Brady R T Roberts, Colin M MacLeod, Myra A Fernandes
The enactment effect is the phenomenon that physically performing an action represented by a word or phrase (e.g., clap, clap your hands) results in better memory than does simply reading it. We examined data from three different methodological approaches to provide a comprehensive review of the enactment effect across 145 behavioral, 7 neuroimaging, and 31 neurological patient studies. Boosts in memory performance following execution of a physical action were compared to those produced by reading words or phrases, by watching an experimenter perform actions, or by engaging in self-generated imagery. Across the behavioral studies, we employed random-effects meta-regression with robust variance estimation (RVE) to reveal an average enactment effect size of g = 1.23. Further meta-analyses revealed that variations in study design and comparison task reliably influence the size of the enactment effect, whereas four other experiment factors-test format, learning instruction type, retention interval, and the presence of objects during encoding-likely do not influence the effect. Neuroimaging studies demonstrated enactment-related activation to be prevalent in the motor cortex and inferior parietal lobule. Patient studies indicated that, regardless of whether impairments of memory (e.g., Alzheimer's) or of motor capability (e.g., Parkinson's) were present, patients were able to benefit from enactment. The findings of this systematic review and meta-analysis highlight two components accounting for the memory benefit from enactment: a primary mental contribution relating to planning the action and a secondary physical contribution of the action itself. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).
动作效应是指在身体上做一个单词或短语所代表的动作(例如,拍手,拍手)比简单地阅读它能提高记忆力的现象。我们检查了来自三种不同方法的数据,对145项行为研究、7项神经影像学研究和31项神经学患者研究的颁布效应进行了全面回顾。在进行物理动作后,记忆力的提高与阅读单词或短语、观看实验者的动作或参与自我生成的想象所产生的记忆效果进行了比较。在行为研究中,我们采用随机效应元回归与稳健方差估计(RVE),发现平均制定效应大小g = 1.23。进一步的元分析显示,研究设计和比较任务的变化可靠地影响了制定效应的大小,而其他四个实验因素-测试格式,学习教学类型,保留时间间隔和编码过程中物体的存在-可能不影响效果。神经影像学研究表明,动作相关的激活在运动皮层和下顶叶中普遍存在。患者研究表明,无论是否存在记忆障碍(如阿尔茨海默氏症)或运动能力障碍(如帕金森症),患者都能够从立法中受益。这项系统回顾和荟萃分析的发现突出了两个因素,说明了制定行动对记忆的好处:与计划行动有关的主要精神贡献和行动本身的次要身体贡献。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"The enactment effect: A systematic review and meta-analysis of behavioral, neuroimaging, and patient studies.","authors":"Brady R T Roberts, Colin M MacLeod, Myra A Fernandes","doi":"10.1037/bul0000360","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000360","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The enactment effect is the phenomenon that physically performing an action represented by a word or phrase (e.g., clap, clap your hands) results in better memory than does simply reading it. We examined data from three different methodological approaches to provide a comprehensive review of the enactment effect across 145 behavioral, 7 neuroimaging, and 31 neurological patient studies. Boosts in memory performance following execution of a physical action were compared to those produced by reading words or phrases, by watching an experimenter perform actions, or by engaging in self-generated imagery. Across the behavioral studies, we employed random-effects meta-regression with robust variance estimation (RVE) to reveal an average enactment effect size of <i>g</i> = 1.23. Further meta-analyses revealed that variations in study design and comparison task reliably influence the size of the enactment effect, whereas four other experiment factors-test format, learning instruction type, retention interval, and the presence of objects during encoding-likely do not influence the effect. Neuroimaging studies demonstrated enactment-related activation to be prevalent in the motor cortex and inferior parietal lobule. Patient studies indicated that, regardless of whether impairments of memory (e.g., Alzheimer's) or of motor capability (e.g., Parkinson's) were present, patients were able to benefit from enactment. The findings of this systematic review and meta-analysis highlight two components accounting for the memory benefit from enactment: a primary mental contribution relating to planning the action and a secondary physical contribution of the action itself. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":20854,"journal":{"name":"Psychological bulletin","volume":"148 5-6","pages":"397-434"},"PeriodicalIF":22.4,"publicationDate":"2022-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10415768","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
B. Hutchinson, K. Pammer, Kavindu Bandara, B. Jack
{"title":"A tale of two theories: A meta-analysis of the attention set and load theories of inattentional blindness.","authors":"B. Hutchinson, K. Pammer, Kavindu Bandara, B. Jack","doi":"10.1037/bul0000371","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000371","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":20854,"journal":{"name":"Psychological bulletin","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":22.4,"publicationDate":"2022-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41656593","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Jonas Everaert, J. Vrijsen, R. Martin-Willett, Livia N. M. van de Kraats, J. Joormann
Emotional bias in explicit memory is theorized to play a prominent role in the etiology, maintenance, and recurrence of depression. Even though this cognitive bias is regarded as one of the most robust phenomena in depression, its magnitudeandboundaryconditions indepressionare currentlyunknown.Thisreviewpresents two three-level meta-analyses to estimate the overall effect size and identify moderators of explicit memory bias in depression. Meta-analysis I (153 studies, 686 contrasts) revealed a small overall effect size for naturalistic explicit memory bias in depression, g = 0.241, 95% CI [0.179, 0.304]. The magnitude of the overall effect was moderated by emotional valence of stimuli, operational de fi nition of memory bias, depth of processing during encoding, explicit memory task, and the (non-)verbal nature of stimuli. Equivalent effect sizes were found for minors and adults as well as for clinical and subclinical depression. Remarkably, a nonsigni fi canteffectsizeemergedfor remitteddepression.Followinguponthelatter fi nding,Meta-analysisII (21 studies, 80 contrasts) examined explicit memory bias in remitted depression under naturalistic conditions and under mood/stress induction. Results yielded a nonsigni fi cant overall effect size, g = 0.131, 95% CI [ − 0.045, 0.307], but a signi fi cant effect size for study conditions with mood or stress induction, g = 0.273, 95% CI [0.004, 0.542]. Both meta-analyses indicated high levels of heterogeneity, even after accounting for variation explained by sample and study characteristics. The fi ndings are consistent with the view that depression is characterized by an explicit memory bias that may persist beyond a depressive episode. These fi ndingshaveimplicationsforcognitivetheoriesofvulnerabilitytodepressionaswellasclinicalinterventions.
{"title":"A meta-analytic review of the relationship between explicit memory bias and depression: Depression features an explicit memory bias that persists beyond a depressive episode.","authors":"Jonas Everaert, J. Vrijsen, R. Martin-Willett, Livia N. M. van de Kraats, J. Joormann","doi":"10.1037/bul0000367","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000367","url":null,"abstract":"Emotional bias in explicit memory is theorized to play a prominent role in the etiology, maintenance, and recurrence of depression. Even though this cognitive bias is regarded as one of the most robust phenomena in depression, its magnitudeandboundaryconditions indepressionare currentlyunknown.Thisreviewpresents two three-level meta-analyses to estimate the overall effect size and identify moderators of explicit memory bias in depression. Meta-analysis I (153 studies, 686 contrasts) revealed a small overall effect size for naturalistic explicit memory bias in depression, g = 0.241, 95% CI [0.179, 0.304]. The magnitude of the overall effect was moderated by emotional valence of stimuli, operational de fi nition of memory bias, depth of processing during encoding, explicit memory task, and the (non-)verbal nature of stimuli. Equivalent effect sizes were found for minors and adults as well as for clinical and subclinical depression. Remarkably, a nonsigni fi canteffectsizeemergedfor remitteddepression.Followinguponthelatter fi nding,Meta-analysisII (21 studies, 80 contrasts) examined explicit memory bias in remitted depression under naturalistic conditions and under mood/stress induction. Results yielded a nonsigni fi cant overall effect size, g = 0.131, 95% CI [ − 0.045, 0.307], but a signi fi cant effect size for study conditions with mood or stress induction, g = 0.273, 95% CI [0.004, 0.542]. Both meta-analyses indicated high levels of heterogeneity, even after accounting for variation explained by sample and study characteristics. The fi ndings are consistent with the view that depression is characterized by an explicit memory bias that may persist beyond a depressive episode. These fi ndingshaveimplicationsforcognitivetheoriesofvulnerabilitytodepressionaswellasclinicalinterventions.","PeriodicalId":20854,"journal":{"name":"Psychological bulletin","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":22.4,"publicationDate":"2022-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"47831068","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Jeromy Anglim, P. Dunlop, Serena Wee, Sharon Horwood, Joshua K. Wood, Andrew Marty
This study provides a comprehensive assessment of the associations of personality and intelligence. It presents a meta-analysis ( N = 162,636, k = 272) of domain, facet, and item-level correlations between personality and intelligence (general, fl uid, and crystallized) for the major Big Five and HEXACO hierarchical frameworks of personality: NEO Personality Inventory – Revised, Big Five Aspect Scales, Big Five Inventory – 2, and HEXACO Personality Inventory – Revised. It provides the fi rst meta-analysis of personality and intelligence to comprehensively examine (a) facet-level correlations for these hierarchical frameworks of personality, (b) item-level correlations, (c) domain- and facet-level predictive models. Age and sex differences in personality and intelligence, and study-level moderators, are also examined. The study was complemented by four of our own unpublished data sets ( N = 26,813) which were used to assess the ability of item-level models to provide generalizable prediction. Results showed that openness ( ρ = .20) and neuroticism ( ρ = − .09) were the strongest Big Five correlates of intelligence and that openness correlated more with crystallized than fl uid intelligence. At the facet level, traits related to intellectual engagement and unconventionality were more strongly related to intelligence than other openness facets, and sociability and orderliness were negatively correlated with intelligence. Facets of gregariousness and excitement seeking had stronger negative correlations, and openness to aesthetics, feelings, and values had stronger positive correlations with crystallized than fl uid intelligence. Facets explained more than twice the variance of domains. Overall, the results provide the most nuanced and robust evidence to date of the relationship between personality and intelligence. It is the fi rst to meta-analytically compare how intelligence relates to domains, facets, and items on the major hierarchical measures of personality. In so doing, it provides a robust empirical basis for informing discussion of the reciprocal pathways through which personality and intelligence interact.
{"title":"Personality and intelligence: A meta-analysis.","authors":"Jeromy Anglim, P. Dunlop, Serena Wee, Sharon Horwood, Joshua K. Wood, Andrew Marty","doi":"10.1037/bul0000373","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000373","url":null,"abstract":"This study provides a comprehensive assessment of the associations of personality and intelligence. It presents a meta-analysis ( N = 162,636, k = 272) of domain, facet, and item-level correlations between personality and intelligence (general, fl uid, and crystallized) for the major Big Five and HEXACO hierarchical frameworks of personality: NEO Personality Inventory – Revised, Big Five Aspect Scales, Big Five Inventory – 2, and HEXACO Personality Inventory – Revised. It provides the fi rst meta-analysis of personality and intelligence to comprehensively examine (a) facet-level correlations for these hierarchical frameworks of personality, (b) item-level correlations, (c) domain- and facet-level predictive models. Age and sex differences in personality and intelligence, and study-level moderators, are also examined. The study was complemented by four of our own unpublished data sets ( N = 26,813) which were used to assess the ability of item-level models to provide generalizable prediction. Results showed that openness ( ρ = .20) and neuroticism ( ρ = − .09) were the strongest Big Five correlates of intelligence and that openness correlated more with crystallized than fl uid intelligence. At the facet level, traits related to intellectual engagement and unconventionality were more strongly related to intelligence than other openness facets, and sociability and orderliness were negatively correlated with intelligence. Facets of gregariousness and excitement seeking had stronger negative correlations, and openness to aesthetics, feelings, and values had stronger positive correlations with crystallized than fl uid intelligence. Facets explained more than twice the variance of domains. Overall, the results provide the most nuanced and robust evidence to date of the relationship between personality and intelligence. It is the fi rst to meta-analytically compare how intelligence relates to domains, facets, and items on the major hierarchical measures of personality. In so doing, it provides a robust empirical basis for informing discussion of the reciprocal pathways through which personality and intelligence interact.","PeriodicalId":20854,"journal":{"name":"Psychological bulletin","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":22.4,"publicationDate":"2022-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"47470288","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
{"title":"Supplemental Material for Young People’s Perceptions of Their Parents’ Expectations and Criticism Are Increasing Over Time: Implications for Perfectionism","authors":"","doi":"10.1037/bul0000347.supp","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000347.supp","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":20854,"journal":{"name":"Psychological bulletin","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":22.4,"publicationDate":"2022-03-24","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41918476","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Leah A L Wang, Victoria Petrulla, Casey J Zampella, Rebecca Waller, Robert T Schultz
Gross motor ability is associated with profound differences in how children experience and interact with their social world. A rapidly growing literature on motor development in autism spectrum disorder (ASD) indicates that autistic individuals exhibit impairment in gross motor skills. However, due to substantial heterogeneity across studies, it remains unclear which gross motor skills are impaired in ASD, when and for whom these differences emerge, and whether motor and social impairments are related. The present article addressed these questions by synthesizing research on gross motor skills in ASD in two separate meta-analyses. The first examined gross motor deficits in ASD compared to neurotypical (NT) controls, aggregating data from 114 studies representing 6,423 autistic and 2,941 NT individuals. Results demonstrated a significant overall deficit in gross motor skills in ASD (Hedges' g = -1.04) that was robust to methodological and phenotypic variation and was significant at every level of the tested moderators. However, moderation analyses revealed that this deficit was most pronounced for object control skills (i.e., ball skills), clinical assessment measures, and movements of the upper extremities or the whole body. The second meta-analysis investigated whether gross motor and social skills are related in ASD, synthesizing data from 21 studies representing 654 autistic individuals. Findings revealed a modest but significant overall correlation between gross motor and social skills in ASD (r = 0.27). Collectively, results support the conclusion that motor deficits are tied to the core symptoms of ASD. Further research is needed to test the causality and directionality of this relationship. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).
大肌肉运动能力与儿童如何体验和与社会世界互动的深刻差异有关。越来越多关于自闭症谱系障碍(ASD)运动发育的文献表明,自闭症个体在大运动技能方面表现出损伤。然而,由于研究之间存在很大的异质性,目前尚不清楚哪些大运动技能在ASD中受损,这些差异何时以及为谁出现,以及运动和社交障碍是否相关。本文通过两项独立的荟萃分析,对ASD中粗大运动技能的综合研究来解决这些问题。第一项研究将ASD患者的大运动缺陷与神经性(NT)对照进行了比较,汇总了114项研究的数据,涉及6423名自闭症患者和2941名NT患者。结果显示,ASD患者在大肌肉运动技能方面存在显著的总体缺陷(Hedges’g = -1.04),这对方法学和表型变异是稳健的,并且在每个水平的测试调节因子上都是显著的。然而,适度分析显示,这种缺陷在物体控制技能(即球技能)、临床评估措施和上肢或全身运动方面最为明显。第二项荟萃分析综合了来自654名自闭症患者的21项研究的数据,调查了大肌肉运动和社交技能是否与自闭症有关。研究结果显示,ASD患者的大肌肉运动和社交技能之间存在适度但显著的总体相关性(r = 0.27)。总的来说,这些结果支持了运动缺陷与自闭症谱系障碍的核心症状有关的结论。需要进一步的研究来检验这种关系的因果关系和方向性。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"Gross motor impairment and its relation to social skills in autism spectrum disorder: A systematic review and two meta-analyses.","authors":"Leah A L Wang, Victoria Petrulla, Casey J Zampella, Rebecca Waller, Robert T Schultz","doi":"10.1037/bul0000358","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000358","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Gross motor ability is associated with profound differences in how children experience and interact with their social world. A rapidly growing literature on motor development in autism spectrum disorder (ASD) indicates that autistic individuals exhibit impairment in gross motor skills. However, due to substantial heterogeneity across studies, it remains unclear which gross motor skills are impaired in ASD, when and for whom these differences emerge, and whether motor and social impairments are related. The present article addressed these questions by synthesizing research on gross motor skills in ASD in two separate meta-analyses. The first examined gross motor deficits in ASD compared to neurotypical (NT) controls, aggregating data from 114 studies representing 6,423 autistic and 2,941 NT individuals. Results demonstrated a significant overall deficit in gross motor skills in ASD (Hedges' <i>g</i> = -1.04) that was robust to methodological and phenotypic variation and was significant at every level of the tested moderators. However, moderation analyses revealed that this deficit was most pronounced for object control skills (i.e., ball skills), clinical assessment measures, and movements of the upper extremities or the whole body. The second meta-analysis investigated whether gross motor and social skills are related in ASD, synthesizing data from 21 studies representing 654 autistic individuals. Findings revealed a modest but significant overall correlation between gross motor and social skills in ASD (<i>r</i> = 0.27). Collectively, results support the conclusion that motor deficits are tied to the core symptoms of ASD. Further research is needed to test the causality and directionality of this relationship. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":20854,"journal":{"name":"Psychological bulletin","volume":"148 3-4","pages":"273-300"},"PeriodicalIF":22.4,"publicationDate":"2022-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC9894569/pdf/nihms-1849086.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10606043","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Mingliang Yuan, Giuliana Spadaro, Shuxian Jin, Junhui Wu, Y. Kou, P. V. van Lange, D. Balliet
{"title":"Supplemental Material for Did Cooperation Among Strangers Decline in the United States? A Cross-Temporal Meta-Analysis of Social Dilemmas (1956–2017)","authors":"Mingliang Yuan, Giuliana Spadaro, Shuxian Jin, Junhui Wu, Y. Kou, P. V. van Lange, D. Balliet","doi":"10.1037/bul0000363.supp","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000363.supp","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":20854,"journal":{"name":"Psychological bulletin","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":22.4,"publicationDate":"2022-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"46517556","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
{"title":"Supplemental Material for Feeling Good, Doing Good, and Getting Ahead: A Meta-Analytic Investigation of the Outcomes of Prosocial Motivation at Work","authors":"Huiyao Liao, Rong Su, Thomas Ptashnik, J. Nielsen","doi":"10.1037/bul0000362.supp","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000362.supp","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":20854,"journal":{"name":"Psychological bulletin","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":22.4,"publicationDate":"2022-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"46845265","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
K. Imuta, Sumin Song, J. Henry, T. Ruffman, C. Peterson, V. Slaughter
{"title":"Supplemental Material for A Meta-Analytic Review on the Social–Emotional Intelligence Correlates of the Six Bullying Roles: Bullies, Followers, Victims, Bully-Victims, Defenders, and Outsiders","authors":"K. Imuta, Sumin Song, J. Henry, T. Ruffman, C. Peterson, V. Slaughter","doi":"10.1037/bul0000364.supp","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000364.supp","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":20854,"journal":{"name":"Psychological bulletin","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":22.4,"publicationDate":"2022-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"48951441","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}