首页 > 最新文献

Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin最新文献

英文 中文
Big Is Bad: Stereotypes About Organizational Size, Profit-Seeking, and Corporate Ethicality. 大就是坏:关于组织规模、逐利性和企业道德的成见。
IF 4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2024-06-01 Epub Date: 2023-02-16 DOI: 10.1177/01461672231151791
Andrea Freund, Francis Flynn, Kieran O'Connor

Individuals tend to hold a dim view of for-profit corporations, believing that profit-seeking comes at the expense of ethicality. In the present research, we show that this belief is not universal; rather, people associate ethicality with an organization's size. Across nine experiments (N = 4,796), people stereotyped large companies as less ethical than small companies. This size-ethicality stereotype emerged spontaneously (Study 1), implicitly (Study 2), and across industries (Study 3). Moreover, we find this stereotype can be partly explained by perceptions of profit-seeking behavior (Supplementary Studies A and B), and that people construe profit-seeking and its relationship to ethicality differently when considering large and small companies (Study 4). People attribute greater profit-maximizing motives (relative to profit-satisficing motives) to large companies, and these attributions shape their subsequent judgments of ethicality (Study 5; Supplementary Studies C and D).

人们往往对营利性公司持负面看法,认为追求利润是以牺牲道德为代价的。在本研究中,我们发现这种看法并不普遍;相反,人们会将道德性与组织的规模联系起来。在九个实验中(N = 4,796 ),人们刻板地认为大公司比小公司道德水平低。这种规模-道德刻板印象是自发出现的(研究 1)、隐性出现的(研究 2)和跨行业出现的(研究 3)。此外,我们发现这种刻板印象在一定程度上可以用人们对追求利润行为的看法来解释(补充研究 A 和 B),而且在考虑大公司和小公司时,人们对追求利润及其与道德性的关系的理解是不同的(研究 4)。人们认为大公司具有更大的利润最大化动机(相对于利润满足动机而言),而这些动机影响了他们随后对道德性的判断(研究 5;补充研究 C 和 D)。
{"title":"Big Is Bad: Stereotypes About Organizational Size, Profit-Seeking, and Corporate Ethicality.","authors":"Andrea Freund, Francis Flynn, Kieran O'Connor","doi":"10.1177/01461672231151791","DOIUrl":"10.1177/01461672231151791","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Individuals tend to hold a dim view of for-profit corporations, believing that profit-seeking comes at the expense of ethicality. In the present research, we show that this belief is not universal; rather, people associate ethicality with an organization's <i>size</i>. Across nine experiments (<i>N</i> = 4,796), people stereotyped large companies as less ethical than small companies. This size-ethicality stereotype emerged spontaneously (Study 1), implicitly (Study 2), and across industries (Study 3). Moreover, we find this stereotype can be partly explained by perceptions of profit-seeking behavior (Supplementary Studies A and B), and that people construe profit-seeking and its relationship to ethicality <i>differently</i> when considering large and small companies (Study 4). People attribute greater profit-<i>maximizing</i> motives (relative to profit-<i>satisficing</i> motives) to large companies, and these attributions shape their subsequent judgments of ethicality (Study 5; Supplementary Studies C and D).</p>","PeriodicalId":19834,"journal":{"name":"Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin","volume":" ","pages":"924-941"},"PeriodicalIF":4.0,"publicationDate":"2024-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9297207","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
A Comparative Investigation of the Predictive Validity of Four Indirect Measures of Bias and Prejudice. 对偏见和成见的四种间接测量方法的预测有效性进行比较研究。
IF 4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2024-06-01 Epub Date: 2023-01-20 DOI: 10.1177/01461672221150229
Jordan Axt, Nicholas Buttrick, Ruo Ying Feng

Although measures of implicit associations are influential in the prejudice literature, comparative tests of the predictive power of these measures are lacking. A large-scale (N > 100,000) analysis of four commonly used measures-the Implicit Association Test (IAT), Single-Category IAT (SC-IAT), evaluative priming task (EPT), and sorting paired features task (SPF)-across 10 intergroup domains and 250 outcomes found clear evidence for the superiority of the SC-IAT in predictive and incremental predictive validity. Follow-up analyses suggested that the SC-IAT benefited from an exclusive focus on associations toward stigmatized group members, as associations toward non-stigmatized group members diluted the predictive strength of relative measures like the IAT, SPF, and EPT. These results highlight how conclusions about predictive validity can vary drastically depending on the measure selected and reveal novel insights about the value of different measures when focusing on predictive than convergent validity.

尽管内隐联想的测量方法在偏见文献中很有影响力,但缺乏对这些测量方法预测能力的比较测试。一项对四种常用测量方法--内隐联想测验(IAT)、单类别内隐联想测验(SC-IAT)、评价性引物任务(EPT)和成对特征分类任务(SPF)--在10个群体间领域和250个结果中进行的大规模(N > 100,000)分析发现,有明确证据表明单类别内隐联想测验(SC-IAT)在预测性和增量预测有效性方面更胜一筹。后续分析表明,SC-IAT 得益于只关注对受鄙视群体成员的关联,因为对非鄙视群体成员的关联削弱了 IAT、SPF 和 EPT 等相对测量的预测强度。这些结果突显了预测效度的结论如何因所选测量指标的不同而大相径庭,并揭示了在关注预测效度而非收敛效度时不同测量指标价值的新见解。
{"title":"A Comparative Investigation of the Predictive Validity of Four Indirect Measures of Bias and Prejudice.","authors":"Jordan Axt, Nicholas Buttrick, Ruo Ying Feng","doi":"10.1177/01461672221150229","DOIUrl":"10.1177/01461672221150229","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Although measures of implicit associations are influential in the prejudice literature, comparative tests of the predictive power of these measures are lacking. A large-scale (<i>N</i> > 100,000) analysis of four commonly used measures-the Implicit Association Test (IAT), Single-Category IAT (SC-IAT), evaluative priming task (EPT), and sorting paired features task (SPF)-across 10 intergroup domains and 250 outcomes found clear evidence for the superiority of the SC-IAT in predictive and incremental predictive validity. Follow-up analyses suggested that the SC-IAT benefited from an exclusive focus on associations toward stigmatized group members, as associations toward non-stigmatized group members diluted the predictive strength of relative measures like the IAT, SPF, and EPT. These results highlight how conclusions about predictive validity can vary drastically depending on the measure selected and reveal novel insights about the value of different measures when focusing on predictive than convergent validity.</p>","PeriodicalId":19834,"journal":{"name":"Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin","volume":" ","pages":"871-888"},"PeriodicalIF":4.0,"publicationDate":"2024-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11080383/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10549740","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
On the Disposition to Think Analytically: Four Distinct Intuitive-Analytic Thinking Styles. 论分析思维倾向:四种不同的直觉分析思维方式。
IF 4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2024-06-01 Epub Date: 2023-03-02 DOI: 10.1177/01461672231154886
Christie Newton, Justin Feeney, Gordon Pennycook

Many measures have been developed to index intuitive versus analytic thinking. Yet it remains an open question whether people primarily vary along a single dimension or if there are genuinely different types of thinking styles. We distinguish between four distinct types of thinking styles: Actively Open-minded Thinking, Close-Minded Thinking, Preference for Intuitive Thinking, and Preference for Effortful Thinking. We discovered strong predictive validity across several outcome measures (e.g., epistemically suspect beliefs, bullshit receptivity, empathy, moral judgments), with some subscales having stronger predictive validity for some outcomes but not others. Furthermore, Actively Open-minded Thinking, in particular, strongly outperformed the Cognitive Reflection Test in predicting misperceptions about COVID-19 and the ability to discern between vaccination-related true and false news. Our results indicate that people do, in fact, differ along multiple dimensions of intuitive-analytic thinking styles and that these dimensions have consequences for understanding a wide range of beliefs and behaviors.

已经开发了许多指标来索引直觉思维与分析思维。然而,人们是否主要在一个维度上存在差异,或者是否存在真正不同类型的思维方式,这仍然是一个悬而未决的问题。我们区分了四种不同类型的思维方式:积极开放思维、封闭思维、偏好直觉思维和偏好费力思维。我们发现,在几个结果测量中(例如,认知怀疑信念、狗屁接受能力、同理心、道德判断),有很强的预测有效性,一些分量表对某些结果有更强的预测有效率,但对其他结果没有。此外,特别是在预测对新冠肺炎的误解以及辨别与疫苗接种相关的真实和虚假新闻的能力方面,积极开放思维显著优于认知反射测试。我们的研究结果表明,事实上,人们在直觉分析思维风格的多个维度上确实存在差异,这些维度对理解广泛的信仰和行为有影响。
{"title":"On the Disposition to Think Analytically: Four Distinct Intuitive-Analytic Thinking Styles.","authors":"Christie Newton, Justin Feeney, Gordon Pennycook","doi":"10.1177/01461672231154886","DOIUrl":"10.1177/01461672231154886","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Many measures have been developed to index intuitive versus analytic thinking. Yet it remains an open question whether people primarily vary along a single dimension or if there are genuinely different types of thinking styles. We distinguish between four distinct types of thinking styles: Actively Open-minded Thinking, Close-Minded Thinking, Preference for Intuitive Thinking, and Preference for Effortful Thinking. We discovered strong predictive validity across several outcome measures (e.g., epistemically suspect beliefs, bullshit receptivity, empathy, moral judgments), with some subscales having stronger predictive validity for some outcomes but not others. Furthermore, Actively Open-minded Thinking, in particular, strongly outperformed the Cognitive Reflection Test in predicting misperceptions about COVID-19 and the ability to discern between vaccination-related true and false news. Our results indicate that people do, in fact, differ along multiple dimensions of intuitive-analytic thinking styles and that these dimensions have consequences for understanding a wide range of beliefs and behaviors.</p>","PeriodicalId":19834,"journal":{"name":"Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin","volume":" ","pages":"906-923"},"PeriodicalIF":4.0,"publicationDate":"2024-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11080384/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9380186","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Social Class and Social Pain: Target SES Biases Judgments of Pain and Support for White Target Individuals. 社会阶层和社会痛苦:目标SES对白人目标个体的痛苦和支持的判断存在偏见。
IF 3.4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2024-06-01 Epub Date: 2023-03-10 DOI: 10.1177/01461672231156025
Brielle N Johnson, Erin Freiburger, Jason C Deska, Jonathan W Kunstman

Social pain, defined as distress caused by negative interpersonal experiences (e.g., ostracism, mistreatment), is detrimental to health. Yet, it is unclear how social class might shape judgments of the social pains of low-socioeconomic status (SES) and high-SES individuals. Five studies tested competing toughness and empathy predictions for SES's effect on social pain judgments. Consistent with an empathy account, in all studies (Ncumulative = 1,046), low-SES White targets were judged more sensitive to social pain than high-SES White targets. Further, empathy mediated these effects, such that participants felt greater empathy and expected more social pain for low-SES targets relative to high-SES targets. Social pain judgments also informed judgments of social support needs, as low-SES targets were presumed to need more coping resources to manage hurtful events than high-SES targets. The current findings provide initial evidence that empathic concern for low-SES White individuals sensitizes social pain judgments and increases expected support needs for lower class White individuals.

社会痛苦,定义为负面人际经历(如排斥、虐待)引起的痛苦,对健康有害。然而,尚不清楚社会阶层如何影响对低社会经济地位(SES)和高社会经济地位个体的社会痛苦的判断。五项研究测试了社会经济地位对社会痛苦判断的影响的竞争韧性和同理心预测。与移情分析一致,在所有研究中(Ncumulative=1046),低SES白人目标被认为比高SES白人对象对社会疼痛更敏感。此外,同理心介导了这些影响,因此参与者对低社会经济地位目标的同理心更强,并期望相对于高社会经济地位的目标有更多的社会痛苦。社会痛苦判断也为社会支持需求的判断提供了依据,因为低社会经济地位目标被认为比高社会经济地位对象需要更多的应对资源来管理伤害事件。目前的研究结果提供了初步证据,表明对低社会经济地位白人个体的移情关注提高了社会痛苦判断的敏感性,并增加了对低阶层白人个体的预期支持需求。
{"title":"Social Class and Social Pain: Target SES Biases Judgments of Pain and Support for White Target Individuals.","authors":"Brielle N Johnson, Erin Freiburger, Jason C Deska, Jonathan W Kunstman","doi":"10.1177/01461672231156025","DOIUrl":"10.1177/01461672231156025","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Social pain, defined as distress caused by negative interpersonal experiences (e.g., ostracism, mistreatment), is detrimental to health. Yet, it is unclear how social class might shape judgments of the social pains of low-socioeconomic status (SES) and high-SES individuals. Five studies tested competing toughness and empathy predictions for SES's effect on social pain judgments. Consistent with an empathy account, in all studies (<i>N</i><sub>cumulative</sub> = 1,046), low-SES White targets were judged more sensitive to social pain than high-SES White targets. Further, empathy mediated these effects, such that participants felt greater empathy and expected more social pain for low-SES targets relative to high-SES targets. Social pain judgments also informed judgments of social support needs, as low-SES targets were presumed to need more coping resources to manage hurtful events than high-SES targets. The current findings provide initial evidence that empathic concern for low-SES White individuals sensitizes social pain judgments and increases expected support needs for lower class White individuals.</p>","PeriodicalId":19834,"journal":{"name":"Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin","volume":" ","pages":"957-970"},"PeriodicalIF":3.4,"publicationDate":"2024-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9454845","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Domain-Specific Greed. 特定领域的贪婪
IF 4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2024-06-01 Epub Date: 2023-01-25 DOI: 10.1177/01461672221148004
Martin Weiß, Julian Schulze, Stefan Krumm, Anja S Göritz, Johannes Hewig, Patrick Mussel

Greed, the insatiable and excessive desire and striving for more even at the expense of others, may be directed toward various goods. In this article, we propose that greed may be conceptualized as a domain-specific construct. Based on a literature review and an expert survey, we identified 10 domains of greed which we operationalized with the DOmain-SPEcific Greed (DOSPEG) questionnaire. In Study 1 (N = 725), we found support for the proposed structure and convergent validity with related constructs. Bifactor-(S-1) models revealed that generic greed is differentially related to the greed domains, indicating that generic greed primarily captures a striving for money and material things. In the second study (N = 591), we found that greed domains had incremental validity beyond generic greed with regard to corresponding criteria assessed via self- and other-reports. We conclude that greed can be conceptualized as a domain-specific construct and propose an onion model reflecting this structure.

贪婪是一种贪得无厌、过度的欲望,它甚至以牺牲他人利益为代价来争取更多,它可能针对各种物品。在本文中,我们建议将贪婪概念化为一个特定领域的概念。根据文献综述和专家调查,我们确定了 10 个贪婪领域,并通过 DOmain-SPEcific Greed(DOSPEG)问卷将其操作化。在研究 1(N = 725)中,我们发现所提出的结构得到了支持,并且与相关结构具有收敛有效性。双因子-(S-1)模型显示,一般贪婪与贪婪领域之间存在差异,这表明一般贪婪主要反映了对金钱和物质的追求。在第二项研究(N = 591)中,我们发现,通过自我和他人报告评估的相应标准,贪婪领域具有超越一般贪婪的增量有效性。我们的结论是,贪婪可以被概念化为一个特定领域的结构,并提出了一个反映这一结构的洋葱模型。
{"title":"Domain-Specific Greed.","authors":"Martin Weiß, Julian Schulze, Stefan Krumm, Anja S Göritz, Johannes Hewig, Patrick Mussel","doi":"10.1177/01461672221148004","DOIUrl":"10.1177/01461672221148004","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Greed, the insatiable and excessive desire and striving for more even at the expense of others, may be directed toward various goods. In this article, we propose that greed may be conceptualized as a domain-specific construct. Based on a literature review and an expert survey, we identified 10 domains of greed which we operationalized with the DOmain-SPEcific Greed (DOSPEG) questionnaire. In Study 1 (<i>N</i> = 725), we found support for the proposed structure and convergent validity with related constructs. Bifactor-(S-1) models revealed that generic greed is differentially related to the greed domains, indicating that generic greed primarily captures a striving for money and material things. In the second study (<i>N</i> = 591), we found that greed domains had incremental validity beyond generic greed with regard to corresponding criteria assessed via self- and other-reports. We conclude that greed can be conceptualized as a domain-specific construct and propose an onion model reflecting this structure.</p>","PeriodicalId":19834,"journal":{"name":"Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin","volume":" ","pages":"889-905"},"PeriodicalIF":4.0,"publicationDate":"2024-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11080388/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9183277","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
How Much Is Enough? The Relationship Between Prosocial Effort and Moral Character Judgments. 多少才算足够?亲社会努力与道德品格判断之间的关系》(The Relationship Between Prosocial Effort and Moral Character Judgments.
IF 4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2024-05-01 Epub Date: 2022-12-28 DOI: 10.1177/01461672221135954
Zachariah Berry, Brian J Lucas

The amount of effort required to bring about a prosocial outcome can vary from low-handing a stranger the wallet she just dropped-to high-spending days tracking down the owner of a lost wallet. The goal of the current research is to characterize the relationship between prosocial effort and moral character judgments. Does more prosocial effort always lead to rosier moral character judgments? Across four studies (N = 1,658), we find that moral character judgments increase with prosocial effort to a point and then plateau. We find evidence that this pattern is produced, in part, by descriptive and prescriptive norms: exceeding descriptive norms increases moral character judgments, but exceeding prescriptive norms has the opposite effect, which leads to a tapering off of moral character judgments at higher levels of effort.

实现亲社会结果所需的努力程度各不相同,小到把陌生人刚掉的钱包交给她,大到花几天时间寻找丢失钱包的失主。当前研究的目标是描述亲社会努力与道德品格判断之间的关系。更多的亲社会努力是否总是会导致更美好的道德品质判断?通过四项研究(N = 1,658),我们发现道德品格判断会随着亲社会努力的增加而增加,达到一定程度后会趋于平稳。我们发现有证据表明,这种模式部分是由描述性规范和规定性规范产生的:超过描述性规范会增加道德品格判断,但超过规定性规范则会产生相反的效果,从而导致道德品格判断在较高的努力水平上逐渐减弱。
{"title":"How Much Is Enough? The Relationship Between Prosocial Effort and Moral Character Judgments.","authors":"Zachariah Berry, Brian J Lucas","doi":"10.1177/01461672221135954","DOIUrl":"10.1177/01461672221135954","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The amount of effort required to bring about a prosocial outcome can vary from low-handing a stranger the wallet she just dropped-to high-spending days tracking down the owner of a lost wallet. The goal of the current research is to characterize the relationship between prosocial effort and moral character judgments. Does more prosocial effort always lead to rosier moral character judgments? Across four studies (<i>N</i> = 1,658), we find that moral character judgments increase with prosocial effort to a point and then plateau. We find evidence that this pattern is produced, in part, by descriptive and prescriptive norms: exceeding descriptive norms increases moral character judgments, but exceeding prescriptive norms has the opposite effect, which leads to a tapering off of moral character judgments at higher levels of effort.</p>","PeriodicalId":19834,"journal":{"name":"Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin","volume":" ","pages":"659-678"},"PeriodicalIF":4.0,"publicationDate":"2024-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10444046","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Perceiving a Stable Self-Concept Enables the Experience of Meaning in Life. 感知稳定的自我概念,体验生命的意义。
IF 4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2024-05-01 Epub Date: 2023-02-01 DOI: 10.1177/01461672221150234
Charles Chu, Brian S Lowery

We test the hypothesis that the perception of stability in one's self-concept (i.e., future self-continuity) enables the experience of meaning in life because perceiving a stable sense of self confers a sense of certainty to the self-concept. Study 1 provided initial evidence of the influence of future self-continuity on feelings of meaning in life (MIL) in a nationally representative sample. In Studies 2a and 2b, we manipulated future self-continuity by varying the expectedness of one's future self, demonstrating the causal influence of future self-continuity on self-certainty and feelings of MIL. Study 3 again manipulated future self-continuity, finding an indirect effect on feelings of meaning in life via self-certainty. Our findings thus suggest the experience of meaning in life arises from the perception of a stable sense of self. We discuss the implications for the antecedents and conceptualization of MIL as well as the nature of the self-concept.

我们检验了这样一个假设,即一个人对自我概念稳定性的感知(即未来自我连续性)能够让人体验到生命的意义,因为感知到稳定的自我感觉会赋予自我概念一种确定感。研究 1 提供了具有全国代表性样本的未来自我连续性对人生意义感(MIL)影响的初步证据。在研究 2a 和 2b 中,我们通过改变对未来自我的预期来操纵未来自我连续性,从而证明了未来自我连续性对自我确定性和生命意义感的因果影响。研究 3 再次操纵了未来自我连续性,发现它通过自我确定性对人生意义感产生了间接影响。因此,我们的研究结果表明,人生意义的体验源于对稳定的自我意识的感知。我们将讨论 MIL 的前因和概念化以及自我概念性质的影响。
{"title":"Perceiving a Stable Self-Concept Enables the Experience of Meaning in Life.","authors":"Charles Chu, Brian S Lowery","doi":"10.1177/01461672221150234","DOIUrl":"10.1177/01461672221150234","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>We test the hypothesis that the perception of stability in one's self-concept (i.e., future self-continuity) enables the experience of meaning in life because perceiving a stable sense of self confers a sense of certainty to the self-concept. Study 1 provided initial evidence of the influence of future self-continuity on feelings of meaning in life (MIL) in a nationally representative sample. In Studies 2a and 2b, we manipulated future self-continuity by varying the expectedness of one's future self, demonstrating the causal influence of future self-continuity on self-certainty and feelings of MIL. Study 3 again manipulated future self-continuity, finding an indirect effect on feelings of meaning in life via self-certainty. Our findings thus suggest the experience of meaning in life arises from the perception of a stable sense of self. We discuss the implications for the antecedents and conceptualization of MIL as well as the nature of the self-concept.</p>","PeriodicalId":19834,"journal":{"name":"Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin","volume":" ","pages":"780-792"},"PeriodicalIF":4.0,"publicationDate":"2024-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9143454","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Examining the Contribution of Physical Cues for Same- and Cross-Race Face Individuation. 研究物理线索对同种族和跨种族人脸识别的贡献
IF 4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2024-05-01 Epub Date: 2023-01-03 DOI: 10.1177/01461672221141510
Joshua Correll, Debbie S Ma, David A Kenny, Tomás A Palma

Face individuation involves sensitivity to physical characteristics that provide information about identity. We examined whether Black and White American faces differ in terms of individuating information, and whether Black and White perceivers differentially weight information when judging same-race and cross-race faces. Study 1 analyzed 20 structural metrics (e.g., eye width, nose length) of 158 Black and White faces to determine which differentiate faces within each group. High-utility metrics (e.g., nose length, eye height, chin length) differentiated faces of both groups, low-utility metrics (e.g., face width, eye width, face length) offered less individuating information. Study 2 (N = 4,510) explored Black and White participants' sensitivity to variation on structural metrics using similarity ratings. High-utility metrics affected perceived dissimilarity more than low-utility metrics. This relationship was non-significantly stronger for same-race faces rather than cross-race faces. Perceivers also relied more on features that were racially stereotypic of the faces they were rating.

人脸识别涉及对提供身份信息的物理特征的敏感性。我们研究了美国黑人和白人面孔在个体化信息方面是否存在差异,以及黑人和白人感知者在判断同种族面孔和跨种族面孔时对信息的权重是否不同。研究 1 分析了 158 张黑人和白人面孔的 20 个结构指标(如眼睛宽度、鼻子长度),以确定哪些指标能区分每个群体中的面孔。高效用指标(如鼻长、眼高、下巴长)可区分两组人脸,低效用指标(如脸宽、眼宽、脸长)提供的个体化信息较少。研究 2(N=4,510)使用相似度评分法探讨了黑人和白人参与者对结构指标变化的敏感度。与低效用指标相比,高效用指标对感知相似性的影响更大。这种关系在同种族面孔上比跨种族面孔上更为明显。感知者也更依赖于他们所评定的人脸的种族刻板印象特征。
{"title":"Examining the Contribution of Physical Cues for Same- and Cross-Race Face Individuation.","authors":"Joshua Correll, Debbie S Ma, David A Kenny, Tomás A Palma","doi":"10.1177/01461672221141510","DOIUrl":"10.1177/01461672221141510","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Face individuation involves sensitivity to physical characteristics that provide information about identity. We examined whether Black and White American faces differ in terms of individuating information, and whether Black and White perceivers differentially weight information when judging same-race and cross-race faces. Study 1 analyzed 20 structural metrics (e.g., eye width, nose length) of 158 Black and White faces to determine which differentiate faces within each group. High-utility metrics (e.g., nose length, eye height, chin length) differentiated faces of both groups, low-utility metrics (e.g., face width, eye width, face length) offered less individuating information. Study 2 (<i>N</i> = 4,510) explored Black and White participants' sensitivity to variation on structural metrics using similarity ratings. High-utility metrics affected perceived dissimilarity more than low-utility metrics. This relationship was non-significantly stronger for same-race faces rather than cross-race faces. Perceivers also relied more on features that were racially stereotypic of the faces they were rating.</p>","PeriodicalId":19834,"journal":{"name":"Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin","volume":" ","pages":"694-714"},"PeriodicalIF":4.0,"publicationDate":"2024-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10826327","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The Impact of Awe on Existential Isolation: Evidence for Contrasting Pathways. 敬畏对存在孤独的影响:相反途径的证据。
IF 4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2024-05-01 Epub Date: 2023-01-11 DOI: 10.1177/01461672221144597
Megan E Edwards, Peter J Helm, Steven Pratscher, B Ann Bettencourt, Jamie Arndt

We propose that awe has multifaceted relations with existential isolation, a feeling of separation between the self and others or the world. Three studies examined the relation between awe and existential isolation via feelings of small self (vastness, self-size, self-perspectives) and a sense of connectedness. Awe (vs. a control topic) was induced either using virtual reality (Study 1) or a recall task (Studies 2 and 3) and was indirectly associated with higher and lower levels of existential isolation through differing pathways. Awe was associated with lower feelings of existential isolation via an increased sense of vastness, which in turn predicted greater connectedness; whereas awe was associated with higher feelings of existential isolation via increased sense of feeling small, which in turn predicted lower connectedness. This work advances understanding of the complex nature of awe-revealing its competing effects on the self and the social connectedness pathways through which awe can influence existential isolation.

我们认为,敬畏感与存在孤独感(一种自我与他人或世界分离的感觉)有着多方面的关系。三项研究通过渺小的自我感觉(广阔感、自我尺寸、自我观点)和联系感来考察敬畏与生存隔离之间的关系。通过虚拟现实(研究 1)或回忆任务(研究 2 和研究 3)来诱发敬畏感(与对照主题相比),并通过不同的途径将敬畏感与较高或较低的存在孤独感间接联系起来。敬畏与较低的存在孤独感相关,因为存在孤独感会增加人的广阔感,而广阔感又会增加人与人之间的联系;而敬畏与较高的存在孤独感相关,因为存在孤独感会增加人的渺小感,而渺小感又会降低人与人之间的联系。这项研究加深了人们对敬畏复杂本质的理解--揭示了敬畏对自我的竞争性影响,以及敬畏影响生存孤独感的社会联系途径。
{"title":"The Impact of Awe on Existential Isolation: Evidence for Contrasting Pathways.","authors":"Megan E Edwards, Peter J Helm, Steven Pratscher, B Ann Bettencourt, Jamie Arndt","doi":"10.1177/01461672221144597","DOIUrl":"10.1177/01461672221144597","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>We propose that awe has multifaceted relations with existential isolation, a feeling of separation between the self and others or the world. Three studies examined the relation between awe and existential isolation via feelings of small self (vastness, self-size, self-perspectives) and a sense of connectedness. Awe (vs. a control topic) was induced either using virtual reality (Study 1) or a recall task (Studies 2 and 3) and was indirectly associated with higher and lower levels of existential isolation through differing pathways. Awe was associated with <i>lower</i> feelings of existential isolation via an increased sense of vastness, which in turn predicted greater connectedness; whereas awe was associated with <i>higher</i> feelings of existential isolation via increased sense of feeling small, which in turn predicted lower connectedness. This work advances understanding of the complex nature of awe-revealing its competing effects on the self and the social connectedness pathways through which awe can influence existential isolation.</p>","PeriodicalId":19834,"journal":{"name":"Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin","volume":" ","pages":"715-732"},"PeriodicalIF":4.0,"publicationDate":"2024-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9084298","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Who Made This? Algorithms and Authorship Credit. 这是谁做的?算法与作者信用。
IF 4 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2024-05-01 Epub Date: 2023-02-03 DOI: 10.1177/01461672221149815
Arthur S Jago, Glenn R Carroll

Producers and creators often receive assistance with work from other people. Increasingly, algorithms can provide similar assistance. When algorithms assist or augment producers, does this change individuals' willingness to assign credit to those producers? Across four studies spanning several domains (e.g., painting, construction, sports analytics, and entrepreneurship), we find evidence that producers receive more credit for work when they are assisted by algorithms, compared with humans. We also find that individuals assume algorithmic assistance requires more producer oversight than human assistance does, a mechanism that explains these higher attributions of credit (Studies 1-3). The greater credit individuals assign to producers assisted by algorithms (vs. other people) also manifests itself in increased support for those producers' entrepreneurial endeavors (Study 4). As algorithms proliferate, norms of credit and authorship are likely changing, precipitating a variety of economic and social consequences.

制片人和创作者在工作中经常会得到其他人的帮助。越来越多的算法也能提供类似的帮助。当算法协助或增强制作者时,这是否会改变个人将信用归于这些制作者的意愿?在横跨多个领域(如绘画、建筑、体育分析和创业)的四项研究中,我们发现有证据表明,与人类相比,当生产者在算法的协助下完成工作时,他们会获得更多的荣誉。我们还发现,与人工协助相比,个人认为算法协助需要生产者更多的监督,这也是这些更高的信用归因的原因之一(研究 1-3)。个人对算法协助的生产者(与其他人相比)给予的更多信任也体现在对这些生产者创业努力的更多支持上(研究 4)。随着算法的普及,信用和作者身份的规范很可能会发生变化,从而引发各种经济和社会后果。
{"title":"Who Made This? Algorithms and Authorship Credit.","authors":"Arthur S Jago, Glenn R Carroll","doi":"10.1177/01461672221149815","DOIUrl":"10.1177/01461672221149815","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Producers and creators often receive assistance with work from other people. Increasingly, algorithms can provide similar assistance. When algorithms assist or augment producers, does this change individuals' willingness to assign credit to those producers? Across four studies spanning several domains (e.g., painting, construction, sports analytics, and entrepreneurship), we find evidence that producers receive more credit for work when they are assisted by algorithms, compared with humans. We also find that individuals assume algorithmic assistance requires more producer oversight than human assistance does, a mechanism that explains these higher attributions of credit (Studies 1-3). The greater credit individuals assign to producers assisted by algorithms (vs. other people) also manifests itself in increased support for those producers' entrepreneurial endeavors (Study 4). As algorithms proliferate, norms of credit and authorship are likely changing, precipitating a variety of economic and social consequences.</p>","PeriodicalId":19834,"journal":{"name":"Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin","volume":" ","pages":"793-806"},"PeriodicalIF":4.0,"publicationDate":"2024-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10696685","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1