首页 > 最新文献

Psychological bulletin最新文献

英文 中文
The relationship between religiousness and health among sexual minorities: A meta-analysis. 性少数群体宗教信仰与健康关系的meta分析。
IF 22.4 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2021-07-01 Epub Date: 2021-04-01 DOI: 10.1037/bul0000321
G Tyler Lefevor, Edward B Davis, Jaqueline Y Paiz, Abigail C P Smack

[Correction Notice: An Erratum for this article was reported in Vol 147(7) of Psychological Bulletin (see record 2022-08521-004). In the article, there was an error in the calculation of the effect sizes from one study. The three effect sizes for Wolff et al. (2016) listed in Table B1 of the online supplemental materials should have been "r = .09, r = -.02, r = -.05," rather than "r = -.18, r = .53, r = -.35." We rechecked the calculations for other studies and effect sizes and found no additional errors. Further, analyses rerun with the revised data set resulted in no changes in significance for any analyses that included this study; hence, no conclusions were changed because of this error. In the article, the sentences in the final paragraph of the Statistical Analyses section that described this study as an outlier were deleted and replaced with "No such outliers were found." All versions of this article have been corrected.] Meta-analyses suggest that religiousness/spirituality (R/S) is consistently and positively associated with health (average r = .15); however, the strength and direction of this relationship is much less clear among sexual minorities, and many sexual minorities experience tension related to R/S. To address this, we present results from the first meta-analysis of the relationship between R/S and health among sexual minorities. Using 279 effect sizes nested within 73 studies, multilevel meta-analyses suggest a small but positive overall relationship between R/S and health among sexual minorities (r = .05), with a substantial amount of residual heterogeneity. Moderator analyses clarify that this relationship is particularly positive when R/S is conceptualized as spirituality (r = .14) or as religious cognition (e.g., belief; r = .10). The relationship between R/S and health disappears or becomes negative when participants are sampled from sexual minority venues (e.g., bars/clubs; r = .01). Minority stress, structural stigma, and causal pathways theories provide some structure to understand results; however, none of these theories is able to explain results fully. We synthesize these theories to provide an initial theoretical explanation: the degree to which R/S promotes or harms sexual minorities' health depends on (a) where the individual is in their sexual identity development/integration; (b) what their current R/S beliefs, practices, and motivations are; and (c) how well their environmental circumstances support their sexual and/or religious identities. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA, all rights reserved).

[更正通知:本文的错误报告于《心理学通报》第147卷第7期(见记录2022-08521-004)。]在这篇文章中,有一项研究的效应量计算有误。在线补充资料表B1中列出的Wolff et al.(2016)的三个效应量应该是“r = .09, r = -”。02 r = -。而不是“r = -”。18, r =。53,r = -。35。”我们重新检查了其他研究和效应大小的计算结果,没有发现额外的错误。此外,使用修订后的数据集进行的分析结果表明,包括本研究在内的任何分析的显著性都没有变化;因此,没有结论因为这个错误而改变。在文章中,在统计分析部分的最后一段中,将本研究描述为一个异常值的句子被删除,取而代之的是“No such outliers were found”。本文的所有版本都已更正。元分析表明,宗教信仰/灵性(R/S)与健康始终呈正相关(平均R = 0.15);然而,这种关系的强度和方向在性少数群体中就不那么清楚了,许多性少数群体经历着与R/S相关的紧张关系。为了解决这个问题,我们提出了性少数群体中R/S与健康之间关系的第一次荟萃分析的结果。在73项研究中嵌套了279个效应量,多水平荟萃分析表明,性少数群体的R/S与健康之间存在小而正的总体关系(R = 0.05),存在大量的剩余异质性。调节分析表明,当R/S被概念化为灵性(R = .14)或宗教认知(例如,信仰;R = .10)。当参与者来自性少数场所(例如,酒吧/俱乐部;R = 0.01)。少数民族压力、结构性耻辱和因果途径理论为理解结果提供了一些结构;然而,这些理论都不能完全解释结果。我们综合了这些理论,提供了一个初步的理论解释:R/S促进或损害性少数群体健康的程度取决于(a)个体在其性认同发展/整合中的位置;(b)他们目前的R/S信仰、做法和动机是什么;(c)他们的环境在多大程度上支持他们的性和/或宗教身份。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"The relationship between religiousness and health among sexual minorities: A meta-analysis.","authors":"G Tyler Lefevor,&nbsp;Edward B Davis,&nbsp;Jaqueline Y Paiz,&nbsp;Abigail C P Smack","doi":"10.1037/bul0000321","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000321","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>[Correction Notice: An Erratum for this article was reported in Vol 147(7) of <i>Psychological Bulletin</i> (see record 2022-08521-004). In the article, there was an error in the calculation of the effect sizes from one study. The three effect sizes for Wolff et al. (2016) listed in Table B1 of the online supplemental materials should have been \"<i>r</i> = .09, <i>r</i> = -.02, <i>r</i> = -.05,\" rather than \"<i>r</i> = -.18, <i>r</i> = .53, <i>r</i> = -.35.\" We rechecked the calculations for other studies and effect sizes and found no additional errors. Further, analyses rerun with the revised data set resulted in no changes in significance for any analyses that included this study; hence, no conclusions were changed because of this error. In the article, the sentences in the final paragraph of the Statistical Analyses section that described this study as an outlier were deleted and replaced with \"No such outliers were found.\" All versions of this article have been corrected.] Meta-analyses suggest that religiousness/spirituality (R/S) is consistently and positively associated with health (average r = .15); however, the strength and direction of this relationship is much less clear among sexual minorities, and many sexual minorities experience tension related to R/S. To address this, we present results from the first meta-analysis of the relationship between R/S and health among sexual minorities. Using 279 effect sizes nested within 73 studies, multilevel meta-analyses suggest a small but positive overall relationship between R/S and health among sexual minorities (r = .05), with a substantial amount of residual heterogeneity. Moderator analyses clarify that this relationship is particularly positive when R/S is conceptualized as spirituality (r = .14) or as religious cognition (e.g., belief; r = .10). The relationship between R/S and health disappears or becomes negative when participants are sampled from sexual minority venues (e.g., bars/clubs; r = .01). Minority stress, structural stigma, and causal pathways theories provide some structure to understand results; however, none of these theories is able to explain results fully. We synthesize these theories to provide an initial theoretical explanation: the degree to which R/S promotes or harms sexual minorities' health depends on (a) where the individual is in their sexual identity development/integration; (b) what their current R/S beliefs, practices, and motivations are; and (c) how well their environmental circumstances support their sexual and/or religious identities. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":20854,"journal":{"name":"Psychological bulletin","volume":"147 7","pages":"647-666"},"PeriodicalIF":22.4,"publicationDate":"2021-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"25540206","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 27
A meta-analysis of longitudinal peer influence effects in childhood and adolescence. 儿童期和青春期纵向同伴影响效应的荟萃分析。
IF 22.4 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2021-07-01 DOI: 10.1037/bul0000329
Matteo Giletta, Sophia Choukas-Bradley, Marlies Maes, Kathryn P Linthicum, Noel A Card, Mitchell J Prinstein

For decades, psychological research has examined the extent to which children's and adolescents' behavior is influenced by the behavior of their peers (i.e., peer influence effects). This review provides a comprehensive synthesis and meta-analysis of this vast field of psychological science, with a goal to quantify the magnitude of peer influence effects across a broad array of behaviors (externalizing, internalizing, academic). To provide a rigorous test of peer influence effects, only studies that employed longitudinal designs, controlled for youths' baseline behaviors, and used "external informants" (peers' own reports or other external reporters) were included. These criteria yielded a total of 233 effect sizes from 60 independent studies across four different continents. A multilevel meta-analytic approach, allowing the inclusion of multiple dependent effect sizes from the same study, was used to estimate an average cross-lagged regression coefficient, indicating the extent to which peers' behavior predicted changes in youths' own behavior over time. Results revealed a peer influence effect that was small in magnitude (β¯ = .08) but significant and robust. Peer influence effects did not vary as a function of the behavioral outcome, age, or peer relationship type (one close friend vs. multiple friends). Time lag and peer context emerged as significant moderators, suggesting stronger peer influence effects over shorter time periods, and when the assessment of peer relationships was not limited to the classroom context. Results provide the most thorough and comprehensive synthesis of childhood and adolescent peer influence to date, indicating that peer influence occurs similarly across a broad range of behaviors and attitudes. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA, all rights reserved).

几十年来,心理学研究一直在研究儿童和青少年的行为在多大程度上受到同伴行为的影响(即同伴影响效应)。这篇综述对这一广阔的心理科学领域进行了全面的综合和荟萃分析,目的是量化同伴影响在一系列广泛行为(外化、内化、学术)中的影响程度。为了提供对同伴影响效应的严格检验,只有采用纵向设计、控制青少年基线行为并使用“外部举报人”(同伴自己的报告或其他外部举报人)的研究被包括在内。这些标准从四大洲的60项独立研究中得出了233个效应值。采用多水平荟萃分析方法,允许包含来自同一研究的多个依赖效应大小,用于估计平均交叉滞后回归系数,表明同龄人的行为预测青少年自身行为随时间变化的程度。结果显示同伴影响效应的幅度较小(β¯= 0.08),但显著且稳健。同伴影响效应不随行为结果、年龄或同伴关系类型(一个亲密朋友vs多个朋友)的变化而变化。时间滞后和同伴环境是显著的调节因素,表明在较短的时间内,同伴关系的评估不局限于课堂环境时,同伴影响效应更强。结果提供了迄今为止儿童和青少年同伴影响的最彻底和全面的综合,表明同伴影响在广泛的行为和态度中同样发生。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"A meta-analysis of longitudinal peer influence effects in childhood and adolescence.","authors":"Matteo Giletta,&nbsp;Sophia Choukas-Bradley,&nbsp;Marlies Maes,&nbsp;Kathryn P Linthicum,&nbsp;Noel A Card,&nbsp;Mitchell J Prinstein","doi":"10.1037/bul0000329","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000329","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>For decades, psychological research has examined the extent to which children's and adolescents' behavior is influenced by the behavior of their peers (i.e., peer influence effects). This review provides a comprehensive synthesis and meta-analysis of this vast field of psychological science, with a goal to quantify the magnitude of peer influence effects across a broad array of behaviors (externalizing, internalizing, academic). To provide a rigorous test of peer influence effects, only studies that employed longitudinal designs, controlled for youths' baseline behaviors, and used \"external informants\" (peers' own reports or other external reporters) were included. These criteria yielded a total of 233 effect sizes from 60 independent studies across four different continents. A multilevel meta-analytic approach, allowing the inclusion of multiple dependent effect sizes from the same study, was used to estimate an average cross-lagged regression coefficient, indicating the extent to which peers' behavior predicted changes in youths' own behavior over time. Results revealed a peer influence effect that was small in magnitude (β¯ = .08) but significant and robust. Peer influence effects did not vary as a function of the behavioral outcome, age, or peer relationship type (one close friend vs. multiple friends). Time lag and peer context emerged as significant moderators, suggesting stronger peer influence effects over shorter time periods, and when the assessment of peer relationships was not limited to the classroom context. Results provide the most thorough and comprehensive synthesis of childhood and adolescent peer influence to date, indicating that peer influence occurs similarly across a broad range of behaviors and attitudes. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":20854,"journal":{"name":"Psychological bulletin","volume":"147 7","pages":"719-747"},"PeriodicalIF":22.4,"publicationDate":"2021-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"39797571","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Superiority of external attentional focus for motor performance and learning: Systematic reviews and meta-analyses. 外注意焦点对运动表现和学习的优势:系统回顾和元分析。
IF 22.4 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2021-06-01 DOI: 10.1037/bul0000335
Lee-Kuen Chua, Judith Jimenez-Diaz, Rebecca Lewthwaite, Taewon Kim, Gabriele Wulf

Considerable literature on the role of attentional focus in motor performance and learning has accumulated for over two decades. We report the results of comprehensive meta-analyses that address the impact of an external focus (EF, on intended movement effects) versus internal focus (IF, on movements of body parts) of attention on the performance and learning of motor skills. Values of effect sizes (ES) from 73 studies with 1,824 participants and 40 studies with 1,274 participants were used for examining the effects of EF versus IF on behavioral outcomes of motor performance and learning (separately for retention and transfer phases) respectively. The EF condition was more effective than the IF condition for performance, Hedges' g value = 0.264 (95% CI [0.217, 0.310]), retention learning, Hedges' g value = 0.583 (95% CI [0.425, 0.741]), and transfer learning, Hedges' g value = 0.584 (95% CI [0.325, 0.842]). Multivariable metaregression analyses on behavioral measures further indicated that neither age group, health status, or skill level, nor their two-way interactions, moderated the ES differences between EF and IF in performance, retention, and transfer models (all p > .100). A secondary analysis on 12 studies with 216 participants that examined the effects of EF versus IF on electromyographic outcomes of motor performance also indicated that EF was associated with more efficient neuromuscular processing, Hedges' g value = 0.833 (95% CI [0.453, 1.213]). From nine studies with 272 participants, performance measured by behavioral outcomes was found to be more effective when a more distal, rather than proximal, EF was used, Hedges' g value = 0.224 (95% CI [0.019, 0.429]). Overall, the meta-analytic results are consistent with prior narrative reviews and indicate that an external focus is superior to an internal focus whether considering tests of motor performance or learning, and regardless of age, health condition, and level of skill expertise. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA, all rights reserved).

关于注意力集中在运动表现和学习中的作用的大量文献已经积累了二十多年。我们报告了综合荟萃分析的结果,该结果解决了外部焦点(EF,对预期运动效果)与内部焦点(IF,对身体部位的运动)对运动技能表现和学习的影响。分别使用73项研究(1824名参与者)和40项研究(1274名参与者)的效应量(ES)值来检验EF和IF对运动表现和学习行为结果的影响(分别用于保留和转移阶段)。EF条件比IF条件对表现更有效,Hedges的g值= 0.264 (95% CI[0.217, 0.310]),保留学习,Hedges的g值= 0.583 (95% CI[0.425, 0.741]),迁移学习,Hedges的g值= 0.584 (95% CI[0.325, 0.842])。行为测量的多变量元回归分析进一步表明,年龄组、健康状况或技能水平及其双向相互作用都不能调节EF和IF在表现、保留和迁移模型中的ES差异(均p > .100)。对216名参与者的12项研究进行了二次分析,检查了EF与IF对运动表现肌电图结果的影响,也表明EF与更有效的神经肌肉加工有关,Hedges' g值= 0.833 (95% CI[0.453, 1.213])。从涉及272名参与者的9项研究中发现,使用远端EF比近端EF更有效,Hedges的g值= 0.224 (95% CI[0.019, 0.429])。总体而言,meta分析结果与先前的叙述性综述一致,并表明无论考虑运动表现或学习测试,无论年龄,健康状况和技能专业水平如何,外部焦点都优于内部焦点。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"Superiority of external attentional focus for motor performance and learning: Systematic reviews and meta-analyses.","authors":"Lee-Kuen Chua,&nbsp;Judith Jimenez-Diaz,&nbsp;Rebecca Lewthwaite,&nbsp;Taewon Kim,&nbsp;Gabriele Wulf","doi":"10.1037/bul0000335","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000335","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Considerable literature on the role of attentional focus in motor performance and learning has accumulated for over two decades. We report the results of comprehensive meta-analyses that address the impact of an external focus (EF, on intended movement effects) versus internal focus (IF, on movements of body parts) of attention on the performance and learning of motor skills. Values of effect sizes (ES) from 73 studies with 1,824 participants and 40 studies with 1,274 participants were used for examining the effects of EF versus IF on behavioral outcomes of motor performance and learning (separately for retention and transfer phases) respectively. The EF condition was more effective than the IF condition for performance, Hedges' g value = 0.264 (95% CI [0.217, 0.310]), retention learning, Hedges' g value = 0.583 (95% CI [0.425, 0.741]), and transfer learning, Hedges' g value = 0.584 (95% CI [0.325, 0.842]). Multivariable metaregression analyses on behavioral measures further indicated that neither age group, health status, or skill level, nor their two-way interactions, moderated the ES differences between EF and IF in performance, retention, and transfer models (all p > .100). A secondary analysis on 12 studies with 216 participants that examined the effects of EF versus IF on electromyographic outcomes of motor performance also indicated that EF was associated with more efficient neuromuscular processing, Hedges' g value = 0.833 (95% CI [0.453, 1.213]). From nine studies with 272 participants, performance measured by behavioral outcomes was found to be more effective when a more distal, rather than proximal, EF was used, Hedges' g value = 0.224 (95% CI [0.019, 0.429]). Overall, the meta-analytic results are consistent with prior narrative reviews and indicate that an external focus is superior to an internal focus whether considering tests of motor performance or learning, and regardless of age, health condition, and level of skill expertise. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":20854,"journal":{"name":"Psychological bulletin","volume":"147 6","pages":"618-645"},"PeriodicalIF":22.4,"publicationDate":"2021-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"39943511","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 39
The home math environment and math achievement: A meta-analysis. 家庭数学环境与数学成绩:元分析。
IF 22.4 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2021-06-01 DOI: 10.1037/bul0000330
Mia C Daucourt, Amy R Napoli, Jamie M Quinn, Sarah G Wood, Sara A Hart

Mathematical thinking is in high demand in the global market, but approximately 6 percent of school-age children across the globe experience math difficulties (Shalev et al., 2000). The home math environment (HME), which includes all math-related activities, attitudes, beliefs, expectations, and utterances in the home, may be associated with children's math development. To examine the relation between the HME and children's math abilities, a preregistered meta-analysis was conducted to estimate the average weighted correlation coefficient (r) between the HME and children's math achievement and how potential moderators (i.e., assessment, study, and sample features) might contribute to study heterogeneity. A multilevel correlated effects model using 631 effect sizes from 64 quantitative studies comprising 68 independent samples found a positive, statistically significant average weighted correlation of r = .13 (SE = .02, p < .001). Our combined sensitivity analyses showed that the present findings were robust and that the sample of studies has evidential value. A number of assessment, study, and sample characteristics contributed to study heterogeneity, showing that no single feature of HME research was driving the large between-study differences found for the association between the HME and children's math achievement. These findings indicate that children's environments and interactions related to their learning are supported in the specific context of math learning. Our results also show that the HME represents a setting in which children learn about math through social interactions with their caregivers (Vygotsky, 1978) and what they learn depends on the influence of many levels of environmental input (Bronfenbrenner, 1979) and the specificity of input children receive (Bornstein, 2002). (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA, all rights reserved).

数学思维在全球市场上需求量很大,但全球约有6%的学龄儿童存在数学困难(Shalev et al., 2000)。家庭数学环境(HME)包括家庭中所有与数学相关的活动、态度、信念、期望和话语,可能与儿童的数学发展有关。为了检验家庭家庭环境与儿童数学能力之间的关系,我们进行了一项预登记的荟萃分析,以估计家庭家庭环境与儿童数学成绩之间的平均加权相关系数(r),以及潜在的调节因子(即评估、研究和样本特征)可能对研究异质性有何影响。采用64个定量研究中包含68个独立样本的631个效应大小的多水平相关效应模型发现,平均加权相关性为r = 0.13,具有统计学显著性(SE = 0.02, p < 0.001)。我们的联合敏感性分析表明,目前的发现是稳健的,研究样本具有证据价值。许多评估、研究和样本特征导致了研究的异质性,表明没有单一的HME研究特征导致了HME与儿童数学成绩之间的巨大研究差异。这些发现表明,与儿童学习相关的环境和相互作用在数学学习的特定背景下得到了支持。我们的研究结果还表明,HME代表了一种环境,在这种环境中,儿童通过与照顾者的社会互动来学习数学(Vygotsky, 1978),他们学习的内容取决于许多层次的环境输入的影响(Bronfenbrenner, 1979)和儿童接受的输入的特异性(bernstein, 2002)。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"The home math environment and math achievement: A meta-analysis.","authors":"Mia C Daucourt,&nbsp;Amy R Napoli,&nbsp;Jamie M Quinn,&nbsp;Sarah G Wood,&nbsp;Sara A Hart","doi":"10.1037/bul0000330","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000330","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Mathematical thinking is in high demand in the global market, but approximately 6 percent of school-age children across the globe experience math difficulties (Shalev et al., 2000). The home math environment (HME), which includes all math-related activities, attitudes, beliefs, expectations, and utterances in the home, may be associated with children's math development. To examine the relation between the HME and children's math abilities, a preregistered meta-analysis was conducted to estimate the average weighted correlation coefficient (r) between the HME and children's math achievement and how potential moderators (i.e., assessment, study, and sample features) might contribute to study heterogeneity. A multilevel correlated effects model using 631 effect sizes from 64 quantitative studies comprising 68 independent samples found a positive, statistically significant average weighted correlation of r = .13 (SE = .02, p < .001). Our combined sensitivity analyses showed that the present findings were robust and that the sample of studies has evidential value. A number of assessment, study, and sample characteristics contributed to study heterogeneity, showing that no single feature of HME research was driving the large between-study differences found for the association between the HME and children's math achievement. These findings indicate that children's environments and interactions related to their learning are supported in the specific context of math learning. Our results also show that the HME represents a setting in which children learn about math through social interactions with their caregivers (Vygotsky, 1978) and what they learn depends on the influence of many levels of environmental input (Bronfenbrenner, 1979) and the specificity of input children receive (Bornstein, 2002). (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":20854,"journal":{"name":"Psychological bulletin","volume":"147 6","pages":"565-596"},"PeriodicalIF":22.4,"publicationDate":"2021-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC8634776/pdf/nihms-1715506.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"39943510","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 56
The visual environment and attention in decision making. 决策中的视觉环境与注意。
IF 22.4 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2021-06-01 DOI: 10.1037/bul0000328
Jacob L Orquin, Erik S Lahm, Hrvoje Stojić

Visual attention is a fundamental aspect of most everyday decisions, and governments and companies spend vast resources competing for the attention of decision makers. In natural environments, choice options differ on a variety of visual factors, such as salience, position, or surface size. However, most decision theories ignore such visual factors, focusing on cognitive factors such as preferences as determinants of attention. To provide a systematic review of how the visual environment guides attention we meta-analyze 122 effect sizes on eye movements in decision making. A psychometric meta-analysis and Top10 sensitivity analysis show that visual factors play a similar or larger role than cognitive factors in determining attention. The visual factors that most influence attention are positioning information centrally, ρ = .43 (Top10 = .67), increasing the surface size, ρ = .35 (Top10 = .43), reducing the set size of competing information elements, ρ = .24 (Top10 = .24), and increasing visual salience, ρ = .13 (Top10 = .24). Cognitive factors include attending more to preferred choice options and attributes, ρ = .36 (Top10 = .31), effects of task instructions on attention, ρ = .35 (Top10 = .21), and attending more to the ultimately chosen option, ρ = .59 (Top10 = .26). Understanding real-world decision making will require the integration of both visual and cognitive factors in future theories of attention and decision making. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA, all rights reserved).

视觉注意力是大多数日常决策的一个基本方面,政府和公司花费大量资源来争夺决策者的注意力。在自然环境中,选择选项因各种视觉因素而异,例如显著性、位置或表面尺寸。然而,大多数决策理论忽略了这些视觉因素,而把重点放在认知因素上,如偏好作为注意力的决定因素。为了提供关于视觉环境如何引导注意力的系统综述,我们对决策过程中眼动的122个效应量进行了meta分析。心理测量元分析和Top10敏感性分析表明,视觉因素在决定注意力方面的作用与认知因素相似或更大。对注意力影响最大的视觉因素是集中定位信息,ρ = 0.43 (Top10 = 0.67),增大表面尺寸,ρ = 0.35 (Top10 = 0.43),减小竞争信息元素集合大小,ρ = 0.24 (Top10 = 0.24),增大视觉显著性,ρ = 0.13 (Top10 = 0.24)。认知因素包括更多关注首选选项和属性,ρ = 0.36 (Top10 = 0.31),任务指示对注意力的影响,ρ = 0.35 (Top10 = 0.21),以及更多关注最终选择的选项,ρ = 0.59 (Top10 = 0.26)。理解现实世界的决策将需要在未来的注意力和决策理论中整合视觉和认知因素。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"The visual environment and attention in decision making.","authors":"Jacob L Orquin,&nbsp;Erik S Lahm,&nbsp;Hrvoje Stojić","doi":"10.1037/bul0000328","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000328","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Visual attention is a fundamental aspect of most everyday decisions, and governments and companies spend vast resources competing for the attention of decision makers. In natural environments, choice options differ on a variety of visual factors, such as salience, position, or surface size. However, most decision theories ignore such visual factors, focusing on cognitive factors such as preferences as determinants of attention. To provide a systematic review of how the visual environment guides attention we meta-analyze 122 effect sizes on eye movements in decision making. A psychometric meta-analysis and Top10 sensitivity analysis show that visual factors play a similar or larger role than cognitive factors in determining attention. The visual factors that most influence attention are positioning information centrally, ρ = .43 (Top10 = .67), increasing the surface size, ρ = .35 (Top10 = .43), reducing the set size of competing information elements, ρ = .24 (Top10 = .24), and increasing visual salience, ρ = .13 (Top10 = .24). Cognitive factors include attending more to preferred choice options and attributes, ρ = .36 (Top10 = .31), effects of task instructions on attention, ρ = .35 (Top10 = .21), and attending more to the ultimately chosen option, ρ = .59 (Top10 = .26). Understanding real-world decision making will require the integration of both visual and cognitive factors in future theories of attention and decision making. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":20854,"journal":{"name":"Psychological bulletin","volume":"147 6","pages":"597-617"},"PeriodicalIF":22.4,"publicationDate":"2021-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"39943509","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 8
How experimental methods shaped views on human competence and rationality. 实验方法如何形成对人类能力和理性的看法。
IF 22.4 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2021-06-01 DOI: 10.1037/bul0000324
Tomás Lejarraga, Ralph Hertwig

Within just 7 years, behavioral decision research in psychology underwent a dramatic change: In 1967, Peterson and Beach (1967) reviewed more than 160 experiments concerned with people's statistical intuitions. Invoking the metaphor of the mind as an intuitive statistician, they concluded that "probability theory and statistics can be used as the basis for psychological models that integrate and account for human performance in a wide range of inferential tasks" (p. 29). Yet in a 1974 Science article, Tversky and Kahneman rejected this conclusion, arguing that "people rely on a limited number of heuristic principles which reduce the complex tasks of assessing probabilities and predicting values to simple judgmental operations" (p. 1124). With that, they introduced the heuristics-and-biases research program, which has profoundly altered how psychology, and the behavioral sciences more generally, view the mind's competences and rationality. How was this radical transformation possible? We examine a previously neglected driver: The heuristics-and-biases program established an experimental protocol in behavioral decision research that relied on described scenarios rather than learning and experience. We demonstrate this shift with an analysis of 604 experiments, which shows that the descriptive protocol has dominated post-1974 research. Specifically, we examine two lines of research addressed in the intuitive-statistician program (Bayesian reasoning and judgments of compound events) and two lines of research spurred by the heuristics-and-biases program (framing and anchoring and adjustment). We conclude that the focus on description at the expense of learning has profoundly shaped the influential view of the error-proneness of human cognition. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA, all rights reserved).

在短短7年的时间里,心理学的行为决策研究经历了巨大的变化:1967年,Peterson和Beach(1967)回顾了160多个与人们统计直觉有关的实验。他们援引心灵作为直觉统计学家的比喻,得出结论:“概率论和统计学可以作为心理模型的基础,整合和解释人类在广泛的推理任务中的表现”(第29页)。然而,在1974年《科学》杂志的一篇文章中,特沃斯基和卡尼曼拒绝了这一结论,认为“人们依赖于有限数量的启发式原则,这些原则将评估概率和预测价值的复杂任务简化为简单的判断操作”(第1124页)。在此基础上,他们引入了启发式和偏见研究项目,该项目深刻地改变了心理学以及更广泛的行为科学对思维能力和理性的看法。这种激进的转变是如何实现的?我们研究了一个以前被忽视的驱动因素:启发式和偏见程序在行为决策研究中建立了一个实验协议,该协议依赖于描述的场景,而不是学习和经验。我们通过对604个实验的分析证明了这种转变,这表明描述性协议在1974年后的研究中占主导地位。具体来说,我们研究了直觉统计学家项目(贝叶斯推理和复合事件判断)中涉及的两条研究路线,以及启发和偏见项目(框架、锚定和调整)推动的两条研究路线。我们的结论是,以学习为代价的对描述的关注深刻地塑造了人类认知的错误倾向的有影响力的观点。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"How experimental methods shaped views on human competence and rationality.","authors":"Tomás Lejarraga,&nbsp;Ralph Hertwig","doi":"10.1037/bul0000324","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000324","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Within just 7 years, behavioral decision research in psychology underwent a dramatic change: In 1967, Peterson and Beach (1967) reviewed more than 160 experiments concerned with people's statistical intuitions. Invoking the metaphor of the mind as an intuitive statistician, they concluded that \"probability theory and statistics can be used as the basis for psychological models that integrate and account for human performance in a wide range of inferential tasks\" (p. 29). Yet in a 1974 <i>Science</i> article, Tversky and Kahneman rejected this conclusion, arguing that \"people rely on a limited number of heuristic principles which reduce the complex tasks of assessing probabilities and predicting values to simple judgmental operations\" (p. 1124). With that, they introduced the heuristics-and-biases research program, which has profoundly altered how psychology, and the behavioral sciences more generally, view the mind's competences and rationality. How was this radical transformation possible? We examine a previously neglected driver: The heuristics-and-biases program established an experimental protocol in behavioral decision research that relied on described scenarios rather than learning and experience. We demonstrate this shift with an analysis of 604 experiments, which shows that the descriptive protocol has dominated post-1974 research. Specifically, we examine two lines of research addressed in the intuitive-statistician program (Bayesian reasoning and judgments of compound events) and two lines of research spurred by the heuristics-and-biases program (framing and anchoring and adjustment). We conclude that the focus on description at the expense of learning has profoundly shaped the influential view of the error-proneness of human cognition. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":20854,"journal":{"name":"Psychological bulletin","volume":"147 6","pages":"535-564"},"PeriodicalIF":22.4,"publicationDate":"2021-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"39928906","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 19
A meta-analysis of longitudinal peer influence effects in childhood and adolescence. 儿童和青少年纵向同伴影响效应的荟萃分析。
IF 22.4 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2021-05-06 DOI: 10.31234/OSF.IO/J6CDZ
M. Giletta, Sophia Choukas-Bradley, Marlies Maes, Kathryn P Linthicum, Noel A. Card, M. Prinstein
For decades, psychological research has examined the extent to which children's and adolescents' behavior is influenced by the behavior of their peers (i.e., peer influence effects). This review provides a comprehensive synthesis and meta-analysis of this vast field of psychological science, with a goal to quantify the magnitude of peer influence effects across a broad array of behaviors (externalizing, internalizing, academic). To provide a rigorous test of peer influence effects, only studies that employed longitudinal designs, controlled for youths' baseline behaviors, and used "external informants" (peers' own reports or other external reporters) were included. These criteria yielded a total of 233 effect sizes from 60 independent studies across four different continents. A multilevel meta-analytic approach, allowing the inclusion of multiple dependent effect sizes from the same study, was used to estimate an average cross-lagged regression coefficient, indicating the extent to which peers' behavior predicted changes in youths' own behavior over time. Results revealed a peer influence effect that was small in magnitude (β¯ = .08) but significant and robust. Peer influence effects did not vary as a function of the behavioral outcome, age, or peer relationship type (one close friend vs. multiple friends). Time lag and peer context emerged as significant moderators, suggesting stronger peer influence effects over shorter time periods, and when the assessment of peer relationships was not limited to the classroom context. Results provide the most thorough and comprehensive synthesis of childhood and adolescent peer influence to date, indicating that peer influence occurs similarly across a broad range of behaviors and attitudes. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA, all rights reserved).
几十年来,心理学研究一直在研究儿童和青少年的行为在多大程度上受到同龄人行为的影响(即同伴影响效应)。这篇综述对这一庞大的心理科学领域进行了全面的综合和荟萃分析,目的是量化一系列行为(外化、内化、学术)中同伴影响的程度。为了对同伴影响效应进行严格的测试,只纳入了采用纵向设计、控制青少年基线行为和使用“外部信息者”(同伴自己的报告或其他外部报告者)的研究。这些标准从四大洲的60项独立研究中得出了233个效应大小。一种多水平元分析方法,允许纳入同一研究的多个依赖效应大小,用于估计平均交叉滞后回归系数,表明同龄人的行为在多大程度上预测了年轻人自身行为随时间的变化。结果显示,同伴影响效应的大小很小(β=0.08),但显著且稳健。同伴影响效应并没有随着行为结果、年龄或同伴关系类型(一个亲密朋友与多个朋友)的变化而变化。时间滞后和同伴环境是重要的调节因素,这表明在较短的时间内,当对同伴关系的评估不局限于课堂环境时,同伴影响效应更强。研究结果提供了迄今为止对儿童和青少年同伴影响最彻底、最全面的综合,表明同伴影响在广泛的行为和态度中也发生了类似的情况。(PsycInfo数据库记录(c)2021 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"A meta-analysis of longitudinal peer influence effects in childhood and adolescence.","authors":"M. Giletta, Sophia Choukas-Bradley, Marlies Maes, Kathryn P Linthicum, Noel A. Card, M. Prinstein","doi":"10.31234/OSF.IO/J6CDZ","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.31234/OSF.IO/J6CDZ","url":null,"abstract":"For decades, psychological research has examined the extent to which children's and adolescents' behavior is influenced by the behavior of their peers (i.e., peer influence effects). This review provides a comprehensive synthesis and meta-analysis of this vast field of psychological science, with a goal to quantify the magnitude of peer influence effects across a broad array of behaviors (externalizing, internalizing, academic). To provide a rigorous test of peer influence effects, only studies that employed longitudinal designs, controlled for youths' baseline behaviors, and used \"external informants\" (peers' own reports or other external reporters) were included. These criteria yielded a total of 233 effect sizes from 60 independent studies across four different continents. A multilevel meta-analytic approach, allowing the inclusion of multiple dependent effect sizes from the same study, was used to estimate an average cross-lagged regression coefficient, indicating the extent to which peers' behavior predicted changes in youths' own behavior over time. Results revealed a peer influence effect that was small in magnitude (β¯ = .08) but significant and robust. Peer influence effects did not vary as a function of the behavioral outcome, age, or peer relationship type (one close friend vs. multiple friends). Time lag and peer context emerged as significant moderators, suggesting stronger peer influence effects over shorter time periods, and when the assessment of peer relationships was not limited to the classroom context. Results provide the most thorough and comprehensive synthesis of childhood and adolescent peer influence to date, indicating that peer influence occurs similarly across a broad range of behaviors and attitudes. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA, all rights reserved).","PeriodicalId":20854,"journal":{"name":"Psychological bulletin","volume":"147 7 1","pages":"719-747"},"PeriodicalIF":22.4,"publicationDate":"2021-05-06","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"44772422","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 43
Effectiveness of cognitive stimulation for dementia: A systematic review and meta-analysis. 认知刺激治疗痴呆的有效性:一项系统综述和荟萃分析。
IF 22.4 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2021-05-01 Epub Date: 2021-05-24 DOI: 10.1037/bul0000325
Robin M T Cafferata, Ben Hicks, Claudia C von Bastian

Cognitive stimulation (CS) is a nonpharmacological intervention often involving group activities and social interaction used to treat cognitive declines in people with dementia. This preregistered systematic review and meta-analysis evaluated the effectiveness of CS in producing benefits on cognition (primary outcome) and quality of life, activities of daily living, and psychological symptoms (secondary outcomes) across 44 randomized-controlled trials comprising 45 comparisons including 2,444 participants. A medium-sized effect (g = .49) on global cognition was found immediately after the intervention and was supported by decisive Bayesian evidence. Clinical relevance is defined as a reduction of 3 to 4 points on the Alzheimer's Disease Assessment Scale Cognitive subscale; the average attenuation of cognitive decline observed was 2.41 points (after removing 1 outlier). Therefore, the observed decline was of borderline clinical relevance. CS was also found to significantly improve memory, activities of daily living, depressive symptoms, and dementia ratings; most of these effects were supported by substantial and strong Bayesian evidence. No significant effects were found for global cognition at 1 to 10 months follow-up assessment for language, quality of life, anxiety, and behavior symptoms. However, evidence for the absence of these effects was ambiguous. A review of study bias highlighted that most studies lacked active, double-blinded controls, potentially leading to an overestimation of the effect, and making it difficult to conclusively attribute the observed improvements to the CS intervention. Hence, although effects are promising, the methodological issues highlight there is still a need for better controlled studies that provide more compelling evidence. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA, all rights reserved).

认知刺激(CS)是一种非药物干预,通常涉及群体活动和社会互动,用于治疗痴呆症患者的认知能力下降。本预注册的系统回顾和荟萃分析评估了CS在认知(主要结局)、生活质量、日常生活活动和心理症状(次要结局)方面的有效性,涉及44个随机对照试验,包括45个比较,包括2,444名参与者。干预后立即发现了对全局认知的中等效应(g = .49),并得到决定性贝叶斯证据的支持。临床相关性的定义是在阿尔茨海默病评估量表认知亚量表上减少3到4分;观察到的认知衰退平均衰减为2.41点(剔除1个异常值后)。因此,观察到的下降具有边缘性临床相关性。研究还发现,CS可显著改善记忆、日常生活活动、抑郁症状和痴呆评分;这些影响大多得到了大量有力的贝叶斯证据的支持。在1至10个月的语言、生活质量、焦虑和行为症状随访评估中,未发现对整体认知有显著影响。然而,没有这些影响的证据是模糊的。对研究偏倚的回顾强调,大多数研究缺乏主动的双盲对照,这可能导致对效果的高估,并且难以将观察到的改善最终归因于CS干预。因此,尽管效果是有希望的,但方法问题突出表明,仍然需要更好的对照研究,以提供更有说服力的证据。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"Effectiveness of cognitive stimulation for dementia: A systematic review and meta-analysis.","authors":"Robin M T Cafferata,&nbsp;Ben Hicks,&nbsp;Claudia C von Bastian","doi":"10.1037/bul0000325","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000325","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Cognitive stimulation (CS) is a nonpharmacological intervention often involving group activities and social interaction used to treat cognitive declines in people with dementia. This preregistered systematic review and meta-analysis evaluated the effectiveness of CS in producing benefits on cognition (primary outcome) and quality of life, activities of daily living, and psychological symptoms (secondary outcomes) across 44 randomized-controlled trials comprising 45 comparisons including 2,444 participants. A medium-sized effect (g = .49) on global cognition was found immediately after the intervention and was supported by decisive Bayesian evidence. Clinical relevance is defined as a reduction of 3 to 4 points on the Alzheimer's Disease Assessment Scale Cognitive subscale; the average attenuation of cognitive decline observed was 2.41 points (after removing 1 outlier). Therefore, the observed decline was of borderline clinical relevance. CS was also found to significantly improve memory, activities of daily living, depressive symptoms, and dementia ratings; most of these effects were supported by substantial and strong Bayesian evidence. No significant effects were found for global cognition at 1 to 10 months follow-up assessment for language, quality of life, anxiety, and behavior symptoms. However, evidence for the absence of these effects was ambiguous. A review of study bias highlighted that most studies lacked active, double-blinded controls, potentially leading to an overestimation of the effect, and making it difficult to conclusively attribute the observed improvements to the CS intervention. Hence, although effects are promising, the methodological issues highlight there is still a need for better controlled studies that provide more compelling evidence. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":20854,"journal":{"name":"Psychological bulletin","volume":"147 5","pages":"455-476"},"PeriodicalIF":22.4,"publicationDate":"2021-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"39211641","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 14
Beyond the toddler years: A meta-analysis of communicative abilities in siblings of children with autism spectrum disorder. 超越学步期:自闭症谱系障碍儿童兄弟姐妹沟通能力的荟萃分析。
IF 22.4 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2021-05-01 Epub Date: 2021-05-31 DOI: 10.1037/bul0000326
Emily J Roemer

Communication is a core challenge for individuals on the autism spectrum, and many autistic individuals experience challenges with language. Prospective studies of younger siblings of children with autism show that siblings not only have an increased likelihood of developing autism themselves, but that even siblings without an autism spectrum diagnosis are more likely to have a language delay as toddlers than their peers without familial history of autism (Marrus et al., 2018). While there is substantial variability in the communicative abilities of siblings of children with autism in toddlerhood, it is not clear how these differences translate beyond the first 3 years of life. The present study used meta-analysis to evaluate a wide range of literature and determine whether siblings of children with autism (Sibs-A) over 48 months of age differ from comparison groups in their language and communication abilities. A total of 26 studies describing 22 cohorts were included. Over 2,000 Sibs-A were compared with neurotypical individuals without familial history of autism or siblings of individuals with another neurodevelopmental disability. Meta-analysis revealed a small but significant effect such that Sibs-A performed lower than comparison peers in overall language and communication. Effects were strongest for expressive language and linguistic processing speed, and there were substantially larger differences for experimenter-administered measures than for parent-report, though findings must be considered in the context of other study characteristics. Suggestions based on this meta-analytic review are provided to guide future research in understanding individual differences in the communicative development and outcomes of Sibs-A. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA, all rights reserved).

沟通是自闭症患者的核心挑战,许多自闭症患者在语言方面面临挑战。对自闭症儿童的弟弟妹妹进行的前瞻性研究表明,兄弟姐妹不仅自己患自闭症的可能性增加,而且即使没有自闭症谱系诊断的兄弟姐妹在蹒跚学步时也比没有自闭症家族史的同龄人更容易出现语言迟缓(Marrus等人,2018)。虽然自闭症儿童的兄弟姐妹在幼儿期的沟通能力上有很大的差异,但目前还不清楚这些差异在生命的前3年之后是如何转化的。本研究采用荟萃分析来评估大量文献,并确定年龄超过48个月的自闭症儿童的兄弟姐妹(姐妹-a)在语言和沟通能力方面是否与对照组不同。共纳入26项研究,涉及22个队列。研究人员将2000多名“姐妹a”患者与没有自闭症家族史的神经正常个体或患有其他神经发育障碍的个体的兄弟姐妹进行了比较。荟萃分析显示,姐妹a在整体语言和沟通方面的表现低于对照组,这是一个小而显著的影响。对表达性语言和语言处理速度的影响是最强的,实验人员管理的测量比父母报告的差异要大得多,尽管研究结果必须在其他研究特征的背景下考虑。在此基础上,本研究提出了一些建议,以指导未来的研究,以了解姐妹a交际发展和结果的个体差异。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"Beyond the toddler years: A meta-analysis of communicative abilities in siblings of children with autism spectrum disorder.","authors":"Emily J Roemer","doi":"10.1037/bul0000326","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000326","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Communication is a core challenge for individuals on the autism spectrum, and many autistic individuals experience challenges with language. Prospective studies of younger siblings of children with autism show that siblings not only have an increased likelihood of developing autism themselves, but that even siblings without an autism spectrum diagnosis are more likely to have a language delay as toddlers than their peers without familial history of autism (Marrus et al., 2018). While there is substantial variability in the communicative abilities of siblings of children with autism in toddlerhood, it is not clear how these differences translate beyond the first 3 years of life. The present study used meta-analysis to evaluate a wide range of literature and determine whether siblings of children with autism (Sibs-A) over 48 months of age differ from comparison groups in their language and communication abilities. A total of 26 studies describing 22 cohorts were included. Over 2,000 Sibs-A were compared with neurotypical individuals without familial history of autism or siblings of individuals with another neurodevelopmental disability. Meta-analysis revealed a small but significant effect such that Sibs-A performed lower than comparison peers in overall language and communication. Effects were strongest for expressive language and linguistic processing speed, and there were substantially larger differences for experimenter-administered measures than for parent-report, though findings must be considered in the context of other study characteristics. Suggestions based on this meta-analytic review are provided to guide future research in understanding individual differences in the communicative development and outcomes of Sibs-A. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":20854,"journal":{"name":"Psychological bulletin","volume":"147 5","pages":"437-454"},"PeriodicalIF":22.4,"publicationDate":"2021-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"38969879","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 3
Together, everyone achieves more-or, less? An interdisciplinary meta-analysis on effort gains and losses in teams. 在一起,每个人都取得了更多或更少的成就?团队努力得失的跨学科元分析。
IF 22.4 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2021-05-01 DOI: 10.1037/bul0000251
Ann-Kathrin Torka, Jens Mazei, Joachim Hüffmeier

This preregistered meta-analysis theoretically and empirically integrates the two research strands on effort gains and effort losses in teams. Theoretically, we built on Shepperd's (1993) framework of productivity loss in groups and Karau and Williams' (1993) Collective Effort model (CEM) and developed the Team member Effort Expenditure model (TEEM), an extended Expectancy × Value framework with the explicit addition of an individual work baseline. Empirically, we included studies that allowed calculating a relevant effect size, which represents the difference between an individual's effort under individual work and under teamwork conditions. Overall, we included 622 effect sizes (N = 320,632). We did not find a main effect of teamwork on effort. As predicted, however, multilevel modeling revealed that the (in-)dispensability of the own contribution to the team performance, social comparison potential, and evaluation potential moderated the effect of teamwork versus individual work on expended effort. Depending specifically on the level of (in-)dispensability and the potential to engage in social comparisons, people showed either effort gains or losses in teams. As predicted, we also found that people's self-reports indicated effort gains when they had objectively shown such gains, whereas their self-reports did not indicate effort losses when they had shown such losses. Contrary to our hypotheses, team formation (i.e., ad hoc vs. not ad hoc teams) and task meaningfulness did not emerge as moderators. Altogether, people showed either effort gains or losses in teams depending on the specific design of teamwork. We discuss implications for future research, theory development, and teamwork design in practice. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA, all rights reserved).

这种预先注册的元分析从理论上和经验上整合了团队中努力收益和努力损失的两种研究方向。从理论上讲,我们建立在谢泼德(1993)的团队生产力损失框架和卡劳和威廉姆斯(1993)的集体努力模型(CEM)的基础上,开发了团队成员努力支出模型(TEEM),这是一个扩展的期望x价值框架,明确增加了个人工作基线。根据经验,我们纳入了允许计算相关效应大小的研究,它代表了个人在个人工作和团队工作条件下的努力之间的差异。总的来说,我们纳入了622个效应值(N = 320,632)。我们没有发现团队合作对努力的主要影响。然而,正如预测的那样,多层次模型揭示了个人对团队绩效、社会比较潜力和评价潜力的贡献的可缺性调节了团队合作与个人工作对消耗努力的影响。具体取决于可有可无的程度和参与社会比较的潜力,人们在团队中表现出努力的收益或损失。正如预测的那样,我们还发现,当人们客观地表现出这些收益时,他们的自我报告表明了努力的收益,而当他们表现出这些损失时,他们的自我报告并没有表明努力的损失。与我们的假设相反,团队组成(即,特设与非特设团队)和任务意义并没有成为调节因素。总的来说,人们在团队中表现出的努力收益或损失取决于团队合作的具体设计。我们将讨论对未来研究、理论发展和实践中的团队设计的影响。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"Together, everyone achieves more-or, less? An interdisciplinary meta-analysis on effort gains and losses in teams.","authors":"Ann-Kathrin Torka,&nbsp;Jens Mazei,&nbsp;Joachim Hüffmeier","doi":"10.1037/bul0000251","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000251","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>This preregistered meta-analysis theoretically and empirically integrates the two research strands on effort gains and effort losses in teams. Theoretically, we built on Shepperd's (1993) framework of productivity loss in groups and Karau and Williams' (1993) Collective Effort model (CEM) and developed the Team member Effort Expenditure model (TEEM), an extended Expectancy × Value framework with the explicit addition of an individual work baseline. Empirically, we included studies that allowed calculating a relevant effect size, which represents the difference between an individual's effort under individual work and under teamwork conditions. Overall, we included 622 effect sizes (N = 320,632). We did not find a main effect of teamwork on effort. As predicted, however, multilevel modeling revealed that the (in-)dispensability of the own contribution to the team performance, social comparison potential, and evaluation potential moderated the effect of teamwork versus individual work on expended effort. Depending specifically on the level of (in-)dispensability and the potential to engage in social comparisons, people showed either effort gains or losses in teams. As predicted, we also found that people's self-reports indicated effort gains when they had objectively shown such gains, whereas their self-reports did not indicate effort losses when they had shown such losses. Contrary to our hypotheses, team formation (i.e., ad hoc vs. not ad hoc teams) and task meaningfulness did not emerge as moderators. Altogether, people showed either effort gains or losses in teams depending on the specific design of teamwork. We discuss implications for future research, theory development, and teamwork design in practice. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":20854,"journal":{"name":"Psychological bulletin","volume":"147 5","pages":"504-534"},"PeriodicalIF":22.4,"publicationDate":"2021-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"39211640","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 8
期刊
Psychological bulletin
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1