首页 > 最新文献

Psychological bulletin最新文献

英文 中文
The gender convergence effect in older age: A meta-analytic review comparing modern attitudes toward younger, middle-aged, and older women and men.
IF 17.3 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2025-03-01 Epub Date: 2025-02-17 DOI: 10.1037/bul0000467
Angela Shakeri, Michael S North

As older women drive aging population trends, it is crucial to understand how target age and gender jointly influence perceiver attitudes. Although the prevailing "double jeopardy" perspective portrays older women as the most derogated age-gender group due to facing both age and sex bias, some evidence suggests gender attitudes converge with target age (i.e., a "convergence" perspective). Investigating these competing hypotheses, we meta-analyzed 55 reports (k = 92 samples, N = 37,235) comparing attitudes toward younger, middle-aged, and older women and men. Results suggested more positive overall attitudes toward younger and middle-aged adults versus older adults-and, perhaps surprisingly, toward women versus men. Moderator analyses revealed significant Age × Gender interactions. Consistent with convergence, a pro-female bias emerged toward younger (g = -0.12) and middle-aged (g = -0.11) targets, but attitudes toward older women and men were virtually equivalent (g = -0.01). Consistent with double jeopardy, a stronger pro-younger (vs. older) bias existed for women (g = -0.34) as compared to men (g = -0.22), and a stronger pro-middle aged (vs. older) bias existed for women (g = -0.34) as compared to men (g = -0.22). Attitude dimension emerged as a significant moderator: For example, whereas warmth and agency stereotypes reflected a double jeopardy pattern-older women seen as the warmest but least agentic group-behaviors followed a pattern of gender convergence with age. Our findings highlight the complex interplay of age and gender in shaping attitudes, underscoring the need to simultaneously consider both identities in social perception research. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA, all rights reserved).

{"title":"The gender convergence effect in older age: A meta-analytic review comparing modern attitudes toward younger, middle-aged, and older women and men.","authors":"Angela Shakeri, Michael S North","doi":"10.1037/bul0000467","DOIUrl":"10.1037/bul0000467","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>As older women drive aging population trends, it is crucial to understand how target age and gender jointly influence perceiver attitudes. Although the prevailing \"double jeopardy\" perspective portrays older women as the most derogated age-gender group due to facing both age and sex bias, some evidence suggests gender attitudes converge with target age (i.e., a \"convergence\" perspective). Investigating these competing hypotheses, we meta-analyzed 55 reports (<i>k</i> = 92 samples, <i>N</i> = 37,235) comparing attitudes toward younger, middle-aged, and older women and men. Results suggested more positive overall attitudes toward younger and middle-aged adults versus older adults-and, perhaps surprisingly, toward women versus men. Moderator analyses revealed significant Age × Gender interactions. Consistent with convergence, a pro-female bias emerged toward younger (<i>g</i> = -0.12) and middle-aged (<i>g</i> = -0.11) targets, but attitudes toward older women and men were virtually equivalent (<i>g</i> = -0.01). Consistent with double jeopardy, a stronger pro-younger (vs. older) bias existed for women (<i>g</i> = -0.34) as compared to men (<i>g</i> = -0.22), and a stronger pro-middle aged (vs. older) bias existed for women (<i>g</i> = -0.34) as compared to men (<i>g</i> = -0.22). Attitude dimension emerged as a significant moderator: For example, whereas warmth and agency stereotypes reflected a double jeopardy pattern-older women seen as the warmest but least agentic group-behaviors followed a pattern of gender convergence with age. Our findings highlight the complex interplay of age and gender in shaping attitudes, underscoring the need to simultaneously consider both identities in social perception research. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":20854,"journal":{"name":"Psychological bulletin","volume":" ","pages":"261-284"},"PeriodicalIF":17.3,"publicationDate":"2025-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143441717","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Structuring hierarchy concepts: Evaluating measures of power, status, dominance, and prestige on the basis of an integrative model and systematic literature review.
IF 17.3 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2025-03-01 DOI: 10.1037/bul0000470
Robert Körner, Jennifer R Overbeck, Astrid Schütz

Research on social hierarchy is flourishing. Often, researchers employ self- or peer-report measures to assess variables such as power or dominance. One drawback of studies in this line of research is that researchers use different scales to measure the same constructs and different researchers use the same scale but aim to measure different constructs. Moreover, hierarchy concepts have been used interchangeably and terms have been used for a specific variable but operationalized with a measure that taps into another construct. This practice leads to problems such as the jingle-jangle fallacy. As these fallacies occur at the construct and the measurement levels, we first delineate an Integrative Model of Social Hierarchy Concepts and provide definitions of different hierarchy concepts (power, status, dominance, prestige, motives regarding these variables) to establish conceptual consensus. Based on a systematic literature search, we then present 67 validated scales that aim to measure these constructs. Additionally, we discuss other measurement approaches beyond self-reports (e.g., indirect tests, language features). For a selected subset of scales, we conducted an empirical study to provide additional analyses on reliability, model fit, and exploratory factor analyses to detect similarities and differences between scales. Eventually, we derive recommendations on which scales and measures to use for assessing which hierarchy variable and how to advance measurement practices in this domain. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA, all rights reserved).

{"title":"Structuring hierarchy concepts: Evaluating measures of power, status, dominance, and prestige on the basis of an integrative model and systematic literature review.","authors":"Robert Körner, Jennifer R Overbeck, Astrid Schütz","doi":"10.1037/bul0000470","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000470","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Research on social hierarchy is flourishing. Often, researchers employ self- or peer-report measures to assess variables such as power or dominance. One drawback of studies in this line of research is that researchers use different scales to measure the same constructs and different researchers use the same scale but aim to measure different constructs. Moreover, hierarchy concepts have been used interchangeably and terms have been used for a specific variable but operationalized with a measure that taps into another construct. This practice leads to problems such as the jingle-jangle fallacy. As these fallacies occur at the construct and the measurement levels, we first delineate an Integrative Model of Social Hierarchy Concepts and provide definitions of different hierarchy concepts (power, status, dominance, prestige, motives regarding these variables) to establish conceptual consensus. Based on a systematic literature search, we then present 67 validated scales that aim to measure these constructs. Additionally, we discuss other measurement approaches beyond self-reports (e.g., indirect tests, language features). For a selected subset of scales, we conducted an empirical study to provide additional analyses on reliability, model fit, and exploratory factor analyses to detect similarities and differences between scales. Eventually, we derive recommendations on which scales and measures to use for assessing which hierarchy variable and how to advance measurement practices in this domain. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":20854,"journal":{"name":"Psychological bulletin","volume":"151 3","pages":"322-364"},"PeriodicalIF":17.3,"publicationDate":"2025-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143754246","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Social class and prosociality: A meta-analytic review.
IF 17.3 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2025-03-01 DOI: 10.1037/bul0000469
Junhui Wu, Daniel Balliet, Mingliang Yuan, Wenqi Li, Yanyan Chen, Shuxian Jin, Shenghua Luan, Paul A M Van Lange

Two theoretical perspectives (i.e., the risk management perspective and the resource perspective) offer competing predictions that higher class individuals-relative to lower class individuals-tend to be less versus more prosocial, respectively. Different predictions can also be drawn from each perspective about how the class-prosociality association varies across sociocultural contexts. To date, each perspective has received mixed empirical support. To test these competing perspectives, we synthesized 1,106 effect sizes from 471 independent studies on social class and prosociality (total N = 2,340,806, covering the years 1968-2024) conducted within 60 societies. Supporting the resource perspective, we found higher class individuals to be slightly more prosocial (r = .065, 95% confidence interval [.055, .075]); this association held for children, adolescents, and adults and did not significantly vary by any sociocultural variable. In testing the methodological moderators, we found no significant difference in the class-prosociality association in studies measuring objective social class (r = .066) and those measuring or manipulating subjective social class (r = .063). Nevertheless, the observed class-prosociality association was stronger when assessing prosocial behavior involving actual commitment of material or nonmaterial resources (r = .079) compared to prosocial intention (r = .039), and stronger under public (r = .065) than private (r = .016) circumstances. These findings generally support the resource perspective on class-based differences in prosociality-that the relatively higher cost of prosocial behavior, combined with heightened experience of deprivation, results in lower levels of prosociality among individuals with a lower social class background. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA, all rights reserved).

两种理论视角(即风险管理视角和资源视角)提供了相互竞争的预测,即相对于较低阶层的个人而言,较高阶层的个人倾向于较低的亲社会性或较高的亲社会性。对于阶级与亲社会性之间的联系在不同的社会文化背景下如何变化,每种观点也可以得出不同的预测。迄今为止,每种观点都得到了不同的实证支持。为了检验这些相互竞争的观点,我们综合了在 60 个社会中进行的 471 项关于社会阶层和亲社会性的独立研究(总人数=2,340,806 人,时间跨度为 1968-2024 年)的 1,106 个效应大小。我们发现,阶级越高的人亲社会性越强(r = .065,95% 置信区间为[.055, .075]);儿童、青少年和成年人的亲社会性都与阶级有关,而且与社会文化变量的关系不大。在测试方法调节因子时,我们发现在测量客观社会阶层(r = .066)和测量或操纵主观社会阶层(r = .063)的研究中,阶层与前社会性的关联没有显著差异。然而,在评估涉及实际投入物质或非物质资源的亲社会行为时(r = .079),观察到的阶级-亲社会性关联比亲社会意图(r = .039)更强,在公共(r = .065)环境下比私人(r = .016)环境下更强。这些发现总体上支持了关于亲社会性中基于阶层差异的资源观点--即亲社会行为的成本相对较高,再加上更多的贫困经历,导致社会阶层背景较低的个人亲社会性水平较低。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Social class and prosociality: A meta-analytic review.","authors":"Junhui Wu, Daniel Balliet, Mingliang Yuan, Wenqi Li, Yanyan Chen, Shuxian Jin, Shenghua Luan, Paul A M Van Lange","doi":"10.1037/bul0000469","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000469","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Two theoretical perspectives (i.e., the risk management perspective and the resource perspective) offer competing predictions that higher class individuals-relative to lower class individuals-tend to be less versus more prosocial, respectively. Different predictions can also be drawn from each perspective about how the class-prosociality association varies across sociocultural contexts. To date, each perspective has received mixed empirical support. To test these competing perspectives, we synthesized 1,106 effect sizes from 471 independent studies on social class and prosociality (total N = 2,340,806, covering the years 1968-2024) conducted within 60 societies. Supporting the resource perspective, we found higher class individuals to be slightly more prosocial (r = .065, 95% confidence interval [.055, .075]); this association held for children, adolescents, and adults and did not significantly vary by any sociocultural variable. In testing the methodological moderators, we found no significant difference in the class-prosociality association in studies measuring objective social class (r = .066) and those measuring or manipulating subjective social class (r = .063). Nevertheless, the observed class-prosociality association was stronger when assessing prosocial behavior involving actual commitment of material or nonmaterial resources (r = .079) compared to prosocial intention (r = .039), and stronger under public (r = .065) than private (r = .016) circumstances. These findings generally support the resource perspective on class-based differences in prosociality-that the relatively higher cost of prosocial behavior, combined with heightened experience of deprivation, results in lower levels of prosociality among individuals with a lower social class background. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":20854,"journal":{"name":"Psychological bulletin","volume":"151 3","pages":"285-321"},"PeriodicalIF":17.3,"publicationDate":"2025-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143754243","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Teacher-student relationships and student outcomes: A systematic second-order meta-analytic review.
IF 17.3 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2025-03-01 Epub Date: 2025-02-10 DOI: 10.1037/bul0000461
Valentin Emslander, Doris Holzberger, Sverre Berg Ofstad, Antoine Fischbach, Ronny Scherer

Teacher-student relationships (TSRs) play a vital role in establishing a positive classroom climate and promoting positive student outcomes. Several meta-analyses have suggested significant correlations between positive TSRs and, for example, academic achievement, motivation, executive functions, and well-being, as well as between negative TSRs that result in behavior problems or bullying. These meta-analyses have differed substantially in TSR-outcome relationships, moderators, and methodological quality, thus complicating the interpretation of these findings. In this preregistered systematic review of meta-analyses plus original second-order meta-analyses (SOMAs), we aimed to (a) synthesize the meta-analytic evidence on relations between TSRs and student outcomes, (b) map influential moderators of these relations, and (c) assess the methodological quality of the meta-analyses. We synthesized over 70 years of educational research across 26 meta-analyses encompassing 119 meta-analytic effect sizes based on approximately 2.64 million prekindergarten and K-12 students. We conducted several three-level SOMAs and found that TSRs had similar large significant relations with eight clusters of student outcomes: academic achievement, academic emotions, appropriate student behavior, behavior problems, executive functions and self-control, motivation, school belonging and engagement, and well-being. The link with bullying was only marginally significant. Our moderator analyses suggested a larger TSR-outcome link for middle and high school students. Although more recent meta-analyses fulfilled more methodological quality criteria, these differences were not associated with TSR-outcome relations. These results map the field of TSR research; present their relations, moderators, and methodological quality in meta-analyses; and show how TSRs are equally important for a wide range of student outcomes and samples. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA, all rights reserved).

{"title":"Teacher-student relationships and student outcomes: A systematic second-order meta-analytic review.","authors":"Valentin Emslander, Doris Holzberger, Sverre Berg Ofstad, Antoine Fischbach, Ronny Scherer","doi":"10.1037/bul0000461","DOIUrl":"10.1037/bul0000461","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Teacher-student relationships (TSRs) play a vital role in establishing a positive classroom climate and promoting positive student outcomes. Several meta-analyses have suggested significant correlations between positive TSRs and, for example, academic achievement, motivation, executive functions, and well-being, as well as between negative TSRs that result in behavior problems or bullying. These meta-analyses have differed substantially in TSR-outcome relationships, moderators, and methodological quality, thus complicating the interpretation of these findings. In this preregistered systematic review of meta-analyses plus original second-order meta-analyses (SOMAs), we aimed to (a) synthesize the meta-analytic evidence on relations between TSRs and student outcomes, (b) map influential moderators of these relations, and (c) assess the methodological quality of the meta-analyses. We synthesized over 70 years of educational research across 26 meta-analyses encompassing 119 meta-analytic effect sizes based on approximately 2.64 million prekindergarten and K-12 students. We conducted several three-level SOMAs and found that TSRs had similar large significant relations with eight clusters of student outcomes: academic achievement, academic emotions, appropriate student behavior, behavior problems, executive functions and self-control, motivation, school belonging and engagement, and well-being. The link with bullying was only marginally significant. Our moderator analyses suggested a larger TSR-outcome link for middle and high school students. Although more recent meta-analyses fulfilled more methodological quality criteria, these differences were not associated with TSR-outcome relations. These results map the field of TSR research; present their relations, moderators, and methodological quality in meta-analyses; and show how TSRs are equally important for a wide range of student outcomes and samples. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":20854,"journal":{"name":"Psychological bulletin","volume":" ","pages":"365-397"},"PeriodicalIF":17.3,"publicationDate":"2025-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143391506","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Mind-wandering increases in frequency over time during task performance: An individual-participant meta-analytic review. 在任务执行过程中,思维游离的频率会随着时间的推移而增加:个人参与者荟萃分析综述。
IF 17.3 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2025-02-01 Epub Date: 2024-02-29 DOI: 10.1037/bul0000424
Anthony P Zanesco, Ekaterina Denkova, Amishi P Jha

Attention has a seemingly inevitable tendency to turn inward toward our thoughts. Mind-wandering refers to moments when this inward focus diverts attention away from the current task-at-hand. Mind-wandering is thought to be ubiquitous, having been estimated to occur between 30% and 50% of our waking moments. Yet, it is unclear whether this frequency is similar within-task performance contexts and unknown whether mind-wandering systematically increases with time-on-task for a broad range of tasks. We conducted a systematic literature search and individual participant data meta-analysis of rates of occurrence of mind-wandering during task performance. Our search located 68 research reports providing almost a half-million total responses to experience sampling mind-wandering probes from more than 10,000 unique individuals. Latent growth curve models estimated the initial occurrence of mind-wandering and linear change in mind-wandering over sequential probes for each study sample, and effects were summarized using multivariate meta-analysis. Our results confirm that mind-wandering increases in frequency over time during task performance, implicating mind-wandering in characteristic within-task psychological changes, such as increasing boredom and patterns of worsening behavioral performance with time-on-task. The systematic search and meta-analysis provide the most comprehensive assessment of normative rates of mind-wandering during task performance reported to date. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA, all rights reserved).

注意力有一种看似不可避免的倾向,那就是向内转向我们的思想。思绪游离指的就是这种内向的注意力偏离当前任务的时刻。思想游离被认为是无处不在的,据估计,在我们清醒的时候,有 30% 到 50% 的时间会出现思想游离。然而,目前还不清楚这一频率在任务表现情境中是否相似,也不清楚在广泛的任务中,思维游离是否会随着任务时间的延长而系统地增加。我们对任务执行过程中的思维游离发生率进行了系统的文献检索和个体参与者数据荟萃分析。通过搜索,我们找到了 68 篇研究报告,这些报告提供了来自 10,000 多名独特个体的近 50 万个对思维游离经验取样探查的总回复。潜在增长曲线模型估算了每个研究样本最初出现的思维游移和思维游移在连续探查中的线性变化,并使用多元荟萃分析法对效果进行了总结。我们的研究结果证实,在任务执行过程中,思维游离的频率会随着时间的推移而增加,这表明思维游离与任务内心理变化的特征有关,如随着任务时间的推移,无聊感会增加,行为表现会恶化。该系统性研究和荟萃分析提供了迄今为止对任务执行过程中思维游离常态率最全面的评估报告。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"Mind-wandering increases in frequency over time during task performance: An individual-participant meta-analytic review.","authors":"Anthony P Zanesco, Ekaterina Denkova, Amishi P Jha","doi":"10.1037/bul0000424","DOIUrl":"10.1037/bul0000424","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Attention has a seemingly inevitable tendency to turn inward toward our thoughts. Mind-wandering refers to moments when this inward focus diverts attention away from the current task-at-hand. Mind-wandering is thought to be ubiquitous, having been estimated to occur between 30% and 50% of our waking moments. Yet, it is unclear whether this frequency is similar within-task performance contexts and unknown whether mind-wandering systematically increases with time-on-task for a broad range of tasks. We conducted a systematic literature search and individual participant data meta-analysis of rates of occurrence of mind-wandering during task performance. Our search located 68 research reports providing almost a half-million total responses to experience sampling mind-wandering probes from more than 10,000 unique individuals. Latent growth curve models estimated the initial occurrence of mind-wandering and linear change in mind-wandering over sequential probes for each study sample, and effects were summarized using multivariate meta-analysis. Our results confirm that mind-wandering increases in frequency over time during task performance, implicating mind-wandering in characteristic within-task psychological changes, such as increasing boredom and patterns of worsening behavioral performance with time-on-task. The systematic search and meta-analysis provide the most comprehensive assessment of normative rates of mind-wandering during task performance reported to date. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":20854,"journal":{"name":"Psychological bulletin","volume":" ","pages":"217-239"},"PeriodicalIF":17.3,"publicationDate":"2025-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139997304","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Getting comfortable with physical discomfort: A scoping review of interoceptive exposure in physical and mental health conditions.
IF 17.3 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2025-02-01 DOI: 10.1037/bul0000464
Samantha G Farris, Lilly Derby, Mindy M Kibbey

Interoceptive exposure (IE) involves the use of exercises, activities, or tasks to intentionally induce (or exacerbate) physical symptoms in the body, to challenge misconceptions about the harmful nature of the physical symptoms that maintain fear and problematic avoidance. IE was originally developed for the cognitive behavioral treatment and prevention of panic disorder. Bodily sensations and concern about physical symptoms are common features in many conditions, not limited to panic disorder. For this reason, IE could be theoretically relevant to cognitive behavioral intervention for many psychological, behavioral, and medical conditions. Yet, IE remains relatively underrecognized and underused as an intervention. Exposure involves feeling discomfort before experiencing relief; thus, it is often perceived as an aversive, unsafe, and illogical intervention because of the seemingly paradoxical approach. We conducted a systematic literature search for a scoping review with the aim of locating published studies on IE to understand how it has been studied beyond panic disorder. Studies focused solely on panic disorder were excluded. We were able to identify and extract data from 132 studies (published between 1992 and 2022), though this published literature is difficult to find. The use of IE has been widely investigated in conditions beyond panic disorder, although evidence for its efficacy is difficult to isolate from other forms of exposure and cognitive behavioral features. There is the strongest evidence for the efficacy of IE as a part of multicomponent cognitive behavioral treatments for posttraumatic stress disorder, health anxiety, irritable bowel syndrome, and to aid in benzodiazepine discontinuation. Interventions that were primarily or exclusively IE-based did not consistently or directly influence claustrophobia fear, separation anxiety, suicidality, insomnia symptoms, cigarette or drug abstinence, or pain-related fear. No serious adverse events were reported in any study. Studies of IE require larger sample sizes, detailed descriptions and rationale of IE exercises, higher IE dosing, extended follow-up assessment, and documentation of safety. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA, all rights reserved).

{"title":"Getting comfortable with physical discomfort: A scoping review of interoceptive exposure in physical and mental health conditions.","authors":"Samantha G Farris, Lilly Derby, Mindy M Kibbey","doi":"10.1037/bul0000464","DOIUrl":"10.1037/bul0000464","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Interoceptive exposure (IE) involves the use of exercises, activities, or tasks to intentionally induce (or exacerbate) physical symptoms in the body, to challenge misconceptions about the harmful nature of the physical symptoms that maintain fear and problematic avoidance. IE was originally developed for the cognitive behavioral treatment and prevention of panic disorder. Bodily sensations and concern about physical symptoms are common features in many conditions, not limited to panic disorder. For this reason, IE could be theoretically relevant to cognitive behavioral intervention for many psychological, behavioral, and medical conditions. Yet, IE remains relatively underrecognized and underused as an intervention. Exposure involves feeling discomfort before experiencing relief; thus, it is often perceived as an aversive, unsafe, and illogical intervention because of the seemingly paradoxical approach. We conducted a systematic literature search for a scoping review with the aim of locating published studies on IE to understand how it has been studied beyond panic disorder. Studies focused solely on panic disorder were excluded. We were able to identify and extract data from 132 studies (published between 1992 and 2022), though this published literature is difficult to find. The use of IE has been widely investigated in conditions beyond panic disorder, although evidence for its efficacy is difficult to isolate from other forms of exposure and cognitive behavioral features. There is the strongest evidence for the efficacy of IE as a part of multicomponent cognitive behavioral treatments for posttraumatic stress disorder, health anxiety, irritable bowel syndrome, and to aid in benzodiazepine discontinuation. Interventions that were primarily or exclusively IE-based did not consistently or directly influence claustrophobia fear, separation anxiety, suicidality, insomnia symptoms, cigarette or drug abstinence, or pain-related fear. No serious adverse events were reported in any study. Studies of IE require larger sample sizes, detailed descriptions and rationale of IE exercises, higher IE dosing, extended follow-up assessment, and documentation of safety. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":20854,"journal":{"name":"Psychological bulletin","volume":"151 2","pages":"131-191"},"PeriodicalIF":17.3,"publicationDate":"2025-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11905771/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143523657","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Does confronting prejudice reduce intergroup bias? A meta-analytic review.
IF 17.3 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2025-02-01 DOI: 10.1037/bul0000466
Chantelle Wood, Sofia Persson, Lilith Roberts, Oliver Allchin, Melanie Simmonds-Buckley

Confronting prejudice is a promising strategy for reducing intergroup bias. The current meta-analysis estimated the effects of confronting prejudice on intergroup bias in the confronted person and examined the impact of potential moderators. Eligible studies measured intergroup bias in participants confronted versus not confronted for intergroup bias. A three-level mixed-effects analysis on 91 effect sizes found a significant, medium-sized effect of confronting prejudice on reducing intergroup bias (g+ = 0.54). There was only limited evidence of publication bias. Confrontation was differentially effective at reducing different types of intergroup bias with a medium-to-large effect on using or endorsing stereotypes, small-to-medium effects on behavior and behavioral intentions, and no significant effects on cognitive prejudice. Effects were otherwise largely robust to differences in confrontation, sample, and study design characteristics. Yet, studies predominantly focused on whether confronting the use of stereotypes reduced subsequent use of stereotypes in artificial settings, and primarily sampled U.S.-based, young, White adults, making it difficult to generalize effects to other forms of intergroup bias and populations, particularly in real-world settings. Studies also tended to measure intergroup bias immediately after confrontation, so the duration of effects over longer periods is less clear. To better evaluate the potential of confrontation as a prejudice reduction technique, future research should examine whether confronting prejudice reduces different forms of intergroup bias in more diverse participant samples and settings, over longer periods, and further test theoretical mediators of these effects. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA, all rights reserved).

{"title":"Does confronting prejudice reduce intergroup bias? A meta-analytic review.","authors":"Chantelle Wood, Sofia Persson, Lilith Roberts, Oliver Allchin, Melanie Simmonds-Buckley","doi":"10.1037/bul0000466","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000466","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Confronting prejudice is a promising strategy for reducing intergroup bias. The current meta-analysis estimated the effects of confronting prejudice on intergroup bias in the confronted person and examined the impact of potential moderators. Eligible studies measured intergroup bias in participants confronted versus not confronted for intergroup bias. A three-level mixed-effects analysis on 91 effect sizes found a significant, medium-sized effect of confronting prejudice on reducing intergroup bias (g<sub>+</sub> = 0.54). There was only limited evidence of publication bias. Confrontation was differentially effective at reducing different types of intergroup bias with a medium-to-large effect on using or endorsing stereotypes, small-to-medium effects on behavior and behavioral intentions, and no significant effects on cognitive prejudice. Effects were otherwise largely robust to differences in confrontation, sample, and study design characteristics. Yet, studies predominantly focused on whether confronting the use of stereotypes reduced subsequent use of stereotypes in artificial settings, and primarily sampled U.S.-based, young, White adults, making it difficult to generalize effects to other forms of intergroup bias and populations, particularly in real-world settings. Studies also tended to measure intergroup bias immediately after confrontation, so the duration of effects over longer periods is less clear. To better evaluate the potential of confrontation as a prejudice reduction technique, future research should examine whether confronting prejudice reduces different forms of intergroup bias in more diverse participant samples and settings, over longer periods, and further test theoretical mediators of these effects. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":20854,"journal":{"name":"Psychological bulletin","volume":"151 2","pages":"192-216"},"PeriodicalIF":17.3,"publicationDate":"2025-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143523538","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Effects of acute exercise on cognitive function: A meta-review of 30 systematic reviews with meta-analyses.
IF 22.4 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2025-01-30 DOI: 10.1037/bul0000460
Yu-Kai Chang,Fei-Fei Ren,Ruei-Hong Li,Jing-Yi Ai,Shih-Chun Kao,Jennifer L Etnier
This meta-review provides the first meta-analytic evidence from published meta-analyses examining the effectiveness of acute exercise interventions on cognitive function. A multilevel meta-analysis with a random-effects model and tests of moderators were performed in R. Thirty systematic reviews with meta-analyses (383 unique studies with 18,347 participants) were identified. Acute exercise significantly improved cognitive function with a small-to-medium effect (N of standardized mean difference [SMD] = 44, mean SMD [M SMD] = 0.33, 95% CI [0.24, 0.42], p < .001). A generalized effect was observed across cognitive domains, showing benefits to tasks identified as attention (M SMD = 0.37), mixed/other (M SMD = 0.36), executive function (M SMD = 0.36), memory (M SMD = 0.23), and information processing (M SMD = 0.20). The timepoint of assessment was a significant moderator (p < .05) with the largest benefits observed when cognitive function was assessed following exercise (M SMD = 0.32). Sample descriptors (i.e., age, cognitive status) and exercise parameters (i.e., intensity, type, duration) did not moderate the positive acute exercise effect on cognitive function (ps > .05). Acute exercise facilitates cognitive function, with the size of the effect varying depending on the timing of assessment in relation to exercise. Notably, these benefits are evident across cognitive domains and occur regardless of participants' characteristics and exercise settings, supporting the adoption of acute exercise for improved cognitive function across the lifespan. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA, all rights reserved).
{"title":"Effects of acute exercise on cognitive function: A meta-review of 30 systematic reviews with meta-analyses.","authors":"Yu-Kai Chang,Fei-Fei Ren,Ruei-Hong Li,Jing-Yi Ai,Shih-Chun Kao,Jennifer L Etnier","doi":"10.1037/bul0000460","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000460","url":null,"abstract":"This meta-review provides the first meta-analytic evidence from published meta-analyses examining the effectiveness of acute exercise interventions on cognitive function. A multilevel meta-analysis with a random-effects model and tests of moderators were performed in R. Thirty systematic reviews with meta-analyses (383 unique studies with 18,347 participants) were identified. Acute exercise significantly improved cognitive function with a small-to-medium effect (N of standardized mean difference [SMD] = 44, mean SMD [M SMD] = 0.33, 95% CI [0.24, 0.42], p < .001). A generalized effect was observed across cognitive domains, showing benefits to tasks identified as attention (M SMD = 0.37), mixed/other (M SMD = 0.36), executive function (M SMD = 0.36), memory (M SMD = 0.23), and information processing (M SMD = 0.20). The timepoint of assessment was a significant moderator (p < .05) with the largest benefits observed when cognitive function was assessed following exercise (M SMD = 0.32). Sample descriptors (i.e., age, cognitive status) and exercise parameters (i.e., intensity, type, duration) did not moderate the positive acute exercise effect on cognitive function (ps > .05). Acute exercise facilitates cognitive function, with the size of the effect varying depending on the timing of assessment in relation to exercise. Notably, these benefits are evident across cognitive domains and occur regardless of participants' characteristics and exercise settings, supporting the adoption of acute exercise for improved cognitive function across the lifespan. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA, all rights reserved).","PeriodicalId":20854,"journal":{"name":"Psychological bulletin","volume":"1 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":22.4,"publicationDate":"2025-01-30","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143062048","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Parents favor daughters: A meta-analysis of gender and other predictors of parental differential treatment. 父母偏爱女儿:性别和父母差别对待的其他预测因素的荟萃分析。
IF 22.4 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2025-01-16 DOI: 10.1037/bul0000458
Alexander C Jensen,McKell A Jorgensen-Wells
Decades of research highlight that differential treatment can have negative developmental consequences, particularly for less favored siblings. Despite this robust body of research, less is known about which children in the family tend to be favored or less favored by parents. The present study examined favored treatment as predicted by birth order, gender, temperament, and personality. We also examined whether links were moderated by multiple factors (i.e., parent gender, age, reporter, domain of parenting/favoritism). Multilevel meta-analysis data were collected from 30 peer-reviewed journal articles and dissertations/theses and 14 other databases. In all, the data reflected 19,469 unique participants (Mage = 19.57, SD = 13.92). Results showed that when favoritism was based on autonomy and control, parents tended to favor older siblings. Further, parents reported favoring daughters. Conscientious and agreeable children also received more favored treatment. For conscientious children, favoritism was strongest when based on differences in conflict (i.e., more conscientious children had relatively less conflict with their parents). Parents and clinicians should be aware of which children in a family tend to be favored as a way of recognizing potentially damaging family patterns. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA, all rights reserved).
几十年的研究强调,差别待遇可能会对孩子的发育产生负面影响,尤其是对不那么受宠爱的兄弟姐妹。尽管有大量的研究,但对于家庭中哪些孩子更受父母青睐,哪些孩子更不受父母青睐,我们知之甚少。本研究考察了出生顺序、性别、气质和个性所预测的优待待遇。我们还研究了这种联系是否受到多种因素(即父母性别、年龄、记者、养育领域/偏袒)的调节。多层元分析数据收集自30篇同行评议的期刊论文和学位论文以及14个其他数据库。总的来说,数据反映了19,469个独立参与者(Mage = 19.57, SD = 13.92)。结果表明,当偏爱是基于自主和控制时,父母倾向于偏爱年长的兄弟姐妹。此外,据报道,父母更偏爱女儿。有责任心和随和的孩子也得到了更多的优待。对于有责任心的孩子来说,基于冲突差异的偏袒是最强烈的(即,更有责任心的孩子与父母的冲突相对较少)。父母和临床医生应该意识到在一个家庭中哪些孩子倾向于被偏爱,以此来识别潜在的有害的家庭模式。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"Parents favor daughters: A meta-analysis of gender and other predictors of parental differential treatment.","authors":"Alexander C Jensen,McKell A Jorgensen-Wells","doi":"10.1037/bul0000458","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000458","url":null,"abstract":"Decades of research highlight that differential treatment can have negative developmental consequences, particularly for less favored siblings. Despite this robust body of research, less is known about which children in the family tend to be favored or less favored by parents. The present study examined favored treatment as predicted by birth order, gender, temperament, and personality. We also examined whether links were moderated by multiple factors (i.e., parent gender, age, reporter, domain of parenting/favoritism). Multilevel meta-analysis data were collected from 30 peer-reviewed journal articles and dissertations/theses and 14 other databases. In all, the data reflected 19,469 unique participants (Mage = 19.57, SD = 13.92). Results showed that when favoritism was based on autonomy and control, parents tended to favor older siblings. Further, parents reported favoring daughters. Conscientious and agreeable children also received more favored treatment. For conscientious children, favoritism was strongest when based on differences in conflict (i.e., more conscientious children had relatively less conflict with their parents). Parents and clinicians should be aware of which children in a family tend to be favored as a way of recognizing potentially damaging family patterns. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA, all rights reserved).","PeriodicalId":20854,"journal":{"name":"Psychological bulletin","volume":"31 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":22.4,"publicationDate":"2025-01-16","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142989141","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Cognitive factors underlying mathematical skills: A systematic review and meta-analysis. 数学技能背后的认知因素:系统回顾与元分析。
IF 17.3 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2025-01-01 Epub Date: 2024-12-19 DOI: 10.1037/bul0000457
Tonje Amland, Germán Grande, Ronny Scherer, Arne Lervåg, Monica Melby-Lervåg

In understanding the nature of mathematical skills, the most influential theories suggest that mathematical cognition draws on different systems: numerical, linguistic, spatial, and general cognitive skills. Studies show that skills in these areas are highly predictive of outcomes in mathematics. Nonetheless, the strength of these relations with mathematical achievement varies, and little is known about the moderators or relative importance of each predictor. Based on 269 concurrent and 174 longitudinal studies comprising 2,696 correlations, this meta-analysis summarizes the evidence on cognitive predictors of mathematical skills in children and adolescents. The results showed that nonsymbolic number skills (often labeled approximate number sense) correlate significantly less with mathematical achievement than symbolic number skills and that various aspects of language relate differently to mathematical outcomes. We observed differential predictive patterns for arithmetic and word problems, and these patterns only partly supported the theory of three pathways-quantitative, linguistic, and spatial-for mathematical skills. Concurrently, nonsymbolic number and phonological skills were weak but exclusive predictors of arithmetic skills, whereas nonverbal intelligence quotient (IQ) predicted word problems only. Only symbolic number skills predicted both arithmetic and word problems concurrently. Longitudinally, symbolic number skills, spatial ability, and nonverbal IQ predicted both arithmetic and word problems, whereas language comprehension was important for word problem solving only. As in the concurrent data, nonsymbolic number skill was a weak longitudinal predictor of arithmetic skills. We conclude that the candidates to target in intervention studies are symbolic number skills and language comprehension. It is uncertain whether the two other important predictors, nonverbal IQ and spatial skills, are actually malleable. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA, all rights reserved).

在理解数学技能的本质时,最具影响力的理论认为,数学认知依赖于不同的系统:数字、语言、空间和一般认知技能。研究表明,这些领域的技能可以高度预测数学成绩。尽管如此,这些与数学成绩的关系的强度各不相同,并且对每个预测因子的调节因子或相对重要性知之甚少。基于269项并行研究和174项纵向研究,包括2696项相关性,本荟萃分析总结了儿童和青少年数学技能认知预测因素的证据。结果表明,非符号数技能(通常被称为近似数感)与数学成绩的相关性明显低于符号数技能,语言的各个方面与数学成绩的相关性不同。我们观察到算术和文字问题的不同预测模式,这些模式只部分支持数学技能的三种途径理论——定量的、语言的和空间的。同时,非符号数字和语音技能是算术技能的弱但唯一的预测因素,而非语言智商(IQ)只预测单词问题。只有符号数技能可以同时预测算术和文字问题。纵向上,符号数技能、空间能力和非语言智商预测算术和文字问题,而语言理解仅对解决文字问题重要。在并发数据中,非符号数技能是算术技能的弱纵向预测因子。我们认为干预研究的目标是符号数技能和语言理解。另外两个重要的预测指标——非语言智商和空间能力——是否具有可塑性尚不确定。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"Cognitive factors underlying mathematical skills: A systematic review and meta-analysis.","authors":"Tonje Amland, Germán Grande, Ronny Scherer, Arne Lervåg, Monica Melby-Lervåg","doi":"10.1037/bul0000457","DOIUrl":"10.1037/bul0000457","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>In understanding the nature of mathematical skills, the most influential theories suggest that mathematical cognition draws on different systems: numerical, linguistic, spatial, and general cognitive skills. Studies show that skills in these areas are highly predictive of outcomes in mathematics. Nonetheless, the strength of these relations with mathematical achievement varies, and little is known about the moderators or relative importance of each predictor. Based on 269 concurrent and 174 longitudinal studies comprising 2,696 correlations, this meta-analysis summarizes the evidence on cognitive predictors of mathematical skills in children and adolescents. The results showed that nonsymbolic number skills (often labeled approximate number sense) correlate significantly less with mathematical achievement than symbolic number skills and that various aspects of language relate differently to mathematical outcomes. We observed differential predictive patterns for arithmetic and word problems, and these patterns only partly supported the theory of three pathways-quantitative, linguistic, and spatial-for mathematical skills. Concurrently, nonsymbolic number and phonological skills were weak but exclusive predictors of arithmetic skills, whereas nonverbal intelligence quotient (IQ) predicted word problems only. Only symbolic number skills predicted both arithmetic and word problems concurrently. Longitudinally, symbolic number skills, spatial ability, and nonverbal IQ predicted both arithmetic and word problems, whereas language comprehension was important for word problem solving only. As in the concurrent data, nonsymbolic number skill was a weak longitudinal predictor of arithmetic skills. We conclude that the candidates to target in intervention studies are symbolic number skills and language comprehension. It is uncertain whether the two other important predictors, nonverbal IQ and spatial skills, are actually malleable. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":20854,"journal":{"name":"Psychological bulletin","volume":" ","pages":"88-129"},"PeriodicalIF":17.3,"publicationDate":"2025-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142855247","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Psychological bulletin
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1