首页 > 最新文献

Psychological bulletin最新文献

英文 中文
The stability of cognitive abilities: A meta-analytic review of longitudinal studies. 认知能力的稳定性:纵向研究的元分析回顾。
IF 22.4 1区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-04-01 Epub Date: 2024-02-08 DOI: 10.1037/bul0000425
Moritz Breit, Vsevolod Scherrer, Elliot M Tucker-Drob, Franzis Preckel

Cognitive abilities, including general intelligence and domain-specific abilities such as fluid reasoning, comprehension knowledge, working memory capacity, and processing speed, are regarded as some of the most stable psychological traits, yet there exist no large-scale systematic efforts to document the specific patterns by which their rank-order stability changes over age and time interval, or how their stability differs across abilities, tests, and populations. Determining the conditions under which cognitive abilities exhibit high or low degrees of stability is critical not just to theory development but to applied contexts in which cognitive assessments guide decisions regarding treatment and intervention decisions with lasting consequences for individuals. In order to supplement this important area of research, we present a meta-analysis of longitudinal studies investigating the stability of cognitive abilities. The meta-analysis relied on data from 205 longitudinal studies that involved a total of 87,408 participants, resulting in 1,288 test-retest correlation coefficients among manifest variables. For an age of 20 years and a test-retest interval of 5 years, we found a mean rank-order stability of ρ = .76. The effect of mean sample age on stability was best described by a negative exponential function, with low stability in preschool children, rapid increases in stability in childhood, and consistently high stability from late adolescence to late adulthood. This same functional form continued to best describe age trends in stability after adjusting for test reliability. Stability declined with increasing test-retest interval. This decrease flattened out from an interval of approximately 5 years onward. According to the age and interval moderation models, minimum stability sufficient for individual-level diagnostic decisions (rtt = .80) can only be expected over the age of 7 and for short time intervals in children. In adults, stability levels meeting this criterion are obtained for over 5 years. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

认知能力,包括一般智力和特定领域的能力,如流畅推理、理解知识、工作记忆能力和处理速度,被认为是一些最稳定的心理特征,但目前还没有大规模的系统性工作来记录它们的等级稳定性随年龄和时间间隔变化的具体模式,或者它们的稳定性在不同能力、测试和人群之间有何差异。确定认知能力表现出高度或低度稳定性的条件不仅对理论发展至关重要,而且对应用环境也至关重要,因为在应用环境中,认知评估会指导治疗和干预决策,从而对个人产生持久的影响。为了补充这一重要研究领域,我们对认知能力稳定性的纵向研究进行了荟萃分析。荟萃分析依赖于 205 项纵向研究的数据,这些研究共涉及 87,408 名参与者,得出了 1,288 个表现变量之间的测试-重复相关系数。对于年龄为 20 岁、间隔 5 年的重测,我们发现平均秩序稳定性为 ρ = .76。平均样本年龄对稳定性的影响用一个负指数函数来描述最为恰当,学龄前儿童的稳定性较低,儿童期的稳定性快速上升,从青春期后期到成年期后期的稳定性一直较高。在对测验信度进行调整后,同样的函数形式仍能最好地描述稳定性的年龄趋势。稳定性随着测试-重复测试间隔的增加而下降。从大约 5 年的间隔开始,这种下降趋于平缓。根据年龄和间隔调节模型,只有在儿童 7 岁以上且间隔时间较短的情况下,才有望获得足以做出个人诊断决定的最低稳定性(rtt = .80)。在成人中,符合这一标准的稳定性水平可达到 5 年以上。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"The stability of cognitive abilities: A meta-analytic review of longitudinal studies.","authors":"Moritz Breit, Vsevolod Scherrer, Elliot M Tucker-Drob, Franzis Preckel","doi":"10.1037/bul0000425","DOIUrl":"10.1037/bul0000425","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Cognitive abilities, including general intelligence and domain-specific abilities such as fluid reasoning, comprehension knowledge, working memory capacity, and processing speed, are regarded as some of the most stable psychological traits, yet there exist no large-scale systematic efforts to document the specific patterns by which their rank-order stability changes over age and time interval, or how their stability differs across abilities, tests, and populations. Determining the conditions under which cognitive abilities exhibit high or low degrees of stability is critical not just to theory development but to applied contexts in which cognitive assessments guide decisions regarding treatment and intervention decisions with lasting consequences for individuals. In order to supplement this important area of research, we present a meta-analysis of longitudinal studies investigating the stability of cognitive abilities. The meta-analysis relied on data from 205 longitudinal studies that involved a total of 87,408 participants, resulting in 1,288 test-retest correlation coefficients among manifest variables. For an age of 20 years and a test-retest interval of 5 years, we found a mean rank-order stability of ρ = .76. The effect of mean sample age on stability was best described by a negative exponential function, with low stability in preschool children, rapid increases in stability in childhood, and consistently high stability from late adolescence to late adulthood. This same functional form continued to best describe age trends in stability after adjusting for test reliability. Stability declined with increasing test-retest interval. This decrease flattened out from an interval of approximately 5 years onward. According to the age and interval moderation models, minimum stability sufficient for individual-level diagnostic decisions (<i>r<sub>tt</sub></i> = .80) can only be expected over the age of 7 and for short time intervals in children. In adults, stability levels meeting this criterion are obtained for over 5 years. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":20854,"journal":{"name":"Psychological bulletin","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":22.4,"publicationDate":"2024-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139707691","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Sleep loss and emotion: A systematic review and meta-analysis of over 50 years of experimental research. 失眠与情绪:对 50 多年实验研究的系统回顾和荟萃分析。
IF 22.4 1区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-04-01 Epub Date: 2023-12-21 DOI: 10.1037/bul0000410
Cara A Palmer, Joanne L Bower, Kit W Cho, Michelle A Clementi, Simon Lau, Benjamin Oosterhoff, Candice A Alfano

In a largely sleep-deprived society, quantifying the effects of sleep loss on emotion is critical for promoting psychological health. This preregistered systematic review and meta-analysis quantified the effects of various forms of sleep loss on multiple aspects of emotional experiences. Eligible studies used experimental reductions of sleep via total sleep deprivation, partial sleep restriction, or sleep fragmentation in healthy populations to examine effects on positive affect, negative affect, general mood disturbances, emotional reactivity, anxiety symptoms, and/or depressive symptoms. In total, 1,338 effect sizes across 154 studies were included (N = 5,717; participant age range = 7-79 years). Random effects models were conducted, and all forms of sleep loss resulted in reduced positive affect (standardized mean difference [SMD] = -0.27 to -1.14), increased anxiety symptoms (SMD = 0.57-0.63), and blunted arousal in response to emotional stimuli (SMD = -0.20 to -0.53). Findings for negative affect, reports of emotional valence in response to emotional stimuli, and depressive symptoms were mixed and depended on the type of sleep loss. Nonlinear effects for the amount of sleep loss as well as differences based on the stage of sleep restricted (i.e., rapid eye movement sleep or slow-wave sleep) were also detected. This study represents the most comprehensive quantitative synthesis of experimental sleep and emotion research to date and provides strong evidence that periods of extended wakefulness, shortened sleep duration, and/or nighttime awakenings adversely influence human emotional functioning. Findings provide an integrative foundation for future research on sleep and emotion and elucidate the precise ways that inadequate sleep may impact our daytime emotional lives. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

在一个睡眠严重不足的社会中,量化睡眠不足对情绪的影响对于促进心理健康至关重要。这项预先登记的系统综述和荟萃分析量化了各种形式的睡眠不足对情绪体验多个方面的影响。符合条件的研究在健康人群中采用完全剥夺睡眠、部分限制睡眠或睡眠碎片化等实验性减少睡眠的方法,以考察对积极情绪、消极情绪、一般情绪障碍、情绪反应性、焦虑症状和/或抑郁症状的影响。共纳入了 154 项研究的 1,338 个效应大小(N = 5,717; 参与者年龄范围 = 7-79 岁)。随机效应模型显示,所有形式的失眠都会导致积极情绪降低(标准化平均差 [SMD] = -0.27至-1.14)、焦虑症状增加(SMD = 0.57至0.63)以及对情绪刺激的唤醒迟钝(SMD = -0.20至-0.53)。关于消极情绪、对情绪刺激的情绪价位反应和抑郁症状的研究结果不一,并取决于睡眠不足的类型。研究还发现了睡眠损失量的非线性效应以及睡眠受限阶段(即快速眼动睡眠或慢波睡眠)的差异。这项研究是迄今为止对实验性睡眠和情绪研究进行的最全面的定量综述,它提供了强有力的证据,证明延长觉醒时间、缩短睡眠时间和/或夜间觉醒会对人类的情绪功能产生不利影响。研究结果为今后的睡眠与情绪研究奠定了综合基础,并阐明了睡眠不足可能影响我们白天情绪生活的确切方式。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Sleep loss and emotion: A systematic review and meta-analysis of over 50 years of experimental research.","authors":"Cara A Palmer, Joanne L Bower, Kit W Cho, Michelle A Clementi, Simon Lau, Benjamin Oosterhoff, Candice A Alfano","doi":"10.1037/bul0000410","DOIUrl":"10.1037/bul0000410","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>In a largely sleep-deprived society, quantifying the effects of sleep loss on emotion is critical for promoting psychological health. This preregistered systematic review and meta-analysis quantified the effects of various forms of sleep loss on multiple aspects of emotional experiences. Eligible studies used experimental reductions of sleep via total sleep deprivation, partial sleep restriction, or sleep fragmentation in healthy populations to examine effects on positive affect, negative affect, general mood disturbances, emotional reactivity, anxiety symptoms, and/or depressive symptoms. In total, 1,338 effect sizes across 154 studies were included (<i>N</i> = 5,717; participant age range = 7-79 years). Random effects models were conducted, and all forms of sleep loss resulted in reduced positive affect (standardized mean difference [SMD] = -0.27 to -1.14), increased anxiety symptoms (SMD = 0.57-0.63), and blunted arousal in response to emotional stimuli (SMD = -0.20 to -0.53). Findings for negative affect, reports of emotional valence in response to emotional stimuli, and depressive symptoms were mixed and depended on the type of sleep loss. Nonlinear effects for the amount of sleep loss as well as differences based on the stage of sleep restricted (i.e., rapid eye movement sleep or slow-wave sleep) were also detected. This study represents the most comprehensive quantitative synthesis of experimental sleep and emotion research to date and provides strong evidence that periods of extended wakefulness, shortened sleep duration, and/or nighttime awakenings adversely influence human emotional functioning. Findings provide an integrative foundation for future research on sleep and emotion and elucidate the precise ways that inadequate sleep may impact our daytime emotional lives. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":20854,"journal":{"name":"Psychological bulletin","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":22.4,"publicationDate":"2024-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138831297","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Wipe it off: A meta-analytic review of the psychological consequences and antecedents of physical cleansing. 擦干净:对身体清洁的心理后果和前因后果的元分析回顾。
IF 22.4 1区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-04-01 Epub Date: 2024-02-15 DOI: 10.1037/bul0000421
Spike W S Lee, Kathleen Chen, Cecilia Ma, Joe Hoang

Physical cleansing is a human universal. It serves health and survival functions. It also carries rich psychological meanings that interest scholars across disciplines. What psychological effects result from cleansing? What psychological states trigger cleansing? The present meta-analysis takes stock of all experimental studies examining the psychological consequences and antecedents of cleansing-related thoughts, feelings, and behaviors (e.g., feeling less guilty after cleansing; spontaneously cleansing oneself after thinking of unwelcomed sexual encounter). It includes 129 records, 230 experiments, and 551 effects from 42,793 participants. Effect sizes were synthesized in random-effects models using robust variance estimates with small-sample corrections, supplemented by other techniques. Outliers were excluded using leave-one-out diagnostics and sensitivity analysis. Publication bias was assessed and corrected for using eight methods. Theoretical, methodological, sample, and report moderators were coded. After excluding outliers, without bias correction, the synthesized effect size estimate was g = 0.315, 95% CI [0.277, 0.354]. Using various bias correction methods, the estimate ranged from g = 0.103 to 0.331 and always exhibited considerable heterogeneity. Effect sizes were especially large for behavioral measures and varied significantly between sample types, sample regions, and report types. Meanwhile, effects were domain-general (observed in the moral domain and beyond), bidirectional (physical cleansing ↔ psychological variables), and robust across theoretical types, manipulation operationalizations, and study designs. Limitations included mixed replicability, suboptimal methodological rigor, and restricted sample diversity. We recommend future studies to (a) incorporate power analysis, preregistration, and replication; (b) investigate generalizability across samples; (c) strengthen discriminant validity; and (d) test competing theoretical accounts. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

身体清洁是人类的普遍现象。它具有健康和生存功能。它还蕴含着丰富的心理意义,引起了各学科学者的兴趣。清洁会产生哪些心理影响?哪些心理状态会引发洁癖?本荟萃分析总结了所有与洁净相关的想法、感受和行为(例如,洁净后内疚感减轻;想到不受欢迎的性接触后自发洁净自己)的心理后果和前因后果的实验研究。它包括来自 42,793 名参与者的 129 项记录、230 项实验和 551 项效应。在随机效应模型中,利用稳健方差估计和小样本校正,并辅以其他技术,对效应大小进行了综合。使用留一诊断和敏感性分析排除了异常值。采用八种方法对发表偏倚进行评估和校正。对理论、方法、样本和报告调节因子进行了编码。排除异常值后,在未进行偏倚校正的情况下,综合效应大小估计值为 g = 0.315,95% CI [0.277, 0.354]。使用各种偏差校正方法后,估计值从 g = 0.103 到 0.331 不等,并且始终表现出相当大的异质性。行为测量的效应大小尤其大,并且在样本类型、样本地区和报告类型之间存在显著差异。同时,效应具有领域普遍性(可在道德领域及其他领域观察到)、双向性(身体清洁 ↔ 心理变量),并且在不同理论类型、操纵操作和研究设计中都很稳健。局限性包括可复制性参差不齐、方法不够严谨以及样本多样性受限。我们建议今后的研究应:(a)结合功率分析、预先登记和复制;(b)调查不同样本的普遍性;(c)加强判别有效性;以及(d)测试相互竞争的理论观点。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Wipe it off: A meta-analytic review of the psychological consequences and antecedents of physical cleansing.","authors":"Spike W S Lee, Kathleen Chen, Cecilia Ma, Joe Hoang","doi":"10.1037/bul0000421","DOIUrl":"10.1037/bul0000421","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Physical cleansing is a human universal. It serves health and survival functions. It also carries rich psychological meanings that interest scholars across disciplines. What psychological effects result from cleansing? What psychological states trigger cleansing? The present meta-analysis takes stock of all experimental studies examining the psychological consequences and antecedents of cleansing-related thoughts, feelings, and behaviors (e.g., feeling less guilty after cleansing; spontaneously cleansing oneself after thinking of unwelcomed sexual encounter). It includes 129 records, 230 experiments, and 551 effects from 42,793 participants. Effect sizes were synthesized in random-effects models using robust variance estimates with small-sample corrections, supplemented by other techniques. Outliers were excluded using leave-one-out diagnostics and sensitivity analysis. Publication bias was assessed and corrected for using eight methods. Theoretical, methodological, sample, and report moderators were coded. After excluding outliers, without bias correction, the synthesized effect size estimate was <i>g</i> = 0.315, 95% CI [0.277, 0.354]. Using various bias correction methods, the estimate ranged from <i>g</i> = 0.103 to 0.331 and always exhibited considerable heterogeneity. Effect sizes were especially large for behavioral measures and varied significantly between sample types, sample regions, and report types. Meanwhile, effects were domain-general (observed in the moral domain and beyond), bidirectional (physical cleansing ↔ psychological variables), and robust across theoretical types, manipulation operationalizations, and study designs. Limitations included mixed replicability, suboptimal methodological rigor, and restricted sample diversity. We recommend future studies to (a) incorporate power analysis, preregistration, and replication; (b) investigate generalizability across samples; (c) strengthen discriminant validity; and (d) test competing theoretical accounts. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":20854,"journal":{"name":"Psychological bulletin","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":22.4,"publicationDate":"2024-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139735950","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
A meta-analytic review of the relation between spatial anxiety and spatial skills. 空间焦虑与空间技能之间关系的元分析综述。
IF 22.4 1区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-04-01 Epub Date: 2024-02-08 DOI: 10.1037/bul0000420
Elyssa A Geer, Connie Barroso, Rachel A Conlon, Jamie M Dasher, Colleen M Ganley

Spatial skills are key predictors of achievement in science, technology, engineering, and mathematics disciplines, despite being acquired through everyday life and not formally taught in schools. Spatial skills include a diverse group of abilities broadly related to reasoning about properties of space such as distance and direction. Recently, more research has investigated the link between spatial skills and spatial anxiety, defined as a fear or apprehension felt when engaged in spatial thinking. There has yet to be a meta-analytic review summarizing these findings. Thus, the goal of this preregistered meta-analytic review is to provide an estimate of the size of the relation between spatial anxiety and spatial skills while considering several moderators (grade/age group, sex, spatial skills measure/subtype, spatial anxiety measure/subtype, geographical region of sample, publication type/year, and risk of bias). Analyzing 283 effect sizes accumulated from research conducted between 1994 and 2020, we found a small, negative, and statistically significant (r = -.14) correlation between spatial anxiety and spatial skills. Results showed that effect sizes including mental manipulation anxiety, scalar comparison anxiety, and navigation skill were often significantly stronger than effect sizes including measures of other subtypes. The magnitude of the relation was not significantly different in children and adults, though effect sizes tended to be weaker for younger samples (r = -.08). Our results are consistent with previous findings of a significant relation between spatial anxiety and skills, and this work bridges a gap in the existing research, lending support to future research efforts investigating spatial cognition. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

空间技能是预测科学、技术、工程和数学学科成绩的关键因素,尽管这些技能是在日常生活中习得的,学校并没有正式教授。空间技能包括与推理空间属性(如距离和方向)广泛相关的各种能力。最近,越来越多的研究调查了空间技能与空间焦虑之间的联系,空间焦虑是指在进行空间思考时感到的恐惧或不安。目前还没有对这些研究结果进行荟萃分析的综述。因此,本预注册荟萃分析综述的目的是对空间焦虑与空间技能之间关系的规模进行估计,同时考虑几个调节因素(年级/年龄组、性别、空间技能测量/子类型、空间焦虑测量/子类型、样本的地理区域、出版物类型/年份和偏差风险)。通过分析 1994 年至 2020 年间进行的研究中积累的 283 个效应大小,我们发现空间焦虑与空间技能之间存在微小的负相关,且在统计学上具有显著性(r = -.14)。结果显示,包括心理操纵焦虑、标度比较焦虑和导航技能在内的效应量往往明显强于包括其他亚型的效应量。在儿童和成人中,这种关系的程度并无明显差异,但在年龄较小的样本中,效应大小往往较弱(r = -.08)。我们的研究结果与之前关于空间焦虑与技能之间存在显著关系的研究结果一致,这项研究填补了现有研究的空白,为未来调查空间认知的研究工作提供了支持。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"A meta-analytic review of the relation between spatial anxiety and spatial skills.","authors":"Elyssa A Geer, Connie Barroso, Rachel A Conlon, Jamie M Dasher, Colleen M Ganley","doi":"10.1037/bul0000420","DOIUrl":"10.1037/bul0000420","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Spatial skills are key predictors of achievement in science, technology, engineering, and mathematics disciplines, despite being acquired through everyday life and not formally taught in schools. Spatial skills include a diverse group of abilities broadly related to reasoning about properties of space such as distance and direction. Recently, more research has investigated the link between spatial skills and spatial anxiety, defined as a fear or apprehension felt when engaged in spatial thinking. There has yet to be a meta-analytic review summarizing these findings. Thus, the goal of this preregistered meta-analytic review is to provide an estimate of the size of the relation between spatial anxiety and spatial skills while considering several moderators (grade/age group, sex, spatial skills measure/subtype, spatial anxiety measure/subtype, geographical region of sample, publication type/year, and risk of bias). Analyzing 283 effect sizes accumulated from research conducted between 1994 and 2020, we found a small, negative, and statistically significant (<i>r</i> = -.14) correlation between spatial anxiety and spatial skills. Results showed that effect sizes including mental manipulation anxiety, scalar comparison anxiety, and navigation skill were often significantly stronger than effect sizes including measures of other subtypes. The magnitude of the relation was not significantly different in children and adults, though effect sizes tended to be weaker for younger samples (<i>r</i> = -.08). Our results are consistent with previous findings of a significant relation between spatial anxiety and skills, and this work bridges a gap in the existing research, lending support to future research efforts investigating spatial cognition. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":20854,"journal":{"name":"Psychological bulletin","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":22.4,"publicationDate":"2024-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139707643","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Project harmony: A systematic review and network meta-analysis of psychotherapy and pharmacologic trials for comorbid posttraumatic stress, alcohol, and other drug use disorders. 和谐项目:对共病创伤后应激、酒精和其他药物使用障碍的心理治疗和药理学试验进行系统回顾和网络荟萃分析。
IF 22.4 1区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-03-01 Epub Date: 2023-11-16 DOI: 10.1037/bul0000409
Denise A Hien, Santiago Papini, Lissette M Saavedra, Alexandria G Bauer, Lesia M Ruglass, Chantel T Ebrahimi, Skye Fitzpatrick, Teresa López-Castro, Sonya B Norman, Therese K Killeen, Sudie E Back, Antonio A Morgan-López

We conducted a systematic review and network meta-analyses (NMA) of psychotherapy and pharmacologic treatments for individuals with co-occurring posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) and alcohol or other drug use disorder (AOD). A comprehensive search spanning 1995-2019 yielded a pool of 39 studies for systematic review, including 24 randomized controlled trials for the NMA. Study interventions were grouped by target of treatment (PTSD + AOD, PTSD-only, and AOD-only) and approach (psychotherapy or medication). Standardized mean differences (SMD) from the NMA yielded evidence that at the end of treatment, integrated, trauma-focused therapy for PTSD + AOD was more effective at reducing PTSD symptoms than integrated, non-trauma-focused therapy (SMD = -0.30), AOD-focused psychotherapy (SMD = -0.29), and other control psychotherapies (SMD = -0.43). End-of-treatment alcohol use severity was less for AOD medication compared to placebo medication (SMD = -0.36) and trauma-focused therapy for PTSD + placebo medication (SMD = -0.67), and less for trauma-focused psychotherapy + AOD medication compared to PTSD medication (SMD = -0.53), placebo medication (SMD = -0.50), and trauma-focused psychotherapy + placebo medication (SMD = -0.81). Key limitations include the small number of studies in the NMA for pharmacologic treatments and the lack of demographic diversity apparent in the existing literature. Findings suggest room for new studies that can address limitations in study sample composition, sample sizes, retention, and apply new techniques for conducting comparative effectiveness in PTSD + AOD treatment. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

我们对同时发生创伤后应激障碍(PTSD)和酒精或其他药物使用障碍(AOD)的个体进行了心理治疗和药物治疗的系统回顾和网络荟萃分析(NMA)。一项从1995年到2019年的全面搜索产生了39项研究供系统评价,其中包括24项针对NMA的随机对照试验。研究干预措施按治疗目标(PTSD + AOD、仅PTSD和仅AOD)和方法(心理治疗或药物治疗)分组。来自NMA的标准化平均差异(SMD)证明,在治疗结束时,创伤为重点的PTSD + AOD综合治疗在减轻PTSD症状方面比非创伤为重点的综合治疗(SMD = -0.30)、以AOD为重点的心理治疗(SMD = -0.29)和其他对照心理治疗(SMD = -0.43)更有效。治疗结束时,AOD治疗的酒精使用严重程度低于安慰剂治疗(SMD = -0.36)和创伤性PTSD +安慰剂治疗(SMD = -0.67),创伤性心理治疗+ AOD治疗的酒精使用严重程度低于PTSD治疗(SMD = -0.53)、安慰剂治疗(SMD = -0.50)和创伤性心理治疗+安慰剂治疗(SMD = -0.81)。主要的限制包括NMA中关于药物治疗的研究数量较少,以及现有文献中缺乏明显的人口统计学多样性。研究结果表明,新的研究可以解决研究样本组成、样本量、保留的局限性,并应用新技术来进行PTSD + AOD治疗的比较效果。(PsycInfo数据库记录(c) 2023 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"Project harmony: A systematic review and network meta-analysis of psychotherapy and pharmacologic trials for comorbid posttraumatic stress, alcohol, and other drug use disorders.","authors":"Denise A Hien, Santiago Papini, Lissette M Saavedra, Alexandria G Bauer, Lesia M Ruglass, Chantel T Ebrahimi, Skye Fitzpatrick, Teresa López-Castro, Sonya B Norman, Therese K Killeen, Sudie E Back, Antonio A Morgan-López","doi":"10.1037/bul0000409","DOIUrl":"10.1037/bul0000409","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>We conducted a systematic review and network meta-analyses (NMA) of psychotherapy and pharmacologic treatments for individuals with co-occurring posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) and alcohol or other drug use disorder (AOD). A comprehensive search spanning 1995-2019 yielded a pool of 39 studies for systematic review, including 24 randomized controlled trials for the NMA. Study interventions were grouped by target of treatment (PTSD + AOD, PTSD-only, and AOD-only) and approach (psychotherapy or medication). Standardized mean differences (SMD) from the NMA yielded evidence that at the end of treatment, integrated, trauma-focused therapy for PTSD + AOD was more effective at reducing PTSD symptoms than integrated, non-trauma-focused therapy (SMD = -0.30), AOD-focused psychotherapy (SMD = -0.29), and other control psychotherapies (SMD = -0.43). End-of-treatment alcohol use severity was less for AOD medication compared to placebo medication (SMD = -0.36) and trauma-focused therapy for PTSD + placebo medication (SMD = -0.67), and less for trauma-focused psychotherapy + AOD medication compared to PTSD medication (SMD = -0.53), placebo medication (SMD = -0.50), and trauma-focused psychotherapy + placebo medication (SMD = -0.81). Key limitations include the small number of studies in the NMA for pharmacologic treatments and the lack of demographic diversity apparent in the existing literature. Findings suggest room for new studies that can address limitations in study sample composition, sample sizes, retention, and apply new techniques for conducting comparative effectiveness in PTSD + AOD treatment. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":20854,"journal":{"name":"Psychological bulletin","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":22.4,"publicationDate":"2024-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10939977/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"136398978","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Religiosity predicts prosociality, especially when measured by self-report: A meta-analysis of almost 60 years of research. 宗教信仰可预测亲社会性,尤其是通过自我报告进行测量时:对近 60 年研究的荟萃分析。
IF 22.4 1区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-03-01 Epub Date: 2024-02-26 DOI: 10.1037/bul0000413
John Michael Kelly, Stephanie R Kramer, Azim F Shariff

This meta-analysis explores the long-standing and heavily debated question of whether religiosity is associated with prosocial and antisocial behavior at the individual level. In an analysis of 701 effects across 237 samples, encompassing 811,663 participants, a significant relationship of r = .13 was found between religiosity and prosociality (and antisociality, which was treated as its inverse). Nevertheless, there was substantial heterogeneity of effect sizes, and several potential moderators were explored. The effect was most heavily moderated by the type of measurement used to assess prosocial or antisocial behavior. Religiosity correlated more strongly with self-reported prosociality (r = .15) than with directly measured prosocial behavior (r = .06). Three possible interpretations of this moderation are discussed, namely, that (a) lab-based methods do not accurately or fully capture actual religious prosociality; (b) the self-report effect is explained by religious self-enhancement and overreports actual prosociality; or (c) both religiosity and self-reported prosociality are explained by self-enhancement. The question of whether religiosity more strongly positively predicts prosociality or negatively predicts antisociality is also explored. This moderation is, at most, weak. We test additional potential moderators, including the aspect of religiosity and type of behavior measured, the ingroup or outgroup nature of the recipient, and study characteristics. Finally, we recommend a shift in how researchers investigate questions of religiosity and prosociality in the future. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

本荟萃分析探讨了宗教信仰是否与个人层面的亲社会行为和反社会行为相关这一长期存在且备受争议的问题。在对 237 个样本、811663 名参与者的 701 个效应进行分析后发现,宗教信仰与亲社会性(以及作为其反义词的反社会性)之间存在 r = .13 的显著关系。然而,效应大小存在很大的异质性,研究人员对几个潜在的调节因素进行了探讨。用于评估亲社会或反社会行为的测量类型对该效应的调节作用最大。与直接测量的亲社会行为(r = .06)相比,宗教信仰与自我报告的亲社会性(r = .15)的相关性更强。我们讨论了对这种调节作用的三种可能解释,即:(a)基于实验室的方法不能准确或全面地捕捉到实际的宗教亲社会性;(b)自我报告效应是由宗教自我提升和过度报告实际亲社会性所解释的;或(c)宗教信仰和自我报告的亲社会性都是由自我提升所解释的。此外,我们还探讨了宗教信仰对亲社会性的预测是更积极还是更消极的问题。这种调节作用最多是微弱的。我们还测试了其他潜在的调节因素,包括宗教信仰的方面和所测量的行为类型、受试者的内群体或外群体性质以及研究特点。最后,我们建议研究人员改变今后研究宗教信仰与亲社会性问题的方式。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Religiosity predicts prosociality, especially when measured by self-report: A meta-analysis of almost 60 years of research.","authors":"John Michael Kelly, Stephanie R Kramer, Azim F Shariff","doi":"10.1037/bul0000413","DOIUrl":"10.1037/bul0000413","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>This meta-analysis explores the long-standing and heavily debated question of whether religiosity is associated with prosocial and antisocial behavior at the individual level. In an analysis of 701 effects across 237 samples, encompassing 811,663 participants, a significant relationship of <i>r</i> = .13 was found between religiosity and prosociality (and antisociality, which was treated as its inverse). Nevertheless, there was substantial heterogeneity of effect sizes, and several potential moderators were explored. The effect was most heavily moderated by the type of measurement used to assess prosocial or antisocial behavior. Religiosity correlated more strongly with self-reported prosociality (<i>r</i> = .15) than with directly measured prosocial behavior (<i>r</i> = .06). Three possible interpretations of this moderation are discussed, namely, that (a) lab-based methods do not accurately or fully capture actual religious prosociality; (b) the self-report effect is explained by religious self-enhancement and overreports actual prosociality; or (c) both religiosity and self-reported prosociality are explained by self-enhancement. The question of whether religiosity more strongly positively predicts prosociality or negatively predicts antisociality is also explored. This moderation is, at most, weak. We test additional potential moderators, including the aspect of religiosity and type of behavior measured, the ingroup or outgroup nature of the recipient, and study characteristics. Finally, we recommend a shift in how researchers investigate questions of religiosity and prosociality in the future. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":20854,"journal":{"name":"Psychological bulletin","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":22.4,"publicationDate":"2024-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139973223","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The immediate effect of discrimination on mental health: A meta-analytic review of the causal evidence. 歧视对心理健康的直接影响:对因果证据的元分析回顾。
IF 17.3 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-03-01 Epub Date: 2024-02-08 DOI: 10.1037/bul0000419
Christine Emmer, Julia Dorn, Jutta Mata

This meta-analysis synthesizes experimental studies on the immediate effects of discrimination on mental health, exploring the effects of different paradigms and discrimination types on diverse facets of mental health. We analyzed data from a systematic literature search (73 studies; 12,097 participants; 245 effect sizes) for randomized controlled trials with manipulation of discrimination as a predictor and mental health as an outcome using a three-level random-effects model. Experimentally manipulated discrimination led to poorer mental health (g = -0.30), also after controlling for publication year, region, education level, and methodological quality. Moderator analyses revealed stronger effects for pervasive (g = -0.55) compared to single-event manipulations (g = -0.25) and a trend toward weaker effects for samples with nonmarginalized (g = -0.16) compared to marginalized identities (g = -0.34). Gender and age did not moderate the effect. Discrimination had the largest effects on externalizing (g = -0.66) and distress-related outcomes (g = -0.41); heterosexism (g = -0.66), racism (g = -0.32), and sexism (g = -0.30) had the largest effects on mental health. Convenience sampling compromised generalizability to subgroups and the general population, downgrading methodological quality for all included studies. When interpreting the findings, selective samples (mostly young female adults with higher education), often limited ecological validity, and ethical restrictions of lab-induced discrimination need to be considered. These constraints likely led to conservative estimates of the mental health effects of discrimination in this meta-analysis. Future research should investigate more diverse samples, further explain the heterogeneity of findings, and explore protective factors of the effects of discrimination on mental health. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

本荟萃分析综合了有关歧视对心理健康直接影响的实验研究,探讨了不同范例和歧视类型对心理健康不同方面的影响。我们通过系统性文献检索(73 项研究;12,097 名参与者;245 个效应大小),使用三水平随机效应模型分析了以操纵歧视为预测因素、以心理健康为结果的随机对照试验数据。在控制了发表年份、地区、教育水平和方法质量之后,实验操纵的歧视导致了较差的心理健康(g = -0.30)。调节因素分析表明,与单一事件操纵(g = -0.25)相比,普遍性操纵(g = -0.55)的影响更大;与边缘化身份(g = -0.34)相比,非边缘化身份(g = -0.16)样本的影响呈减弱趋势。性别和年龄对这一效应没有调节作用。歧视对外在化(g = -0.66)和痛苦相关结果(g = -0.41)的影响最大;异性恋(g = -0.66)、种族主义(g = -0.32)和性别歧视(g = -0.30)对心理健康的影响最大。方便取样影响了亚群体和普通人群的普遍性,降低了所有纳入研究的方法学质量。在解释研究结果时,需要考虑选择性样本(大多为受过高等教育的年轻女性成年人)、往往有限的生态效度以及实验室歧视的伦理限制。这些限制因素很可能导致本荟萃分析对歧视对心理健康影响的估计偏于保守。未来的研究应调查更多样化的样本,进一步解释研究结果的异质性,并探索歧视对心理健康影响的保护因素。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"The immediate effect of discrimination on mental health: A meta-analytic review of the causal evidence.","authors":"Christine Emmer, Julia Dorn, Jutta Mata","doi":"10.1037/bul0000419","DOIUrl":"10.1037/bul0000419","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>This meta-analysis synthesizes experimental studies on the immediate effects of discrimination on mental health, exploring the effects of different paradigms and discrimination types on diverse facets of mental health. We analyzed data from a systematic literature search (73 studies; 12,097 participants; 245 effect sizes) for randomized controlled trials with manipulation of discrimination as a predictor and mental health as an outcome using a three-level random-effects model. Experimentally manipulated discrimination led to poorer mental health (<i>g</i> = -0.30), also after controlling for publication year, region, education level, and methodological quality. Moderator analyses revealed stronger effects for <i>pervasive (g</i> = -0.55) compared to single-event manipulations (<i>g</i> = -0.25) and a trend toward weaker effects for samples with nonmarginalized (<i>g</i> = -0.16) compared to marginalized identities (<i>g</i> = -0.34). Gender and age did not moderate the effect. Discrimination had the largest effects on externalizing (<i>g</i> = -0.66) and distress-related outcomes (<i>g</i> = -0.41); heterosexism (<i>g</i> = -0.66), racism (<i>g</i> = -0.32), and sexism (<i>g</i> = -0.30) had the largest effects on mental health. Convenience sampling compromised generalizability to subgroups and the general population, downgrading methodological quality for all included studies. When interpreting the findings, selective samples (mostly young female adults with higher education), often limited ecological validity, and ethical restrictions of lab-induced discrimination need to be considered. These constraints likely led to conservative estimates of the mental health effects of discrimination in this meta-analysis. Future research should investigate more diverse samples, further explain the heterogeneity of findings, and explore protective factors of the effects of discrimination on mental health. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":20854,"journal":{"name":"Psychological bulletin","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":17.3,"publicationDate":"2024-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139707690","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
A meta-analytic review of the associations of personality, intelligence, and physical size with social status. 关于性格、智力和体型与社会地位关系的元分析综述。
IF 22.4 1区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-03-01 Epub Date: 2024-02-08 DOI: 10.1037/bul0000416
Michael P Grosz, Robbie C M van Aert, Mitja D Back

Theories have proposed diverse reasons for why individual differences such as personality traits lead to social status attainment in face-to-face groups. We integrated these different theoretical standpoints into a model with four paths from individual differences to status: a dominance, a competence, a virtue, and a micropolitics path. To investigate these paths, we meta-analyzed over 100 years of research on bivariate associations of personality traits, cognitive abilities, and physical size with the attainment of status-related outcomes in face-to-face groups (1,064 effects from 276 samples including 56,153 participants). The status-related outcome variables were admiring respect, social influence, popularity (i.e., being liked by others), leadership emergence, and a mixture of outcome variables. The meta-analytic correlations we found were largely in line with the micropolitics path, tentatively in line with the competence and virtue paths, and only partly in line with the dominance path. These findings suggest that status attainment depends not only on the competence and virtue of an individual but also on how individuals can enhance their apparent competence or virtue by behaving assertively, by being extraverted, or through self-monitoring. We also investigated how the relations between individual differences and status-related outcomes were moderated by kind of status-related outcome, nature of the group task, culture (collectivism/individualism), and length of acquaintance. The moderation analysis yielded mixed and inconclusive results. The review ends with directions for research, such as the need to separately assess and study the different status-related outcomes. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

关于个性特征等个体差异为何会导致在面对面群体中获得社会地位,各种理论提出了不同的原因。我们将这些不同的理论观点整合到一个模型中,该模型包含从个体差异到地位的四条路径:支配力路径、能力路径、美德路径和微观政治路径。为了研究这些路径,我们对 100 多年来关于人格特质、认知能力和体型与在面对面群体中获得地位相关结果的二元关联的研究进行了元分析(来自 276 个样本的 1,064 个效应,包括 56,153 名参与者)。与地位相关的结果变量包括:钦佩尊重、社会影响力、受欢迎程度(即被他人喜欢)、领导力的崛起以及结果变量的混合。我们发现的元分析相关性基本符合微观政治学路径,初步符合能力和美德路径,只有部分符合支配力路径。这些发现表明,地位的获得不仅取决于个人的能力和美德,还取决于个人如何通过自信的行为、外向的性格或自我监督来提高其表面上的能力或美德。我们还研究了个体差异与地位相关结果之间的关系如何受到地位相关结果的种类、群体任务的性质、文化(集体主义/个人主义)和相识时间长短的调节。调节分析的结果好坏参半,没有定论。综述最后提出了研究方向,如需要分别评估和研究不同的地位相关结果。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"A meta-analytic review of the associations of personality, intelligence, and physical size with social status.","authors":"Michael P Grosz, Robbie C M van Aert, Mitja D Back","doi":"10.1037/bul0000416","DOIUrl":"10.1037/bul0000416","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Theories have proposed diverse reasons for why individual differences such as personality traits lead to social status attainment in face-to-face groups. We integrated these different theoretical standpoints into a model with four paths from individual differences to status: a dominance, a competence, a virtue, and a micropolitics path. To investigate these paths, we meta-analyzed over 100 years of research on bivariate associations of personality traits, cognitive abilities, and physical size with the attainment of status-related outcomes in face-to-face groups (1,064 effects from 276 samples including 56,153 participants). The status-related outcome variables were admiring respect, social influence, popularity (i.e., being liked by others), leadership emergence, and a mixture of outcome variables. The meta-analytic correlations we found were largely in line with the micropolitics path, tentatively in line with the competence and virtue paths, and only partly in line with the dominance path. These findings suggest that status attainment depends not only on the competence and virtue of an individual but also on how individuals can enhance their apparent competence or virtue by behaving assertively, by being extraverted, or through self-monitoring. We also investigated how the relations between individual differences and status-related outcomes were moderated by kind of status-related outcome, nature of the group task, culture (collectivism/individualism), and length of acquaintance. The moderation analysis yielded mixed and inconclusive results. The review ends with directions for research, such as the need to separately assess and study the different status-related outcomes. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":20854,"journal":{"name":"Psychological bulletin","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":22.4,"publicationDate":"2024-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139707642","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Mind-wandering increases in frequency over time during task performance: An individual-participant meta-analytic review. 在任务执行过程中,思维游离的频率会随着时间的推移而增加:个人参与者荟萃分析综述。
IF 22.4 1区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-02-29 DOI: 10.1037/bul0000424
Anthony P Zanesco, Ekaterina Denkova, Amishi P Jha

Attention has a seemingly inevitable tendency to turn inward toward our thoughts. Mind-wandering refers to moments when this inward focus diverts attention away from the current task-at-hand. Mind-wandering is thought to be ubiquitous, having been estimated to occur between 30% and 50% of our waking moments. Yet, it is unclear whether this frequency is similar within-task performance contexts and unknown whether mind-wandering systematically increases with time-on-task for a broad range of tasks. We conducted a systematic literature search and individual participant data meta-analysis of rates of occurrence of mind-wandering during task performance. Our search located 68 research reports providing almost a half-million total responses to experience sampling mind-wandering probes from more than 10,000 unique individuals. Latent growth curve models estimated the initial occurrence of mind-wandering and linear change in mind-wandering over sequential probes for each study sample, and effects were summarized using multivariate meta-analysis. Our results confirm that mind-wandering increases in frequency over time during task performance, implicating mind-wandering in characteristic within-task psychological changes, such as increasing boredom and patterns of worsening behavioral performance with time-on-task. The systematic search and meta-analysis provide the most comprehensive assessment of normative rates of mind-wandering during task performance reported to date. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

注意力有一种看似不可避免的倾向,那就是向内转向我们的思想。思绪游离指的就是这种内向的注意力偏离当前任务的时刻。思想游离被认为是无处不在的,据估计,在我们清醒的时候,有 30% 到 50% 的时间会出现思想游离。然而,目前还不清楚这一频率在任务表现情境中是否相似,也不清楚在广泛的任务中,思维游离是否会随着任务时间的延长而系统地增加。我们对任务执行过程中的思维游离发生率进行了系统的文献检索和个体参与者数据荟萃分析。通过搜索,我们找到了 68 篇研究报告,这些报告提供了来自 10,000 多名独特个体的近 50 万个对思维游离经验取样探查的总回复。潜在增长曲线模型估算了每个研究样本最初出现的思维游移和思维游移在连续探查中的线性变化,并使用多元荟萃分析法对效果进行了总结。我们的研究结果证实,在任务执行过程中,思维游离的频率会随着时间的推移而增加,这表明思维游离与任务内心理变化的特征有关,如随着任务时间的推移,无聊感会增加,行为表现会恶化。该系统性研究和荟萃分析提供了迄今为止对任务执行过程中思维游离常态率最全面的评估报告。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"Mind-wandering increases in frequency over time during task performance: An individual-participant meta-analytic review.","authors":"Anthony P Zanesco, Ekaterina Denkova, Amishi P Jha","doi":"10.1037/bul0000424","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000424","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Attention has a seemingly inevitable tendency to turn inward toward our thoughts. Mind-wandering refers to moments when this inward focus diverts attention away from the current task-at-hand. Mind-wandering is thought to be ubiquitous, having been estimated to occur between 30% and 50% of our waking moments. Yet, it is unclear whether this frequency is similar within-task performance contexts and unknown whether mind-wandering systematically increases with time-on-task for a broad range of tasks. We conducted a systematic literature search and individual participant data meta-analysis of rates of occurrence of mind-wandering during task performance. Our search located 68 research reports providing almost a half-million total responses to experience sampling mind-wandering probes from more than 10,000 unique individuals. Latent growth curve models estimated the initial occurrence of mind-wandering and linear change in mind-wandering over sequential probes for each study sample, and effects were summarized using multivariate meta-analysis. Our results confirm that mind-wandering increases in frequency over time during task performance, implicating mind-wandering in characteristic within-task psychological changes, such as increasing boredom and patterns of worsening behavioral performance with time-on-task. The systematic search and meta-analysis provide the most comprehensive assessment of normative rates of mind-wandering during task performance reported to date. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":20854,"journal":{"name":"Psychological bulletin","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":22.4,"publicationDate":"2024-02-29","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139997304","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Supplemental Material for Wipe It Off: A Meta-Analytic Review of the Psychological Consequences and Antecedents of Physical Cleansing 擦干净:身体清洁的心理后果和前因后果的元分析综述》补充材料
IF 22.4 1区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-02-12 DOI: 10.1037/bul0000421.supp
{"title":"Supplemental Material for Wipe It Off: A Meta-Analytic Review of the Psychological Consequences and Antecedents of Physical Cleansing","authors":"","doi":"10.1037/bul0000421.supp","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000421.supp","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":20854,"journal":{"name":"Psychological bulletin","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":22.4,"publicationDate":"2024-02-12","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139842568","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Psychological bulletin
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1