首页 > 最新文献

Psychological bulletin最新文献

英文 中文
Evaluating the robustness of parameter estimates in cognitive models: A meta-analytic review of multinomial processing tree models across the multiverse of estimation methods. 评估认知模型参数估计的稳健性:在多种估算方法中对多叉处理树模型进行元分析回顾。
IF 17.3 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-08-01 Epub Date: 2024-06-27 DOI: 10.1037/bul0000434
Henrik Singmann, Daniel W Heck, Marius Barth, Edgar Erdfelder, Nina R Arnold, Frederik Aust, Jimmy Calanchini, Fabian E Gümüsdagli, Sebastian S Horn, David Kellen, Karl C Klauer, Dora Matzke, Franziska Meissner, Martha Michalkiewicz, Marie Luisa Schaper, Christoph Stahl, Beatrice G Kuhlmann, Julia Groß

Researchers have become increasingly aware that data-analysis decisions affect results. Here, we examine this issue systematically for multinomial processing tree (MPT) models, a popular class of cognitive models for categorical data. Specifically, we examine the robustness of MPT model parameter estimates that arise from two important decisions: the level of data aggregation (complete-pooling, no-pooling, or partial-pooling) and the statistical framework (frequentist or Bayesian). These decisions span a multiverse of estimation methods. We synthesized the data from 13,956 participants (164 published data sets) with a meta-analytic strategy and analyzed the magnitude of divergence between estimation methods for the parameters of nine popular MPT models in psychology (e.g., process-dissociation, source monitoring). We further examined moderators as potential sources of divergence. We found that the absolute divergence between estimation methods was small on average (<.04; with MPT parameters ranging between 0 and 1); in some cases, however, divergence amounted to nearly the maximum possible range (.97). Divergence was partly explained by few moderators (e.g., the specific MPT model parameter, uncertainty in parameter estimation), but not by other plausible candidate moderators (e.g., parameter trade-offs, parameter correlations) or their interactions. Partial-pooling methods showed the smallest divergence within and across levels of pooling and thus seem to be an appropriate default method. Using MPT models as an example, we show how transparency and robustness can be increased in the field of cognitive modeling. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

研究人员越来越意识到,数据分析决策会影响结果。在此,我们针对多叉处理树(MPT)模型系统地研究了这一问题,该模型是一类流行的分类数据认知模型。具体来说,我们研究了 MPT 模型参数估计的稳健性,这源于两个重要的决策:数据聚合水平(完全聚合、无聚合或部分聚合)和统计框架(频繁主义或贝叶斯)。这些决定涉及多种估算方法。我们采用元分析策略综合了来自 13956 名参与者(164 个已发表数据集)的数据,并分析了九种心理学常用 MPT 模型(如过程-解离、源监控)参数估计方法之间的差异程度。我们进一步研究了作为分歧潜在来源的调节因素。我们发现,估计方法之间的绝对分歧平均较小 (
{"title":"Evaluating the robustness of parameter estimates in cognitive models: A meta-analytic review of multinomial processing tree models across the multiverse of estimation methods.","authors":"Henrik Singmann, Daniel W Heck, Marius Barth, Edgar Erdfelder, Nina R Arnold, Frederik Aust, Jimmy Calanchini, Fabian E Gümüsdagli, Sebastian S Horn, David Kellen, Karl C Klauer, Dora Matzke, Franziska Meissner, Martha Michalkiewicz, Marie Luisa Schaper, Christoph Stahl, Beatrice G Kuhlmann, Julia Groß","doi":"10.1037/bul0000434","DOIUrl":"10.1037/bul0000434","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Researchers have become increasingly aware that data-analysis decisions affect results. Here, we examine this issue systematically for multinomial processing tree (MPT) models, a popular class of cognitive models for categorical data. Specifically, we examine the robustness of MPT model parameter estimates that arise from two important decisions: the level of data aggregation (complete-pooling, no-pooling, or partial-pooling) and the statistical framework (frequentist or Bayesian). These decisions span a <i>multiverse</i> of estimation methods. We synthesized the data from 13,956 participants (164 published data sets) with a meta-analytic strategy and analyzed the <i>magnitude of divergence</i> between estimation methods for the parameters of nine popular MPT models in psychology (e.g., process-dissociation, source monitoring). We further examined moderators as potential <i>sources of divergence</i>. We found that the absolute divergence between estimation methods was small on average (<.04; with MPT parameters ranging between 0 and 1); in some cases, however, divergence amounted to nearly the maximum possible range (.97). Divergence was partly explained by few moderators (e.g., the specific MPT model parameter, uncertainty in parameter estimation), but not by other plausible candidate moderators (e.g., parameter trade-offs, parameter correlations) or their interactions. Partial-pooling methods showed the smallest divergence within and across levels of pooling and thus seem to be an appropriate default method. Using MPT models as an example, we show how transparency and robustness can be increased in the field of cognitive modeling. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":20854,"journal":{"name":"Psychological bulletin","volume":" ","pages":"965-1003"},"PeriodicalIF":17.3,"publicationDate":"2024-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141458989","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Gender differences in sex drive: Reply to Conley and Yang (2024). 性欲的性别差异:对 Conley 和 Yang(2024 年)的答复。
IF 17.3 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-08-01 DOI: 10.1037/bul0000444
Julius Frankenbach, Marcel Weber, David D Loschelder, Helena Kilger, Malte Friese

Our meta-analysis on gender differences in sex drive found a stronger sex drive in men compared to women (Frankenbach et al., 2022). Conley and Yang (2024) criticized how we interpreted the findings and provided suggestions regarding the origins of these gender differences, an undertaking that we had refrained from doing in our original work. We concur with several important points made by Conley and Yang (2024): (a) women's sexual experiences are generally more negative than men's, which could partly explain why men report more sex drive; (b) lack of statistical moderation by some sociocultural variables does not imply that the sex drives of men and women are generally unaffected by the social environment; and (c) gender differences in sexuality are likely smaller than they are often portrayed in research, and that the practical impact of this difference is largely unknown. Still, we reject other assertions made by Conley and Yang (2024): (a) we did not frame our findings in support of the view that gender differences in sex drive are determined by biology, (b) we did not conflate response bias with sociocultural biases more broadly, and (c) we did not fail to incorporate and consider gendered cultural messages about sexuality in our methods and discussion. We make several suggestions about future research on these matters. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

我们对性欲性别差异的荟萃分析发现,与女性相比,男性的性欲更强(Frankenbach 等人,2022 年)。康利和杨(2024 年)批评了我们解释研究结果的方式,并就这些性别差异的起源提出了建议,而我们在最初的工作中并没有这样做。我们同意 Conley 和 Yang(2024 年)提出的几个重要观点:(a) 女性的性经历通常比男性更消极,这可以部分解释为什么男性报告的性欲更强;(b) 某些社会文化变量在统计上缺乏调节作用,并不意味着男性和女性的性欲通常不受社会环境的影响;(c) 性方面的性别差异可能比研究中通常描述的要小,而且这种差异的实际影响在很大程度上是未知的。尽管如此,我们仍然拒绝接受康利和杨(2024 年)的其他论断:(a) 我们的研究结果并不支持性欲的性别差异是由生物学决定的观点,(b) 我们并没有将反应偏差与更广泛的社会文化偏差混为一谈,(c) 我们在研究方法和讨论中并没有不考虑关于性的性别文化信息。我们就这些问题的未来研究提出了一些建议。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Gender differences in sex drive: Reply to Conley and Yang (2024).","authors":"Julius Frankenbach, Marcel Weber, David D Loschelder, Helena Kilger, Malte Friese","doi":"10.1037/bul0000444","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000444","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Our meta-analysis on gender differences in sex drive found a stronger sex drive in men compared to women (Frankenbach et al., 2022). Conley and Yang (2024) criticized how we interpreted the findings and provided suggestions regarding the origins of these gender differences, an undertaking that we had refrained from doing in our original work. We concur with several important points made by Conley and Yang (2024): (a) women's sexual experiences are generally more negative than men's, which could partly explain why men report more sex drive; (b) lack of statistical moderation by some sociocultural variables does not imply that the sex drives of men and women are generally unaffected by the social environment; and (c) gender differences in sexuality are likely smaller than they are often portrayed in research, and that the practical impact of this difference is largely unknown. Still, we reject other assertions made by Conley and Yang (2024): (a) we did not frame our findings in support of the view that gender differences in sex drive are determined by biology, (b) we did not conflate response bias with sociocultural biases more broadly, and (c) we did not fail to incorporate and consider gendered cultural messages about sexuality in our methods and discussion. We make several suggestions about future research on these matters. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":20854,"journal":{"name":"Psychological bulletin","volume":"150 8","pages":"1011-1019"},"PeriodicalIF":17.3,"publicationDate":"2024-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142018435","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Maternal and paternal sensitivity: Key determinants of child attachment security examined through meta-analysis. 母亲和父亲的敏感性:通过荟萃分析研究儿童依恋安全的关键决定因素。
IF 17.3 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-07-01 Epub Date: 2024-05-06 DOI: 10.1037/bul0000433
Sheri Madigan, Audrey-Ann Deneault, Robbie Duschinsky, Marian J Bakermans-Kranenburg, Carlo Schuengel, Marinus H van IJzendoorn, Anh Ly, R M Pasco Fearon, Rachel Eirich, Marije L Verhage

Sensitive caregiving behavior, which involves the ability to notice, interpret, and quickly respond to a child's signals of need and/or interest, is a central determinant of secure child-caregiver attachment. Yet, significant heterogeneity in effect sizes exists across the literature, and sources of heterogeneity have yet to be explained. For all child-caregiver dyads, there was a significant and positive pooled association between caregiver sensitivity and parent-child attachment (r = .25, 95% CI [.22, .28], k = 174, 230 effect sizes, N = 22,914). We also found a positive association between maternal sensitivity and child attachment security (r = .26, 95% CI [.22, .29], k = 159, 202 effect sizes, N = 21,483), which was equivalent in magnitude to paternal sensitivity and child attachment security (r = .21, 95% CI [.14, 27], k = 22, 23 effect sizes, N = 1,626). Maternal sensitivity was also negatively associated with all three classifications of insecure attachment (avoidant: k = 43, r = -.24 [-.34, -.13]; resistant: k = 43, r = -.12 [-.19, -.06]; disorganized: k = 24, r = -.19 [-.27, -.11]). For maternal sensitivity, associations were larger in studies that used the Attachment Q-Sort (vs. the Strange Situation), used the Maternal Behavior Q-Sort (vs. Ainsworth or Emotional Availability Scales), had strong (vs. poor) interrater measurement reliability, had a longer observation of sensitivity, and had less time elapse between assessments. For paternal sensitivity, associations were larger in older (vs. younger) fathers and children. These findings confirm the importance of both maternal and paternal sensitivity for the development of child attachment security and add understanding of the methodological and substantive factors that allow this effect to be observed. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

敏感的照料行为涉及到注意、解读和快速回应儿童需求和/或兴趣信号的能力,是儿童与照料者安全依恋关系的核心决定因素。然而,各种文献中的效应大小存在着明显的异质性,而异质性的来源尚待解释。在所有儿童-照料者二元组合中,照料者敏感性与亲子依恋之间存在显著的正相关(r = .25,95% CI [.22, .28],k = 174,230 个效应大小,N = 22,914 人)。我们还发现,母亲敏感性与儿童依恋安全感之间存在正相关(r = .26,95% CI [.22, .29],k = 159,202 个效应量,N = 21,483 ),其程度与父亲敏感性与儿童依恋安全感之间的正相关(r = .21,95% CI [.14, 27],k = 22,23 个效应量,N = 1,626)相当。母亲的敏感性也与所有三种不安全依恋分类呈负相关(回避型:k = 43,r = -.24 [-.34,-.13];抗拒型:k = 43,r = -.12 [-.19,-.06];无组织型:k = 24,r = -.19 [-.27,-.11])。对于母亲的敏感性,使用依恋 Q-分类法(与陌生情境法相比)、使用母亲行为 Q-分类法(与安斯沃斯量表或情感可得性量表相比)、互测可靠性强(与互测可靠性差相比)、敏感性观察时间较长、评估间隔时间较短的研究的相关性更大。就父亲的敏感性而言,年龄较大(与较小)的父亲和子女的相关性更大。这些研究结果证实了母亲和父亲的敏感性对儿童依恋安全感发展的重要性,并增加了对能够观察到这种效应的方法和实质因素的理解。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Maternal and paternal sensitivity: Key determinants of child attachment security examined through meta-analysis.","authors":"Sheri Madigan, Audrey-Ann Deneault, Robbie Duschinsky, Marian J Bakermans-Kranenburg, Carlo Schuengel, Marinus H van IJzendoorn, Anh Ly, R M Pasco Fearon, Rachel Eirich, Marije L Verhage","doi":"10.1037/bul0000433","DOIUrl":"10.1037/bul0000433","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Sensitive caregiving behavior, which involves the ability to notice, interpret, and quickly respond to a child's signals of need and/or interest, is a central determinant of secure child-caregiver attachment. Yet, significant heterogeneity in effect sizes exists across the literature, and sources of heterogeneity have yet to be explained. For all child-caregiver dyads, there was a significant and positive pooled association between caregiver sensitivity and parent-child attachment (<i>r</i> = .25, 95% CI [.22, .28], <i>k</i> = 174, 230 effect sizes, <i>N</i> = 22,914). We also found a positive association between maternal sensitivity and child attachment security (<i>r</i> = .26, 95% CI [.22, .29], <i>k</i> = 159, 202 effect sizes, <i>N</i> = 21,483), which was equivalent in magnitude to paternal sensitivity and child attachment security (<i>r</i> = .21, 95% CI [.14, 27], <i>k</i> = 22, 23 effect sizes, <i>N</i> = 1,626). Maternal sensitivity was also negatively associated with all three classifications of insecure attachment (avoidant: <i>k</i> = 43, <i>r</i> = -.24 [-.34, -.13]; resistant: <i>k</i> = 43, <i>r</i> = -.12 [-.19, -.06]; disorganized: <i>k</i> = 24, <i>r</i> = -.19 [-.27, -.11]). For maternal sensitivity, associations were larger in studies that used the Attachment Q-Sort (vs. the Strange Situation), used the Maternal Behavior Q-Sort (vs. Ainsworth or Emotional Availability Scales), had strong (vs. poor) interrater measurement reliability, had a longer observation of sensitivity, and had less time elapse between assessments. For paternal sensitivity, associations were larger in older (vs. younger) fathers and children. These findings confirm the importance of both maternal and paternal sensitivity for the development of child attachment security and add understanding of the methodological and substantive factors that allow this effect to be observed. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":20854,"journal":{"name":"Psychological bulletin","volume":" ","pages":"839-872"},"PeriodicalIF":17.3,"publicationDate":"2024-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140869484","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Health-promotion interventions targeting multiple behaviors: A meta-analytic review of general and behavior-specific processes of change. 针对多种行为的健康促进干预措施:对一般变化过程和特定行为变化过程的元分析回顾。
IF 17.3 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-07-01 Epub Date: 2024-06-24 DOI: 10.1037/bul0000427
Angela L Zhang, Sicong Liu, Benjamin X White, Xi C Liu, Marta Durantini, Man-Pui Sally Chan, Wenhao Dai, Yubo Zhou, Melody Leung, Qijia Ye, Devlin O'Keefe, Lidia Palmese, Dolores Albarracín

Although health-promotion interventions that recommend changes across multiple behavioral domains are a newer alternative to single-behavior interventions, their general efficacy and their mechanisms of change have not been fully ascertained. This comprehensive meta-analysis (6,878 effect sizes from 803 independent samples from 364 research reports, N = 186,729 participants) examined the association between the number of behavioral recommendations in multiple-behavior interventions and behavioral and clinical change across eight domains (i.e., diet, smoking, exercise, HIV [Human Immunodeficiency Virus] prevention, HIV testing, HIV treatment, alcohol use, and substance use). Results showed a positive, linear effect of the number of behavioral recommendations associated with behavioral and clinical change across all domains, although approximately 87% of the samples included between 0 and 4 behavioral recommendations. This linear relation was mediated by improvements in the psychological well-being of intervention recipients and, in several domains (i.e., HIV, alcohol use, and drug use), suggested behavioral cuing. However, changes in information, motivation, and behavioral skills did not mediate the impact of the number of recommendations on behavioral and clinical change. The implications of these findings for theory and future intervention design are discussed. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

尽管建议改变多个行为领域的健康促进干预是单一行为干预的新替代方案,但其总体疗效及其变化机制尚未完全确定。这项综合荟萃分析(来自 364 份研究报告的 803 个独立样本的 6,878 个效应大小,N = 186,729 名参与者)研究了多重行为干预中行为建议的数量与八个领域(即饮食、吸烟、运动、HIV(人类免疫缺陷病毒)预防、HIV 检测、HIV 治疗、酒精使用和药物使用)的行为和临床变化之间的关系。结果显示,行为建议的数量与所有领域的行为和临床变化呈正线性关系,尽管约 87% 的样本包含 0 到 4 项行为建议。这种线性关系是由干预接受者心理健康的改善所促成的,并且在几个领域(即艾滋病、饮酒和吸毒),这种关系还暗示了行为暗示。然而,信息、动机和行为技能方面的变化并没有调节建议数量对行为和临床变化的影响。本文讨论了这些发现对理论和未来干预设计的影响。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Health-promotion interventions targeting multiple behaviors: A meta-analytic review of general and behavior-specific processes of change.","authors":"Angela L Zhang, Sicong Liu, Benjamin X White, Xi C Liu, Marta Durantini, Man-Pui Sally Chan, Wenhao Dai, Yubo Zhou, Melody Leung, Qijia Ye, Devlin O'Keefe, Lidia Palmese, Dolores Albarracín","doi":"10.1037/bul0000427","DOIUrl":"10.1037/bul0000427","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Although health-promotion interventions that recommend changes across multiple behavioral domains are a newer alternative to single-behavior interventions, their general efficacy and their mechanisms of change have not been fully ascertained. This comprehensive meta-analysis (6,878 effect sizes from 803 independent samples from 364 research reports, <i>N</i> = 186,729 participants) examined the association between the number of behavioral recommendations in multiple-behavior interventions and behavioral and clinical change across eight domains (i.e., diet, smoking, exercise, HIV [Human Immunodeficiency Virus] prevention, HIV testing, HIV treatment, alcohol use, and substance use). Results showed a positive, linear effect of the number of behavioral recommendations associated with behavioral and clinical change across all domains, although approximately 87% of the samples included between 0 and 4 behavioral recommendations. This linear relation was mediated by improvements in the psychological well-being of intervention recipients and, in several domains (i.e., HIV, alcohol use, and drug use), suggested behavioral cuing. However, changes in information, motivation, and behavioral skills did not mediate the impact of the number of recommendations on behavioral and clinical change. The implications of these findings for theory and future intervention design are discussed. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":20854,"journal":{"name":"Psychological bulletin","volume":" ","pages":"798-838"},"PeriodicalIF":17.3,"publicationDate":"2024-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141446860","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
A systematic review and meta-analysis of predictors of response to trauma-focused psychotherapy for posttraumatic stress disorder. 针对创伤后应激障碍的创伤焦点心理疗法反应预测因素的系统回顾和荟萃分析。
IF 17.3 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-07-01 Epub Date: 2024-06-17 DOI: 10.1037/bul0000438
Dharani Keyan, Nadine Garland, Jasmine Choi-Christou, Jenny Tran, Meaghan O'Donnell, Richard A Bryant

Although trauma-focused psychotherapy (T-F psychotherapy) is the treatment of choice for posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD), up to one half of patients do not respond to this treatment. Attempts to improve response to T-F psychotherapy have focused on augmenting fear extinction-based factors. Here, a systematic and meta-analytic review of predictors of T-F psychotherapy outcome was conducted with the goal of using an aggregate data-driven approach to elucidate baseline factors associated with treatment outcome. There were 114 studies that met inclusion criteria (N = 61, 970; Mage = 40.1 years; 40.1% female). There were 237 effect sizes across 24 meta-analytic categories. Poorer treatment response is associated with lower pretreatment levels of activation of fear-related brain regions, psychophysiological reactivity to fear provocation, trauma-related cognitions, anger, depression, high-risk alleles of genes linked to fear, lower levels of executive control, and social support. A range of other factors also predicted poorer responses including being male, non-Caucasian, older in age, early trauma occurrence, more trauma experience, history of combat trauma, as well as comorbid sleep, pain, poor quality life, and alcohol abuse difficulties. This review provides one potential explanation for the limited success of T-F psychotherapy augmentation strategies that have focused only on fear circuity mechanisms at the exclusion of other factors. Here, poor response relating to predictors of early trauma onset and comorbidity are consistent with clinical presentations of complex PTSD, which may suggest T-F psychotherapy is less effective for this condition. This collective evidence suggests that clinicians should consider a tailored approach that targets potential barriers to successful treatment response. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

尽管创伤心理疗法(T-F 心理疗法)是治疗创伤后应激障碍(PTSD)的首选疗法,但多达一半的患者对这种疗法没有反应。改善创伤后应激障碍心理疗法反应的尝试主要集中在增强基于恐惧消退的因素上。在此,我们对T-F心理疗法疗效的预测因素进行了系统性的荟萃分析回顾,目的是采用综合数据驱动法来阐明与疗效相关的基线因素。共有 114 项研究符合纳入标准(N = 61,970;年龄 = 40.1 岁;40.1% 为女性)。在 24 个元分析类别中有 237 个效应大小。较差的治疗反应与治疗前较低水平的恐惧相关脑区激活、对恐惧激起的心理生理反应、创伤相关认知、愤怒、抑郁、与恐惧相关基因的高风险等位基因、较低水平的执行控制和社会支持有关。一系列其他因素也预示着较差的反应,包括男性、非白种人、年龄较大、创伤发生较早、创伤经历较多、战斗创伤史,以及合并睡眠、疼痛、生活质量差和酗酒等困难。这篇综述提供了一个潜在的解释,说明为什么 T-F 心理治疗增强策略的成功率有限,因为这些策略只关注恐惧循环机制,而忽略了其他因素。在这里,与早期创伤发作的预测因素和合并症有关的不良反应与复杂创伤后应激障碍的临床表现一致,这可能表明 T-F 心理治疗对这种情况的效果较差。这些综合证据表明,临床医生应考虑针对成功治疗反应的潜在障碍采取有针对性的方法。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"A systematic review and meta-analysis of predictors of response to trauma-focused psychotherapy for posttraumatic stress disorder.","authors":"Dharani Keyan, Nadine Garland, Jasmine Choi-Christou, Jenny Tran, Meaghan O'Donnell, Richard A Bryant","doi":"10.1037/bul0000438","DOIUrl":"10.1037/bul0000438","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Although trauma-focused psychotherapy (T-F psychotherapy) is the treatment of choice for posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD), up to one half of patients do not respond to this treatment. Attempts to improve response to T-F psychotherapy have focused on augmenting fear extinction-based factors. Here, a systematic and meta-analytic review of predictors of T-F psychotherapy outcome was conducted with the goal of using an aggregate data-driven approach to elucidate baseline factors associated with treatment outcome. There were 114 studies that met inclusion criteria (<i>N</i> = 61, 970; <i>M</i><sub>age</sub> = 40.1 years; 40.1% female). There were 237 effect sizes across 24 meta-analytic categories. Poorer treatment response is associated with lower pretreatment levels of activation of fear-related brain regions, psychophysiological reactivity to fear provocation, trauma-related cognitions, anger, depression, high-risk alleles of genes linked to fear, lower levels of executive control, and social support. A range of other factors also predicted poorer responses including being male, non-Caucasian, older in age, early trauma occurrence, more trauma experience, history of combat trauma, as well as comorbid sleep, pain, poor quality life, and alcohol abuse difficulties. This review provides one potential explanation for the limited success of T-F psychotherapy augmentation strategies that have focused only on fear circuity mechanisms at the exclusion of other factors. Here, poor response relating to predictors of early trauma onset and comorbidity are consistent with clinical presentations of complex PTSD, which may suggest T-F psychotherapy is less effective for this condition. This collective evidence suggests that clinicians should consider a tailored approach that targets potential barriers to successful treatment response. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":20854,"journal":{"name":"Psychological bulletin","volume":" ","pages":"767-797"},"PeriodicalIF":17.3,"publicationDate":"2024-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141331562","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Parent-child boundary dissolution and children's psychological difficulties: A meta-analytic review. 亲子界限解体与儿童的心理障碍:荟萃分析综述。
IF 17.3 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-07-01 Epub Date: 2024-06-17 DOI: 10.1037/bul0000440
Morgan J Thompson, Cory R Platts, Patrick T Davies

Boundary dissolution has broadly been defined as the breakdown of boundaries and loss of psychological distinctiveness in the parent-child subsystem. Qualitative reviews have highlighted the developmental and clinical value of examining boundary dissolution as a multidimensional construct. Though prior work suggests patterns share minimal variance, research has yet to quantitatively synthesize the weighted effect of distinct patterns. The primary aim of this meta-analysis was to aggregate empirical research on associations between boundary dissolution patterns and children's internalizing and externalizing symptoms. Four patterns of boundary dissolution were identified across developmental, clinical, and family systems literatures: (a) enmeshment-entanglement and blurring of the intergenerational parent-child boundary through psychologically controlling and intrusive behaviors, (b) disorganization-chaotic parent-child boundary (e.g., inexplicable, contradictory behaviors, and responses) reflecting no coherent pattern of relating, (c) caregiving-child functions as a caregiver providing parents with instrumental and emotional support and guidance, and (d) coerciveness-child operates as a disciplinarian or authoritarian to intimidate and control parents. The meta-analysis reviewed 478 studies. Although each boundary dissolution pattern was associated with internalizing and externalizing symptoms, weighted effects across patterns significantly varied in magnitude. Regarding externalizing symptoms, the weighted effect of enmeshment was stronger relative to the weighted effect of caregiving. Turning to internalizing symptoms, the weighted effect of enmeshment was stronger than the weighted effect of caregiving and coerciveness. Additionally, the weighted effect of disorganization was stronger than the weighted effect of caregiving. The robustness of weighted effects depended on child, contextual, and methodological characteristics as well as time lag. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

界限解体被广泛定义为亲子子系统中界限的破裂和心理独特性的丧失。定性评论强调了将边界解体作为多维结构进行研究的发展和临床价值。尽管之前的研究表明,各种模式之间的差异极小,但研究还没有对不同模式的加权效应进行定量综合。本荟萃分析的主要目的是汇总有关边界解体模式与儿童内化和外化症状之间关联的实证研究。在发展、临床和家庭系统文献中,确定了四种边界解体模式:(a) 缠结--通过心理上的控制和侵扰行为,缠结和模糊亲子间的界限;(b) 杂乱--混乱的亲子界限(如无法解释、自相矛盾的行为和反应),反映出没有连贯的关系模式;(c) 照顾--子女作为照顾者,为父母提供工具性和情感上的支持和指导;(d) 强制--子女作为管教者或独裁者,恐吓和控制父母。荟萃分析审查了 478 项研究。虽然每种边界解体模式都与内化和外化症状有关,但不同模式的加权效应在程度上存在显著差异。就外化症状而言,与照顾的加权效应相比,介入的加权效应更强。至于内化症状,"融入 "的加权效应强于 "照顾 "和 "胁迫 "的加权效应。此外,混乱的加权效应强于照顾的加权效应。加权效应的稳健性取决于儿童、环境和方法特征以及时滞。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Parent-child boundary dissolution and children's psychological difficulties: A meta-analytic review.","authors":"Morgan J Thompson, Cory R Platts, Patrick T Davies","doi":"10.1037/bul0000440","DOIUrl":"10.1037/bul0000440","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Boundary dissolution has broadly been defined as the breakdown of boundaries and loss of psychological distinctiveness in the parent-child subsystem. Qualitative reviews have highlighted the developmental and clinical value of examining boundary dissolution as a multidimensional construct. Though prior work suggests patterns share minimal variance, research has yet to quantitatively synthesize the weighted effect of distinct patterns. The primary aim of this meta-analysis was to aggregate empirical research on associations between boundary dissolution patterns and children's internalizing and externalizing symptoms. Four patterns of boundary dissolution were identified across developmental, clinical, and family systems literatures: (a) enmeshment-entanglement and blurring of the intergenerational parent-child boundary through psychologically controlling and intrusive behaviors, (b) disorganization-chaotic parent-child boundary (e.g., inexplicable, contradictory behaviors, and responses) reflecting no coherent pattern of relating, (c) caregiving-child functions as a caregiver providing parents with instrumental and emotional support and guidance, and (d) coerciveness-child operates as a disciplinarian or authoritarian to intimidate and control parents. The meta-analysis reviewed 478 studies. Although each boundary dissolution pattern was associated with internalizing and externalizing symptoms, weighted effects across patterns significantly varied in magnitude. Regarding externalizing symptoms, the weighted effect of enmeshment was stronger relative to the weighted effect of caregiving. Turning to internalizing symptoms, the weighted effect of enmeshment was stronger than the weighted effect of caregiving and coerciveness. Additionally, the weighted effect of disorganization was stronger than the weighted effect of caregiving. The robustness of weighted effects depended on child, contextual, and methodological characteristics as well as time lag. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":20854,"journal":{"name":"Psychological bulletin","volume":" ","pages":"873-919"},"PeriodicalIF":17.3,"publicationDate":"2024-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141331563","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Digital data and personality: A systematic review and meta-analysis of human perception and computer prediction. 数字数据与人格:人类感知与计算机预测的系统回顾与荟萃分析。
IF 17.3 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-06-01 Epub Date: 2024-05-16 DOI: 10.1037/bul0000430
Joanne Hinds, Adam N Joinson

In recent years, our increasing use of technology has resulted in the production of vast amounts of data. Consequently, many researchers have analyzed digital data in attempt to understand its relationship with individuals' personalities. Such endeavors have inspired efforts from divergent fields, resulting in widely dispersed findings that are seldom synthesized. In this two-part study, we draw from two distinct areas of personality prediction across psychology and computer science to explore the convergent validity of self-reports with human perception and machine learning algorithms, the identifiability of the Big Five traits, and the predictability of different types of data. In Study 1, five meta-analyses of human perception studies integrating findings from 24,124 individuals rated across 30 independent samples demonstrated moderate convergent validity across all traits (ranging from ρ = 0.38 for Neuroticism, to ρ = 0.57 for Openness). In Study 2, a multilevel meta-analysis of computer prediction studies reporting 534 effect sizes across 42 studies also demonstrated moderate convergent validity (ρ = 0.30). Multivariate analyses of the significant moderators highlighted that X, Facebook, Sina Weibo, videos, and smartphones had a negative impact on the variance identified. Finally, in synthesizing the extant literature, we discuss the measures used to assess personality and the analytical approaches adopted. We identify the strengths and limitations across each field and explain how interdisciplinary methodologies could advance the testing and development of psychological theory. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

近年来,我们越来越多地使用科技,从而产生了大量数据。因此,许多研究人员对数字数据进行了分析,试图了解这些数据与个人性格之间的关系。这些努力激发了来自不同领域的研究人员的积极性,但研究结果却非常分散,很少得到综合。在这项由两部分组成的研究中,我们从心理学和计算机科学两个不同的人格预测领域出发,探讨了自我报告与人类感知和机器学习算法的趋同有效性、五大特质的可识别性以及不同类型数据的可预测性。在研究 1 中,对人类感知研究进行了五项元分析,整合了 30 个独立样本中 24124 人的评价结果,结果表明所有特质都具有适度的趋同有效性(神经质的趋同有效性为 ρ = 0.38,开放性的趋同有效性为 ρ = 0.57)。在研究 2 中,对 42 项研究中 534 个效应大小的计算机预测研究进行的多层次荟萃分析也证明了中度的趋同有效性(ρ = 0.30)。对重要调节因素的多变量分析表明,X、Facebook、新浪微博、视频和智能手机对所发现的变异具有负面影响。最后,在总结现有文献时,我们讨论了用于评估人格的测量方法和采用的分析方法。我们指出了每个领域的优势和局限性,并解释了跨学科方法如何推动心理学理论的检验和发展。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"Digital data and personality: A systematic review and meta-analysis of human perception and computer prediction.","authors":"Joanne Hinds, Adam N Joinson","doi":"10.1037/bul0000430","DOIUrl":"10.1037/bul0000430","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>In recent years, our increasing use of technology has resulted in the production of vast amounts of data. Consequently, many researchers have analyzed digital data in attempt to understand its relationship with individuals' personalities. Such endeavors have inspired efforts from divergent fields, resulting in widely dispersed findings that are seldom synthesized. In this two-part study, we draw from two distinct areas of personality prediction across psychology and computer science to explore the convergent validity of self-reports with human perception and machine learning algorithms, the identifiability of the Big Five traits, and the predictability of different types of data. In Study 1, five meta-analyses of human perception studies integrating findings from 24,124 individuals rated across 30 independent samples demonstrated moderate convergent validity across all traits (ranging from ρ = 0.38 for Neuroticism, to ρ = 0.57 for Openness). In Study 2, a multilevel meta-analysis of computer prediction studies reporting 534 effect sizes across 42 studies also demonstrated moderate convergent validity (ρ = 0.30). Multivariate analyses of the significant moderators highlighted that X, Facebook, Sina Weibo, videos, and smartphones had a negative impact on the variance identified. Finally, in synthesizing the extant literature, we discuss the measures used to assess personality and the analytical approaches adopted. We identify the strengths and limitations across each field and explain how interdisciplinary methodologies could advance the testing and development of psychological theory. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":20854,"journal":{"name":"Psychological bulletin","volume":" ","pages":"727-766"},"PeriodicalIF":17.3,"publicationDate":"2024-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140945727","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Development of narcissism across the life span: A meta-analytic review of longitudinal studies. 自恋在一生中的发展:纵向研究的元分析回顾。
IF 17.3 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-06-01 DOI: 10.1037/bul0000436
Ulrich Orth, Samantha Krauss, Mitja D Back

This meta-analytic review investigated the development of narcissism across the life span, by synthesizing the available longitudinal data on mean-level change and rank-order stability. Three factors of narcissism were examined: agentic, antagonistic, and neurotic narcissism. Analyses were based on data from 51 samples, including 37,247 participants. As effect size measures, we used the standardized mean change d per year and test-retest correlations that were corrected for attenuation due to measurement error. The results suggested that narcissism typically decreases from age 8 to 77 years (i.e., the observed age range), with aggregated changes of d = -0.28 for agentic narcissism, d = -0.41 for antagonistic narcissism, and d = -0.55 for neurotic narcissism. Rank-order stability of narcissism was high, with average values of .73 (agentic), .68 (antagonistic), and .60 (neurotic), based on an average time lag of 11.42 years. Rank-order stability did not vary as a function of age. However, rank-order stability declined as a function of time lag, asymptotically approaching values of .62 (agentic), .52 (antagonistic), and .33 (neurotic) across long time lags. Moderator analyses indicated that the findings on mean-level change and rank-order stability held across gender and birth cohort. The meta-analytic data set included mostly Western and White/European samples, pointing to the need of conducting more research with non-Western and ethnically diverse samples. In sum, the findings suggest that agentic, antagonistic, and neurotic narcissism show normative declines across the life span and that individual differences in these factors are moderately (neurotic) to highly (agentic, antagonistic) stable over time. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

本荟萃分析综述通过综合有关平均水平变化和等级顺序稳定性的现有纵向数据,研究了自恋在整个生命周期中的发展。研究了自恋的三个因素:代理型自恋、对抗型自恋和神经质自恋。分析基于 51 个样本的数据,包括 37 247 名参与者。作为效果大小的衡量标准,我们使用了标准化的每年平均变化 d 和测试-再测试相关性,并对测量误差造成的衰减进行了校正。结果表明,自恋通常会在 8 岁到 77 岁(即观察到的年龄范围)之间下降,代理型自恋的总变化 d = -0.28,对抗型自恋的总变化 d = -0.41,神经型自恋的总变化 d = -0.55。根据 11.42 年的平均时滞,自恋的等级稳定性很高,平均值为 0.73(代理型)、0.68(对抗型)和 0.60(神经型)。等级顺序稳定性与年龄无关。然而,秩序稳定性随时间滞后而下降,在长时间滞后的情况下,秩序稳定性逐渐接近 0.62(积极型)、0.52(对抗型)和 0.33(神经型)的值。调节分析表明,关于平均水平变化和秩序稳定性的研究结果在不同性别和出生组群中都成立。元分析数据集主要包括西方和白/欧洲样本,这表明需要对非西方和不同种族的样本进行更多的研究。总之,研究结果表明,代理人自恋、对抗性自恋和神经质自恋在整个生命周期中呈现出正常的下降趋势,这些因素的个体差异随着时间的推移呈现出中度(神经质)到高度(代理人自恋、对抗性自恋)的稳定性。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"Development of narcissism across the life span: A meta-analytic review of longitudinal studies.","authors":"Ulrich Orth, Samantha Krauss, Mitja D Back","doi":"10.1037/bul0000436","DOIUrl":"10.1037/bul0000436","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>This meta-analytic review investigated the development of narcissism across the life span, by synthesizing the available longitudinal data on mean-level change and rank-order stability. Three factors of narcissism were examined: agentic, antagonistic, and neurotic narcissism. Analyses were based on data from 51 samples, including 37,247 participants. As effect size measures, we used the standardized mean change d per year and test-retest correlations that were corrected for attenuation due to measurement error. The results suggested that narcissism typically decreases from age 8 to 77 years (i.e., the observed age range), with aggregated changes of d = -0.28 for agentic narcissism, d = -0.41 for antagonistic narcissism, and d = -0.55 for neurotic narcissism. Rank-order stability of narcissism was high, with average values of .73 (agentic), .68 (antagonistic), and .60 (neurotic), based on an average time lag of 11.42 years. Rank-order stability did not vary as a function of age. However, rank-order stability declined as a function of time lag, asymptotically approaching values of .62 (agentic), .52 (antagonistic), and .33 (neurotic) across long time lags. Moderator analyses indicated that the findings on mean-level change and rank-order stability held across gender and birth cohort. The meta-analytic data set included mostly Western and White/European samples, pointing to the need of conducting more research with non-Western and ethnically diverse samples. In sum, the findings suggest that agentic, antagonistic, and neurotic narcissism show normative declines across the life span and that individual differences in these factors are moderately (neurotic) to highly (agentic, antagonistic) stable over time. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":20854,"journal":{"name":"Psychological bulletin","volume":"150 6","pages":"643-665"},"PeriodicalIF":17.3,"publicationDate":"2024-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141591161","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Influences of past moral behavior on future behavior: A review of sequential moral behavior studies using meta-analytic techniques. 过去的道德行为对未来行为的影响:使用元分析技术对连续道德行为研究进行回顾。
IF 17.3 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-06-01 DOI: 10.1037/bul0000441
Rose Ferguson, Leah Kaufmann, Aimee Brown, Xochitl de la Piedad Garcia

Experimental research on sequential moral behavior (SMB) has found that engaging in an initial moral (or immoral) behavior can sometimes lead to moral balancing (i.e., switching between positive and negative behavior) and sometimes to moral consistency (i.e., maintaining a consistent pattern of positive or negative behavior). In two meta-analyses, we present the first comprehensive syntheses of SMB studies and test moderators to identify the conditions under which moral balancing and moral consistency are most likely to occur. Meta-Analysis 1 (k = 217 effect sizes, N = 31,242) revealed that engaging in an initial positive behavior only reliably resulted in moral licensing (i.e., balancing) in studies that measured engagement in negative target behaviors (Hedges' g = 0.25, 95% CI [0.16, 0.44]) and only resulted in positive consistency in foot-in-the-door studies using prosocial requests (Hedges' g = -0.44, 95% CI [-0.59, -0.29]). Meta-Analysis 2 (k = 132 effect sizes, N = 14,443) revealed that engaging in an initial negative behavior only reliably resulted in moral compensation (i.e., balancing) in studies that measured engagement in positive target behaviors (Hedges' g = 0.27, 95% CI [0.18, 0.37]). We found no evidence for reliable negative consistency effects in any conditions. These results cannot be readily explained by current theories of SMB effects, and so further research is needed to better understand the mechanisms that drive moral balancing and consistency under the conditions observed. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

有关连续道德行为(SMB)的实验研究发现,参与最初的道德(或不道德)行为有时会导致道德平衡(即在积极和消极行为之间转换),有时会导致道德一致性(即保持积极或消极行为的一贯模式)。在两项荟萃分析中,我们首次对SMB研究进行了全面综合,并测试了调节因子,以确定在何种条件下最有可能出现道德平衡和道德一致性。元分析 1(k = 217 个效应大小,N = 31,242 个)显示,在测量参与消极目标行为的研究中,参与最初的积极行为只会可靠地导致道德许可(即平衡)(Hedges' g = 0.25,95% CI [0.16,0.44]),而在使用亲社会请求的门脚研究中,参与最初的积极行为只会导致积极一致性(Hedges' g = -0.44,95% CI [-0.59,-0.29])。元分析 2(k = 132 个效应大小,N = 14443)显示,只有在测量参与积极目标行为的研究中,参与最初的消极行为才会可靠地导致道德补偿(即平衡)(Hedges' g = 0.27,95% CI [0.18,0.37])。在任何条件下,我们都没有发现可靠的负面一致性效应的证据。这些结果无法用当前的 SMB 效应理论轻易解释,因此需要进一步研究,以更好地了解在观察到的条件下驱动道德平衡和一致性的机制。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Influences of past moral behavior on future behavior: A review of sequential moral behavior studies using meta-analytic techniques.","authors":"Rose Ferguson, Leah Kaufmann, Aimee Brown, Xochitl de la Piedad Garcia","doi":"10.1037/bul0000441","DOIUrl":"10.1037/bul0000441","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Experimental research on sequential moral behavior (SMB) has found that engaging in an initial moral (or immoral) behavior can sometimes lead to moral balancing (i.e., switching between positive and negative behavior) and sometimes to moral consistency (i.e., maintaining a consistent pattern of positive or negative behavior). In two meta-analyses, we present the first comprehensive syntheses of SMB studies and test moderators to identify the conditions under which moral balancing and moral consistency are most likely to occur. Meta-Analysis 1 (k = 217 effect sizes, N = 31,242) revealed that engaging in an initial positive behavior only reliably resulted in moral licensing (i.e., balancing) in studies that measured engagement in negative target behaviors (Hedges' g = 0.25, 95% CI [0.16, 0.44]) and only resulted in positive consistency in foot-in-the-door studies using prosocial requests (Hedges' g = -0.44, 95% CI [-0.59, -0.29]). Meta-Analysis 2 (k = 132 effect sizes, N = 14,443) revealed that engaging in an initial negative behavior only reliably resulted in moral compensation (i.e., balancing) in studies that measured engagement in positive target behaviors (Hedges' g = 0.27, 95% CI [0.18, 0.37]). We found no evidence for reliable negative consistency effects in any conditions. These results cannot be readily explained by current theories of SMB effects, and so further research is needed to better understand the mechanisms that drive moral balancing and consistency under the conditions observed. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":20854,"journal":{"name":"Psychological bulletin","volume":"150 6","pages":"694-726"},"PeriodicalIF":17.3,"publicationDate":"2024-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141591163","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Exposure to community violence and parenting behaviors: A meta-analytic review. 接触社区暴力与养育行为:荟萃分析综述。
IF 17.3 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-06-01 DOI: 10.1037/bul0000435
Daneele Thorpe, Rebecca Mirhashem, Tori Peña, Jill Smokoski, Kristin Bernard

This meta-analysis examines the association between exposure to community violence and parenting behaviors (i.e., positive parenting, harsh/neglectful parenting, parent-child relationship quality, and behavior control). A systematic search yielded 437 articles that measured community violence exposure before or at the time of parenting, assessed parenting, and were available in English. There were 342 effect sizes across parenting constructs: positive (k = 101; 68 studies), harsh/neglectful (k = 95; 60 studies), relationship quality (k = 68; 41 studies), and behavior control (k = 78; 51 studies), from 160 reports representing 147 distinct studies. Results of the three-level meta-analyses found small but significant effects between community violence and positive parenting (r = -.059, 95% CI [-.086, -.032]; 95% PI [-.268, .151]), harsh/neglectful parenting (r = .133, 95% CI [.100, .166]; 95% PI [-.107, .372]), parent-child relationship quality (r = -.106, 95% CI [-.145, -.067]; 95% PI [-.394, .182]), and behavior control (r = -.047, 95% CI [-.089, -.005]; 95% PI [-.331, .237]). The association between exposure to community violence and harsh/neglectful parenting and behavior control was moderated by the type of exposure to community violence, informant or source of community violence and parenting data, child age, sex, and race/ethnicity. Given the substantial degree of heterogeneity in overall effect sizes, implications for policy and intervention are tentatively considered while emphasizing that more empirical research on the association between community violence and parenting is essential for advancing the field. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

本荟萃分析研究了社区暴力暴露与养育行为(即积极养育、严厉/忽视养育、亲子关系质量和行为控制)之间的关系。通过系统性检索,我们找到了 437 篇文章,这些文章测量了在养育子女之前或当时所接触的社区暴力,评估了养育子女的情况,并且是用英语撰写的。从代表 147 项不同研究的 160 份报告中得出了 342 个效应大小,包括:积极养育(k = 101;68 项研究)、严厉/忽视(k = 95;60 项研究)、关系质量(k = 68;41 项研究)和行为控制(k = 78;51 项研究)。三级荟萃分析结果发现,社区暴力与积极养育(r = -.059,95% CI [-.086,-.032];95% PI [-.268,.151])、严厉/忽视养育(r = .133,95% CI [.100,.166];95% PI [-.107,.372])、亲子关系质量(r = -.106,95% CI [-.145,-.067];95% PI [-.394,.182])和行为控制(r = -.047,95% CI [-.089,-.005];95% PI [-.331,.237])。暴露于社区暴力与严厉/忽视型养育和行为控制之间的关联受暴露于社区暴力的类型、社区暴力和养育数据的提供者或来源、儿童年龄、性别和种族/民族的影响。鉴于总体效应大小存在很大程度的异质性,我们初步考虑了对政策和干预措施的影响,同时强调对社区暴力与养育子女之间的关联进行更多的实证研究对于推动该领域的发展至关重要。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"Exposure to community violence and parenting behaviors: A meta-analytic review.","authors":"Daneele Thorpe, Rebecca Mirhashem, Tori Peña, Jill Smokoski, Kristin Bernard","doi":"10.1037/bul0000435","DOIUrl":"10.1037/bul0000435","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>This meta-analysis examines the association between exposure to community violence and parenting behaviors (i.e., positive parenting, harsh/neglectful parenting, parent-child relationship quality, and behavior control). A systematic search yielded 437 articles that measured community violence exposure before or at the time of parenting, assessed parenting, and were available in English. There were 342 effect sizes across parenting constructs: positive (k = 101; 68 studies), harsh/neglectful (k = 95; 60 studies), relationship quality (k = 68; 41 studies), and behavior control (k = 78; 51 studies), from 160 reports representing 147 distinct studies. Results of the three-level meta-analyses found small but significant effects between community violence and positive parenting (r = -.059, 95% CI [-.086, -.032]; 95% PI [-.268, .151]), harsh/neglectful parenting (r = .133, 95% CI [.100, .166]; 95% PI [-.107, .372]), parent-child relationship quality (r = -.106, 95% CI [-.145, -.067]; 95% PI [-.394, .182]), and behavior control (r = -.047, 95% CI [-.089, -.005]; 95% PI [-.331, .237]). The association between exposure to community violence and harsh/neglectful parenting and behavior control was moderated by the type of exposure to community violence, informant or source of community violence and parenting data, child age, sex, and race/ethnicity. Given the substantial degree of heterogeneity in overall effect sizes, implications for policy and intervention are tentatively considered while emphasizing that more empirical research on the association between community violence and parenting is essential for advancing the field. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":20854,"journal":{"name":"Psychological bulletin","volume":"150 6","pages":"666-693"},"PeriodicalIF":17.3,"publicationDate":"2024-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141591162","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Psychological bulletin
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1