加人的广阔感,而广阔感又会增加人与人之间的联系;而敬畏与较高的存在孤独感相关,因为存在孤独感会增加人的渺小感,而渺小感又会降低人与人之间的联系。这项研究加深了人们对敬畏复杂本质的理解--揭示了敬畏对自我的竞争性影响,以及敬畏影响生存孤独感的社会联系途径。
{"title":"The Impact of Awe on Existential Isolation: Evidence for Contrasting Pathways.","authors":"Megan E Edwards, Peter J Helm, Steven Pratscher, B Ann Bettencourt, Jamie Arndt","doi":"10.1177/01461672221144597","DOIUrl":"10.1177/01461672221144597","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>We propose that awe has multifaceted relations with existential isolation, a feeling of separation between the self and others or the world. Three studies examined the relation between awe and existential isolation via feelings of small self (vastness, self-size, self-perspectives) and a sense of connectedness. Awe (vs. a control topic) was induced either using virtual reality (Study 1) or a recall task (Studies 2 and 3) and was indirectly associated with higher and lower levels of existential isolation through differing pathways. Awe was associated with <i>lower</i> feelings of existential isolation via an increased sense of vastness, which in turn predicted greater connectedness; whereas awe was associated with <i>higher</i> feelings of existential isolation via increased sense of feeling small, which in turn predicted lower connectedness. This work advances understanding of the complex nature of awe-revealing its competing effects on the self and the social connectedness pathways through which awe can influence existential isolation.</p>","PeriodicalId":19834,"journal":{"name":"Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":4.0,"publicationDate":"2024-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9084298","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Who Made This? Algorithms and Authorship Credit. 这是谁做的?算法与作者信用。
IF 4 2区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-05-01 Epub Date: 2023-02-03 DOI: 10.1177/01461672221149815
Arthur S Jago, Glenn R Carroll

Producers and creators often receive assistance with work from other people. Increasingly, algorithms can provide similar assistance. When algorithms assist or augment producers, does this change individuals' willingness to assign credit to those producers? Across four studies spanning several domains (e.g., painting, construction, sports analytics, and entrepreneurship), we find evidence that producers receive more credit for work when they are assisted by algorithms, compared with humans. We also find that individuals assume algorithmic assistance requires more producer oversight than human assistance does, a mechanism that explains these higher attributions of credit (Studies 1-3). The greater credit individuals assign to producers assisted by algorithms (vs. other people) also manifests itself in increased support for those producers' entrepreneurial endeavors (Study 4). As algorithms proliferate, norms of credit and authorship are likely changing, precipitating a variety of economic and social consequences.

制片人和创作者在工作中经常会得到其他人的帮助。越来越多的算法也能提供类似的帮助。当算法协助或增强制作者时,这是否会改变个人将信用归于这些制作者的意愿?在横跨多个领域(如绘画、建筑、体育分析和创业)的四项研究中,我们发现有证据表明,与人类相比,当生产者在算法的协助下完成工作时,他们会获得更多的荣誉。我们还发现,与人工协助相比,个人认为算法协助需要生产者更多的监督,这也是这些更高的信用归因的原因之一(研究 1-3)。个人对算法协助的生产者(与其他人相比)给予的更多信任也体现在对这些生产者创业努力的更多支持上(研究 4)。随着算法的普及,信用和作者身份的规范很可能会发生变化,从而引发各种经济和社会后果。
{"title":"Who Made This? Algorithms and Authorship Credit.","authors":"Arthur S Jago, Glenn R Carroll","doi":"10.1177/01461672221149815","DOIUrl":"10.1177/01461672221149815","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Producers and creators often receive assistance with work from other people. Increasingly, algorithms can provide similar assistance. When algorithms assist or augment producers, does this change individuals' willingness to assign credit to those producers? Across four studies spanning several domains (e.g., painting, construction, sports analytics, and entrepreneurship), we find evidence that producers receive more credit for work when they are assisted by algorithms, compared with humans. We also find that individuals assume algorithmic assistance requires more producer oversight than human assistance does, a mechanism that explains these higher attributions of credit (Studies 1-3). The greater credit individuals assign to producers assisted by algorithms (vs. other people) also manifests itself in increased support for those producers' entrepreneurial endeavors (Study 4). As algorithms proliferate, norms of credit and authorship are likely changing, precipitating a variety of economic and social consequences.</p>","PeriodicalId":19834,"journal":{"name":"Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":4.0,"publicationDate":"2024-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10696685","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Effects of Message-Sidedness on Perceived Source Bias: When Presenting Two Sides Does Versus Does Not Alleviate Concerns About Bias. 信息的两面性对感知来源偏差的影响:当呈现两面性时,是否能减轻对偏差的担忧。
IF 4 2区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-05-01 Epub Date: 2023-02-18 DOI: 10.1177/01461672231155389
Laura E Wallace, Lucas Hinsenkamp, Duane T Wegener, Zachary Braun

Communicators commonly present two-sided messages to avoid being perceived as biased. This approach equates bias with one-sidedness rather than divergence from the position supported by available data. Messages often concern topics with mixed qualities: a product is exceptional but expensive; a politician is inexperienced but ethical. For these topics, providing a two-sided message should reduce perceived bias according to both views of bias as one-sidedness and divergence from available data. However, if perceived bias follows divergence from available data, for topics viewed as one-sided (univalent), a two-sided message should not reduce perceived bias. Across five studies, acknowledging two sides reduced perceived bias for novel topics. In two of the studies, two-sidedness no longer reduced perceived bias for topics viewed as univalent. This work clarifies that people conceptualize bias as a divergence from available data, not simply one-sidedness. It also clarifies when and how to leverage message-sidedness to reduce perceived bias.

传播者通常会提出双面信息,以避免被认为有偏见。这种方法将偏见等同于片面性,而不是偏离现有数据所支持的立场。信息通常涉及品质参差不齐的话题:产品出类拔萃,但价格昂贵;政治家缺乏经验,但道德高尚。对于这些话题,根据片面性和偏离现有数据这两种偏见观点,提供双面信息应能减少感知偏见。然而,如果认为偏见是与现有数据相背离的,那么对于被认为是片面的(不对等的)话题,双面信息就不应该减少认为的偏见。在五项研究中,承认两面性减少了对新话题的认知偏差。在其中两项研究中,双面性不再减少人们对被视为单面的话题的认知偏差。这项研究澄清了人们将偏见概念化为与现有数据的偏差,而不仅仅是片面性。它还阐明了何时以及如何利用信息的两面性来减少感知偏见。
{"title":"Effects of Message-Sidedness on Perceived Source Bias: When Presenting Two Sides Does Versus Does Not Alleviate Concerns About Bias.","authors":"Laura E Wallace, Lucas Hinsenkamp, Duane T Wegener, Zachary Braun","doi":"10.1177/01461672231155389","DOIUrl":"10.1177/01461672231155389","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Communicators commonly present two-sided messages to avoid being perceived as biased. This approach equates bias with one-sidedness rather than divergence from the position supported by available data. Messages often concern topics with mixed qualities: a product is exceptional but expensive; a politician is inexperienced but ethical. For these topics, providing a two-sided message should reduce perceived bias according to both views of bias as one-sidedness and divergence from available data. However, if perceived bias follows divergence from available data, for topics viewed as one-sided (univalent), a two-sided message should not reduce perceived bias. Across five studies, acknowledging two sides reduced perceived bias for novel topics. In two of the studies, two-sidedness no longer reduced perceived bias for topics viewed as univalent. This work clarifies that people conceptualize bias as a divergence from available data, not simply one-sidedness. It also clarifies when and how to leverage message-sidedness to reduce perceived bias.</p>","PeriodicalId":19834,"journal":{"name":"Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":4.0,"publicationDate":"2024-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10809534","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1