首页 > 最新文献

Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology最新文献

英文 中文
Personal sense of power predicts financial risk-taking propensity: But only when risk-related decisions are made without cognitive load. 快讯个人力量感可预测金融风险倾向--但仅限于在无认知负荷的情况下做出风险相关决策时。
IF 1.5 3区 心理学 Q4 PHYSIOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-09-25 DOI: 10.1177/17470218241280654
Katarzyna Sekścińska, Diana Jaworska, Joanna Rudzińska-Wojciechowska

A significant relationship between a sense of power and financial risk-taking has been established in the literature. However, the boundary conditions for this relationship remain unclear. This article presents the results of an online experimental study (N = 192) that explores the moderating role of cognitive load in the relationship between power and financial risk-taking in the domains of gambling and investing. The findings validate a positive association between a sense of power and financial risk-taking, alongside a negative impact of cognitive load on financial risk. Notably, cognitive load moderates the relationship between power and financial risk-taking in a way that the link is positive when individuals have full access to their cognitive resources, but it becomes nonsignificant when they are under cognitive load.

权力感与金融风险承担之间的重要关系已在文献中得到证实。然而,这种关系的边界条件仍不明确。本文介绍了一项在线实验研究(N = 192)的结果,该研究探讨了认知负荷在赌博和投资领域权力与金融风险承担之间关系中的调节作用。研究结果验证了权力感与金融冒险之间的正相关,以及认知负荷对金融风险的负面影响。值得注意的是,认知负荷调节了权力与金融冒险之间的关系,当个体能够充分使用其认知资源时,两者之间的联系是正向的,但当个体处于认知负荷下时,两者之间的联系就变得不显著了。
{"title":"Personal sense of power predicts financial risk-taking propensity: But only when risk-related decisions are made without cognitive load.","authors":"Katarzyna Sekścińska, Diana Jaworska, Joanna Rudzińska-Wojciechowska","doi":"10.1177/17470218241280654","DOIUrl":"10.1177/17470218241280654","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>A significant relationship between a sense of power and financial risk-taking has been established in the literature. However, the boundary conditions for this relationship remain unclear. This article presents the results of an online experimental study (<i>N</i> = 192) that explores the moderating role of cognitive load in the relationship between power and financial risk-taking in the domains of gambling and investing. The findings validate a positive association between a sense of power and financial risk-taking, alongside a negative impact of cognitive load on financial risk. Notably, cognitive load moderates the relationship between power and financial risk-taking in a way that the link is positive when individuals have full access to their cognitive resources, but it becomes nonsignificant when they are under cognitive load.</p>","PeriodicalId":20869,"journal":{"name":"Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology","volume":" ","pages":"17470218241280654"},"PeriodicalIF":1.5,"publicationDate":"2024-09-25","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142018437","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Speaker variability, but not bilingualism, influences cross-situational word learning. 表达:说话者的差异性(而非双语性)会影响跨情景词汇学习
IF 1.5 3区 心理学 Q4 PHYSIOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-09-25 DOI: 10.1177/17470218241277805
Kimberly Crespo, Margarita Kaushanskaya

When learning new words, listeners must contend with multiple sources of ambiguity and variability. Research has revealed that learners can resolve referential ambiguity by tracking co-occurrence statistics between words and their referents across multiple exposures over time-a process termed cross-situational word learning (XSWL). However, the degree to which variability in the input, such as input from multiple speakers, and variability in learner experience, such as bilingual language experience, modulate XSWL remains unclear. In this study, we examined the effects of speaker variability in XSWL performance in monolingual adults and bilingual adults with a range of second language backgrounds and language acquisition histories. Results revealed above-chance word learning in both the single and the multiple-speaker conditions across language groups. An advantage for word learning was observed in the single-speaker condition, but the effects of bilingual language experience were null. This research adds to the limited body of work dedicated to extending theories of statistical learning to account for variations in both input and learner characteristics as well as their interactions.

在学习新词时,听者必须面对多种来源的模糊性和可变性。研究表明,学习者可以通过跟踪词与指代物在一段时间内多次接触时的共现统计来解决指代模糊问题--这一过程被称为跨情景词语学习(XSWL)。然而,输入的可变性(如来自多个说话者的输入)和学习者经验的可变性(如双语语言经验)对 XSWL 的调节程度仍不清楚。在本研究中,我们考察了说话者的可变性对具有不同第二语言背景和语言习得历史的单语成人和双语成人的跨情景单词学习成绩的影响。结果表明,在单个和多个说话者条件下,不同语言组的单词学习都高于机会。在单一说话者条件下,单词学习具有优势,但双语语言经验的影响为零。这项研究是对有限的统计学习理论进行扩展,以解释输入和学习者特征的变化及其相互作用的研究成果的补充。
{"title":"Speaker variability, but not bilingualism, influences cross-situational word learning.","authors":"Kimberly Crespo, Margarita Kaushanskaya","doi":"10.1177/17470218241277805","DOIUrl":"10.1177/17470218241277805","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>When learning new words, listeners must contend with multiple sources of ambiguity and variability. Research has revealed that learners can resolve referential ambiguity by tracking co-occurrence statistics between words and their referents across multiple exposures over time-a process termed cross-situational word learning (XSWL). However, the degree to which variability in the input, such as input from multiple speakers, and variability in learner experience, such as bilingual language experience, modulate XSWL remains unclear. In this study, we examined the effects of speaker variability in XSWL performance in monolingual adults and bilingual adults with a range of second language backgrounds and language acquisition histories. Results revealed above-chance word learning in both the single and the multiple-speaker conditions across language groups. An advantage for word learning was observed in the single-speaker condition, but the effects of bilingual language experience were null. This research adds to the limited body of work dedicated to extending theories of statistical learning to account for variations in both input and learner characteristics as well as their interactions.</p>","PeriodicalId":20869,"journal":{"name":"Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology","volume":" ","pages":"17470218241277805"},"PeriodicalIF":1.5,"publicationDate":"2024-09-25","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142018438","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Incremental structure building in the processing of ellipsis. EXPRESS:处理省略号时的增量结构构建。
IF 1.5 3区 心理学 Q4 PHYSIOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-09-24 DOI: 10.1177/17470218241280567
Hyosik Kim, Wesley J Orth, Masaya Yoshida

This article presents the results of two experiments conducted to examine how ellipsis sites are processed during the processing of backward sluicing, which is superficially similar to non-sluicing wh-filler-gap dependencies. Previous studies on long-distance wh-filler-gap dependencies established that the processing of these dependencies is sensitive to the syntactic structure of materials within the dependency: CP vs. NP. Results from two maze experiments show that backward sluicing processing is sensitive to the same structural factors, confirming that the same processing mechanism underlies both constructions. We suggest that an active search mechanism is operating at the core for these structures and with the interaction of the ellipsis-specific mechanism, e.g., a word-by-word copying mechanism, the parser builds antecedent structure within the ellipsis site incrementally during the processing of backward sluicing.

本文介绍了两个实验的结果,这两个实验旨在研究省略点在倒装依存加工过程中是如何被处理的,倒装依存表面上与非倒装wh-filler-gap依存相似。以前对长距离wh-filler-gap依存关系的研究表明,这些依存关系的处理对依存关系中材料的句法结构很敏感:CP vs NP(Gibson & Warren, 2004; Keine, 2020)。两个迷宫实验的结果表明,后向滑动加工对相同的结构因素也很敏感,这证实了两种结构的基础是相同的加工机制。我们认为,这些结构的核心是一种主动搜索机制,在省略号特异性机制(如逐字复制机制(Murphy 1985))的相互作用下,解析器在处理后向贯通时会在省略号位置内逐步建立前因结构。
{"title":"Incremental structure building in the processing of ellipsis.","authors":"Hyosik Kim, Wesley J Orth, Masaya Yoshida","doi":"10.1177/17470218241280567","DOIUrl":"10.1177/17470218241280567","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>This article presents the results of two experiments conducted to examine how ellipsis sites are processed during the processing of backward sluicing, which is superficially similar to non-sluicing wh-filler-gap dependencies. Previous studies on long-distance wh-filler-gap dependencies established that the processing of these dependencies is sensitive to the syntactic structure of materials within the dependency: CP vs. NP. Results from two maze experiments show that backward sluicing processing is sensitive to the same structural factors, confirming that the same processing mechanism underlies both constructions. We suggest that an active search mechanism is operating at the core for these structures and with the interaction of the ellipsis-specific mechanism, e.g., a word-by-word copying mechanism, the parser builds antecedent structure within the ellipsis site incrementally during the processing of backward sluicing.</p>","PeriodicalId":20869,"journal":{"name":"Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology","volume":" ","pages":"17470218241280567"},"PeriodicalIF":1.5,"publicationDate":"2024-09-24","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142018436","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Prior multisensory learning can facilitate auditory-only voice-identity and speech recognition in noise. 表达:先前的多感官学习可促进噪音中的听觉声音识别和语音识别。
IF 1.5 3区 心理学 Q4 PHYSIOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-09-20 DOI: 10.1177/17470218241278649
Corrina Maguinness, Sonja Schall, Brian Mathias, Martin Schoemann, Katharina von Kriegstein

Seeing the visual articulatory movements of a speaker, while hearing their voice, helps with understanding what is said. This multisensory enhancement is particularly evident in noisy listening conditions. Multisensory enhancement also occurs even in auditory-only conditions: auditory-only speech and voice-identity recognition are superior for speakers previously learned with their face, compared to control learning; an effect termed the "face-benefit." Whether the face-benefit can assist in maintaining robust perception in increasingly noisy listening conditions, similar to concurrent multisensory input, is unknown. Here, in two behavioural experiments, we examined this hypothesis. In each experiment, participants learned a series of speakers' voices together with their dynamic face or control image. Following learning, participants listened to auditory-only sentences spoken by the same speakers and recognised the content of the sentences (speech recognition, Experiment 1) or the voice-identity of the speaker (Experiment 2) in increasing levels of auditory noise. For speech recognition, we observed that 14 of 30 participants (47%) showed a face-benefit. 19 of 25 participants (76%) showed a face-benefit for voice-identity recognition. For those participants who demonstrated a face-benefit, the face-benefit increased with auditory noise levels. Taken together, the results support an audio-visual model of auditory communication and suggest that the brain can develop a flexible system in which learned facial characteristics are used to deal with varying auditory uncertainty.

在听到说话者声音的同时,看到说话者的视觉发音动作,有助于理解说话内容。在嘈杂的听力条件下,这种多感官增强效果尤为明显。即使在纯听力条件下,多感官增强也会发生:与对照学习相比,纯听力条件下的语音和声音识别对以前用面孔学习过的说话者更为有利;这种效应被称为 "面孔优势"。面孔优势是否能在日益嘈杂的听力条件下帮助维持稳健的感知,类似于同时进行的多感官输入,目前尚不清楚。在这里,我们通过两个行为实验对这一假设进行了检验。在每个实验中,参与者都学习了一系列发言人的声音以及他们的动态脸部或控制图像。学习结束后,被试聆听由相同说话者说出的纯听觉句子,并在听觉噪声水平不断增加的情况下识别句子的内容(语音识别,实验 1)或说话者的声音特征(实验 2)。在语音识别方面,我们发现有 14/30 名参与者(47%)表现出了脸部优势。而有 19/25 名参与者(76%)在语音识别中表现出了人脸优势。对于那些表现出人脸优势的参与者来说,人脸优势随着听觉噪音水平的增加而增加。综上所述,这些结果支持听觉交流的视听模型,并表明大脑可以开发出一种灵活的系统,利用学习到的面部特征来应对不同的听觉不确定性。
{"title":"Prior multisensory learning can facilitate auditory-only voice-identity and speech recognition in noise.","authors":"Corrina Maguinness, Sonja Schall, Brian Mathias, Martin Schoemann, Katharina von Kriegstein","doi":"10.1177/17470218241278649","DOIUrl":"10.1177/17470218241278649","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Seeing the visual articulatory movements of a speaker, while hearing their voice, helps with understanding what is said. This multisensory enhancement is particularly evident in noisy listening conditions. Multisensory enhancement also occurs even in auditory-only conditions: auditory-only speech and voice-identity recognition are superior for speakers previously learned with their face, compared to control learning; an effect termed the \"face-benefit.\" Whether the face-benefit can assist in maintaining robust perception in increasingly noisy listening conditions, similar to concurrent multisensory input, is unknown. Here, in two behavioural experiments, we examined this hypothesis. In each experiment, participants learned a series of speakers' voices together with their dynamic face or control image. Following learning, participants listened to auditory-only sentences spoken by the same speakers and recognised the content of the sentences (speech recognition, Experiment 1) or the voice-identity of the speaker (Experiment 2) in increasing levels of auditory noise. For speech recognition, we observed that 14 of 30 participants (47%) showed a face-benefit. 19 of 25 participants (76%) showed a face-benefit for voice-identity recognition. For those participants who demonstrated a face-benefit, the face-benefit increased with auditory noise levels. Taken together, the results support an audio-visual model of auditory communication and suggest that the brain can develop a flexible system in which learned facial characteristics are used to deal with varying auditory uncertainty.</p>","PeriodicalId":20869,"journal":{"name":"Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology","volume":" ","pages":"17470218241278649"},"PeriodicalIF":1.5,"publicationDate":"2024-09-20","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142009326","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Negative or positive left or right? The influence of attribute label position on IAT effects in picture-word IATs and word IATs. 快递:左还是右?属性标签位置对图画-文字 IAT 和文字 IAT 中 IAT 效果的影响。
IF 1.5 3区 心理学 Q4 PHYSIOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-09-20 DOI: 10.1177/17470218241275941
Lisa M Viegas, Christina Bermeitinger, Pamela Baess

The Implicit Association Test (IAT) is a widely used measure of implicit attitudes. Despite its application in various fields, the malleability of the IAT by different methodological factors has been shown frequently. In this article, we focus on two factors that potentially influence the IAT effect, but which have received either inconsistent or no support so far: the IAT version (i.e., picture-word IAT vs. word IAT) and the position of the attribute labels on the screen (i.e., the positive or negative label on the left side). In two experiments, we used the original flower-insect IAT to systematically analyse the effects of the position of attribute labels (i.e., the assignment of the positive or the negative attribute label to the left screen position) and the block order of compatible (e.g., flower and positive) and incompatible blocks (e.g., flower and negative) as between-subjects factors. Reliable IAT effects were observed for the picture-word IAT and the word IAT when calculating the IAT effect as a difference in the response times as well as when computing the recommended D Score as IAT outcome. Smaller IAT effects occurred in the picture-word IAT than in the word IAT, supporting existing literature. In addition, an effect of the position of the attribute labels on the screen was found in both experiments, resulting in larger IAT effects when the negative attribute label was positioned on the left. This effect also appeared when calculating the D Score. The study highlights the importance of methodical factors for the IAT outcome.

内隐联想测验(IAT)是一种广泛使用的内隐态度测量方法。尽管 IAT 被广泛应用于各个领域,但不同方法论因素对其影响的可塑性却屡见不鲜。在本文中,我们重点研究了两个可能影响 IAT 效果的因素,但这两个因素迄今为止尚未得到一致的支持或没有得到支持:IAT 版本(即图片-文字 IAT 与文字 IAT)和属性标签在屏幕上的位置(即左侧的正面或负面标签)。在两个实验中,我们使用原始的花-昆虫 IAT 系统分析了属性标签位置(即将正属性标签或负属性标签分配到屏幕左侧位置)以及相容块(如花和正)和不相容块顺序的影响、在计算作为反应时间差异的 IAT 效果以及计算作为 IAT 结果的推荐 D 分数时,观察到了图片-文字 IAT 和文字 IAT 的可靠 IAT 效果。与单词 IAT 相比,图片-单词 IAT 的 IAT 效果较小,这与现有文献相吻合。此外,两次实验中都发现了属性标签在屏幕上位置的影响,当负面属性标签位于左侧时,IAT 效果更大。在计算 D 分数时也出现了这种效应。这项研究强调了方法因素对 IAT 结果的重要性。
{"title":"Negative or positive left or right? The influence of attribute label position on IAT effects in picture-word IATs and word IATs.","authors":"Lisa M Viegas, Christina Bermeitinger, Pamela Baess","doi":"10.1177/17470218241275941","DOIUrl":"10.1177/17470218241275941","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The Implicit Association Test (IAT) is a widely used measure of implicit attitudes. Despite its application in various fields, the malleability of the IAT by different methodological factors has been shown frequently. In this article, we focus on two factors that potentially influence the IAT effect, but which have received either inconsistent or no support so far: the IAT version (i.e., picture-word IAT vs. word IAT) and the position of the attribute labels on the screen (i.e., the positive or negative label on the left side). In two experiments, we used the original flower-insect IAT to systematically analyse the effects of the position of attribute labels (i.e., the assignment of the positive or the negative attribute label to the left screen position) and the block order of compatible (e.g., flower and positive) and incompatible blocks (e.g., flower and negative) as between-subjects factors. Reliable IAT effects were observed for the picture-word IAT and the word IAT when calculating the IAT effect as a difference in the response times as well as when computing the recommended <i>D</i> Score as IAT outcome. Smaller IAT effects occurred in the picture-word IAT than in the word IAT, supporting existing literature. In addition, an effect of the position of the attribute labels on the screen was found in both experiments, resulting in larger IAT effects when the negative attribute label was positioned on the left. This effect also appeared when calculating the <i>D</i> Score. The study highlights the importance of methodical factors for the IAT outcome.</p>","PeriodicalId":20869,"journal":{"name":"Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology","volume":" ","pages":"17470218241275941"},"PeriodicalIF":1.5,"publicationDate":"2024-09-20","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141913759","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Visual letter similarity effects in Korean word recognition: The role of distinctive strokes. EXPRESS:韩语单词识别中的视觉字母相似性效应:独特笔画的作用
IF 1.5 3区 心理学 Q4 PHYSIOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-09-19 DOI: 10.1177/17470218241278600
Sungbong Bae, Chang H Lee, Hye K Pae

While the impact of visual letter similarity on word recognition in the Latin script has been extensively documented using masked priming techniques, research into non-Latin scripts such as Hangul remains limited. Hangul letters are systematically formed by adding one or two strokes to the base form, creating a pool of visually similar letters in the inventory. This study investigated the role of added distinctive strokes in word recognition by employing two experimental tasks: a lexical decision task (Experiment 1) and a same-different word matching task (Experiment 2). The results of Experiment 1 revealed a visual similarity effect only for primes without distinctive strokes, indicating an asymmetry in the priming effects. Conversely, Experiment 2 showed that visually similar primes facilitated target word processing regardless of the presence of the distinctive stroke, indicating no asymmetric priming effect. These findings suggest initial uncertainty of letter identity during Korean word processing and the processing of distinctive strokes in differentiating visually similar words.

虽然视觉字母相似性对拉丁字母单词识别的影响已经通过掩蔽引物技术得到了广泛的记录,但对韩文等非拉丁字母的研究仍然有限。韩文字母是通过在基本形式上添加一个或两个笔画而系统形成的,从而在库存中形成视觉相似性字母库。本研究通过两个实验任务:词性判断任务(实验 1)和同异词匹配任务(实验 2),研究了添加的独特笔画在单词识别中的作用。实验 1 的结果显示,视觉相似性效应仅适用于无明显笔画的预设,这表明引物效应不对称。相反,实验 2 显示,无论是否存在独特笔画,视觉相似的预设都会促进目标单词的处理,这表明不存在非对称的引物效应。这些研究结果表明,在韩语单词处理过程中,字母身份的初始不确定性以及在区分视觉相似单词时对独特笔画的处理。
{"title":"Visual letter similarity effects in Korean word recognition: The role of distinctive strokes.","authors":"Sungbong Bae, Chang H Lee, Hye K Pae","doi":"10.1177/17470218241278600","DOIUrl":"10.1177/17470218241278600","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>While the impact of visual letter similarity on word recognition in the Latin script has been extensively documented using masked priming techniques, research into non-Latin scripts such as Hangul remains limited. Hangul letters are systematically formed by adding one or two strokes to the base form, creating a pool of visually similar letters in the inventory. This study investigated the role of added distinctive strokes in word recognition by employing two experimental tasks: a lexical decision task (Experiment 1) and a same-different word matching task (Experiment 2). The results of Experiment 1 revealed a visual similarity effect only for primes without distinctive strokes, indicating an asymmetry in the priming effects. Conversely, Experiment 2 showed that visually similar primes facilitated target word processing regardless of the presence of the distinctive stroke, indicating no asymmetric priming effect. These findings suggest initial uncertainty of letter identity during Korean word processing and the processing of distinctive strokes in differentiating visually similar words.</p>","PeriodicalId":20869,"journal":{"name":"Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology","volume":" ","pages":"17470218241278600"},"PeriodicalIF":1.5,"publicationDate":"2024-09-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141988742","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
From task-general towards task-specific cognitive operations in a few minutes? Working memory performance as an adaptive process. 快递:几分钟内从一般任务到特定任务的认知操作?工作记忆表现是一个适应过程。
IF 1.5 3区 心理学 Q4 PHYSIOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-09-18 DOI: 10.1177/17470218241278272
Jussi Jylkkä, Zachary Stickley, Daniel Fellman, Otto Waris, Liisa Ritakallio, Todd D Little, Juha Salmi, Matti Laine

Measurement of cognitive functions is typically based on the implicit assumption that the mental architecture underlying cognitive task performance is constant throughout the task. In contrast, skill learning theory implies that cognitively demanding task performance is an adaptive process that progresses from initial heavy engagement of effortful and task-general metacognitive and executive control processes towards more automatic and task-specific performance. However, this hypothesis is rarely applied to the short time spans of traditional cognitive tasks such as working memory (WM) tasks. We utilised longitudinal structural equation models on two well-powered data sets to test the hypothesis that the initial stages of WM task performances load heavily on a task-general g-factor and then start to diverge towards factors specific to task structure. In line with the hypothesis, data from the first experiment (N = 296) were successfully fitted in a model with task-initial unity of the WM paradigm-specific latent factors, after which their intercorrelations started to diverge. The second experiment (N = 201) replicated this pattern except for one paradigm-specific latent factor. These preliminary results suggest that the processes underlying WM task performance tend to progress rapidly from more task-general towards task-specific, in line with the cognitive skill learning framework. Such task-internal dynamics has important implications for the measurement of complex cognitive functions.

认知功能的测量通常基于一个隐含的假设,即认知任务表现所依赖的心理结构在整个任务过程中是不变的。与此相反,技能学习理论则暗示,认知任务表现是一个适应过程,从最初的大量参与努力和任务一般的元认知和执行控制过程,到更自动和任务特定的表现。然而,这一假设很少被应用于时间跨度较短的传统认知任务,如工作记忆(WM)任务。我们利用两组具有较强能力的数据建立了纵向结构方程模型,以检验以下假设:工作记忆任务执行的最初阶段在很大程度上依赖于任务的一般 g 因子,然后开始向任务结构的特定因子分化。与这一假设相符的是,第一个实验(N = 296)的数据被成功地拟合到了一个具有任务初始统一性的 WM 范式特定潜因子模型中,之后它们之间的相互关系开始分化。第二次实验(N = 201)复制了这一模式,只有一个特定范式的潜因子除外。这些初步结果表明,工作记忆任务表现的基础过程倾向于从更多的任务一般性向任务特定性快速发展,这与认知技能学习框架是一致的。这种任务内部的动态变化对复杂认知功能的测量具有重要意义。
{"title":"From task-general towards task-specific cognitive operations in a few minutes? Working memory performance as an adaptive process.","authors":"Jussi Jylkkä, Zachary Stickley, Daniel Fellman, Otto Waris, Liisa Ritakallio, Todd D Little, Juha Salmi, Matti Laine","doi":"10.1177/17470218241278272","DOIUrl":"10.1177/17470218241278272","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Measurement of cognitive functions is typically based on the implicit assumption that the mental architecture underlying cognitive task performance is constant throughout the task. In contrast, skill learning theory implies that cognitively demanding task performance is an adaptive process that progresses from initial heavy engagement of effortful and task-general metacognitive and executive control processes towards more automatic and task-specific performance. However, this hypothesis is rarely applied to the short time spans of traditional cognitive tasks such as working memory (WM) tasks. We utilised longitudinal structural equation models on two well-powered data sets to test the hypothesis that the initial stages of WM task performances load heavily on a task-general g-factor and then start to diverge towards factors specific to task structure. In line with the hypothesis, data from the first experiment (<i>N</i> = 296) were successfully fitted in a model with task-initial unity of the WM paradigm-specific latent factors, after which their intercorrelations started to diverge. The second experiment (<i>N</i> = 201) replicated this pattern except for one paradigm-specific latent factor. These preliminary results suggest that the processes underlying WM task performance tend to progress rapidly from more task-general towards task-specific, in line with the cognitive skill learning framework. Such task-internal dynamics has important implications for the measurement of complex cognitive functions.</p>","PeriodicalId":20869,"journal":{"name":"Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology","volume":" ","pages":"17470218241278272"},"PeriodicalIF":1.5,"publicationDate":"2024-09-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142009325","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The cross-script cognate effect in spoken and written second language production: A study based on Chinese-English bilinguals. 表达:第二语言口语和书面语中的跨文字同义词效应:基于汉英双语者的研究。
IF 1.5 3区 心理学 Q4 PHYSIOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-09-18 DOI: 10.1177/17470218241279047
Guorui Zheng, Tingting Yang, Weihao Lin, Yueran Yang, Ruiming Wang

Previous studies using cognates with the same writing system have found cognate facilitation effect in the lexical processes of spoken and typewritten productions and cognate interference effect in the sub-lexical process of typewritten production. This study focused on cross-script cognates, Chinese-English, which have different writing systems, and explored cognate effects based on the input and output modalities by using a Chinese-English translation task. Experiment 1 was under visual input modality and investigated the cross-script cognate effect in all three output modalities: spoken, typewritten and handwritten. Results revealed a cognate facilitation effect in the lexical processes across all three output modalities. However, it showed a cognate facilitation effect rather than a cognate interference effect in the sub-lexical process of typewritten production. Experiment 2 was under auditory input modality and focused on exploring cross-script cognate effect on typewritten and handwritten modalities, finding a consistent result on cognate effects with Experiment 1. Both experiments showed higher accuracy for cognates, and there was no significant difference in cgnate effect between visual and auditory inputs. In summary, these findings indicated that the use of cross-script cognates could effectively mitigate cognate interference. While spoken, handwritten and typewritten production share lexical processes, differences emerge in sub-lexical processes, with spoken production being less influenced by orthography. Furthermore, combining the results of Experiments 1 and 2, typewritten production may lean towards the phonological route while handwritten production may favour the direct lexical-orthographic route in the sub-lexical processes.

以往使用书写系统相同的同源物进行的研究发现,同源物在口语和打字制作的词汇过程中具有促进效应,而在打字制作的次词汇过程中具有干扰效应。本研究以书写系统不同的汉英跨文字同源语为研究对象,通过汉英翻译任务探讨了基于输入和输出模式的同源语效应。实验 1 采用视觉输入模式,研究了口语、打字和手写三种输出模式下的跨文字认知效应。结果显示,在所有三种输出模式的词汇过程中都存在同义词促进效应。然而,在打字制作的次级词汇过程中,出现的是认知促进效应,而不是认知干扰效应。实验 2 是在听觉输入模式下进行的,重点是探索打字和手写模式下的跨文字认知效应,结果发现认知效应与实验 1 一致。两个实验都显示,同义词的准确率更高,而且视觉输入和听觉输入的同义词效应没有显著差异。总之,这些研究结果表明,使用跨文字同义词可以有效缓解同义词干扰。虽然口语、手写和打字都有共同的词汇过程,但在次词汇过程中出现了差异,口语表达受正字法的影响较小。此外,综合实验 1 和实验 2 的结果,打字制作可能更倾向于语音途径,而手写制作在次词汇过程中可能更倾向于直接的词汇-正字法途径。
{"title":"The cross-script cognate effect in spoken and written second language production: A study based on Chinese-English bilinguals.","authors":"Guorui Zheng, Tingting Yang, Weihao Lin, Yueran Yang, Ruiming Wang","doi":"10.1177/17470218241279047","DOIUrl":"10.1177/17470218241279047","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Previous studies using cognates with the same writing system have found cognate facilitation effect in the lexical processes of spoken and typewritten productions and cognate interference effect in the sub-lexical process of typewritten production. This study focused on cross-script cognates, Chinese-English, which have different writing systems, and explored cognate effects based on the input and output modalities by using a Chinese-English translation task. Experiment 1 was under visual input modality and investigated the cross-script cognate effect in all three output modalities: spoken, typewritten and handwritten. Results revealed a cognate facilitation effect in the lexical processes across all three output modalities. However, it showed a cognate facilitation effect rather than a cognate interference effect in the sub-lexical process of typewritten production. Experiment 2 was under auditory input modality and focused on exploring cross-script cognate effect on typewritten and handwritten modalities, finding a consistent result on cognate effects with Experiment 1. Both experiments showed higher accuracy for cognates, and there was no significant difference in cgnate effect between visual and auditory inputs. In summary, these findings indicated that the use of cross-script cognates could effectively mitigate cognate interference. While spoken, handwritten and typewritten production share lexical processes, differences emerge in sub-lexical processes, with spoken production being less influenced by orthography. Furthermore, combining the results of Experiments 1 and 2, typewritten production may lean towards the phonological route while handwritten production may favour the direct lexical-orthographic route in the sub-lexical processes.</p>","PeriodicalId":20869,"journal":{"name":"Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology","volume":" ","pages":"17470218241279047"},"PeriodicalIF":1.5,"publicationDate":"2024-09-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142009327","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The distinctive role of vowel harmony in visual word recognition: The case of Turkish. 表达:元音和谐在视觉词语识别中的独特作用:以土耳其语为例。
IF 1.5 3区 心理学 Q4 PHYSIOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-09-13 DOI: 10.1177/17470218241277683
Zeynep G Özkan, Berceste Özdemir, Pablo Gómez, Manuel Perea

Vowel harmony is a phenomenon in which the vowels in a word share some features (e.g., frontness vs. backness). It occurs in several families of languages (e.g., Turkic and Finno-Ugric languages) and serves as an effective segmenting cue in continuous speech and when reading compound words. The present study examined whether vowel harmony also plays a role in visual word recognition. We chose Turkish, a language with four front and four back vowels in which approximately 75% of words are harmonious. If vowel harmony contributes to the formation of coherent phonological codes during lexical access, harmonious words will reach a stable orthographic-phonological state more rapidly than disharmonious words. To test this hypothesis, in Experiment 1, we selected two types of monomorphemic Turkish words: harmonious (containing only front vowels or back vowels) and disharmonious (containing front and back vowels)-a parallel manipulation was applied to the pseudowords. Results showed faster lexical decisions for harmonious than disharmonious words, whereas vowel harmony did not affect pseudowords. In Experiment 2, where all words were harmonious, we found a small but reliable advantage for disharmonious over harmonious pseudowords. These findings suggest that vowel harmony helps the formation of stable phonological codes in Turkish words, but it does not play a key role in pseudoword rejection.

元音和谐是指一个词中的元音共享某些特征(如前元音与后元音)的现象。它出现在多个语系(如突厥语和芬兰-乌戈尔语)中,在连续语音和阅读复合词时可作为有效的分词线索。本研究考察了元音和谐是否也在视觉单词识别中发挥作用。我们选择了土耳其语,这是一种有四个前元音和四个后元音的语言,其中约 75% 的单词是和谐的。如果元音和谐有助于在词汇访问过程中形成连贯的语音编码,那么和谐的单词将比不和谐的单词更快达到稳定的正字法-语音状态。为了验证这一假设,在实验 1 中,我们选择了两类单韵母土耳其语单词:和谐的(只包含前元音或后元音)和不和谐的(包含前元音和后元音)--对假词进行了平行操作。结果显示,和谐词的词汇决策速度比不和谐词快,而元音和谐对伪词没有影响。在实验 2 中,所有单词都是和谐的,我们发现不和谐的假词比和谐的假词具有微小但可靠的优势。这些发现表明,元音和谐有助于在土耳其语单词中形成稳定的语音编码,但它在伪词排斥中并不起关键作用。
{"title":"The distinctive role of vowel harmony in visual word recognition: The case of Turkish.","authors":"Zeynep G Özkan, Berceste Özdemir, Pablo Gómez, Manuel Perea","doi":"10.1177/17470218241277683","DOIUrl":"10.1177/17470218241277683","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Vowel harmony is a phenomenon in which the vowels in a word share some features (e.g., frontness vs. backness). It occurs in several families of languages (e.g., Turkic and Finno-Ugric languages) and serves as an effective segmenting cue in continuous speech and when reading compound words. The present study examined whether vowel harmony also plays a role in visual word recognition. We chose Turkish, a language with four front and four back vowels in which approximately 75% of words are harmonious. If vowel harmony contributes to the formation of coherent phonological codes during lexical access, harmonious words will reach a stable orthographic-phonological state more rapidly than disharmonious words. To test this hypothesis, in Experiment 1, we selected two types of monomorphemic Turkish words: harmonious (containing only front vowels or back vowels) and disharmonious (containing front and back vowels)-a parallel manipulation was applied to the pseudowords. Results showed faster lexical decisions for harmonious than disharmonious words, whereas vowel harmony did not affect pseudowords. In Experiment 2, where all words were harmonious, we found a small but reliable advantage for disharmonious over harmonious pseudowords. These findings suggest that vowel harmony helps the formation of stable phonological codes in Turkish words, but it does not play a key role in pseudoword rejection.</p>","PeriodicalId":20869,"journal":{"name":"Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology","volume":" ","pages":"17470218241277683"},"PeriodicalIF":1.5,"publicationDate":"2024-09-13","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141976484","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The influence of familiarisation and item repetition on the name agreement effect in picture naming. 表达:熟悉和项目重复对图片命名中名称一致效应的影响。
IF 1.5 3区 心理学 Q4 PHYSIOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-09-12 DOI: 10.1177/17470218241274661
Ruth E Corps, Antje S Meyer

Name agreement (NA) refers to the degree to which speakers agree on a picture's name. A robust finding is that speakers are faster to name pictures with high agreement (HA) than those with low agreement (LA). This NA effect is thought to occur because LA pictures strongly activate several names, so speakers need time to select one. HA pictures, in contrast, strongly activate a single name, so there is no need to select one name out of several alternatives. Recent models of lexical access suggest that the structure of the mental lexicon changes with experience. Thus, speakers should consider a range of names when naming LA pictures, but the extent to which they consider each of these names should change with experience. We tested these hypotheses in two picture-naming experiments. In Experiment 1, participants were faster to name LA than HA pictures when they named each picture once. Importantly, they were faster to produce modal names (provided by most participants) than alternative names for LA pictures, consistent with the view that speakers activate multiple names for LA pictures. In Experiment 2, participants were familiarised with the modal name before the experiment and named each picture three times. Although there was still an NA effect when participants named the pictures the first time, it was reduced in comparison to Experiment 1 and was further reduced with each picture repetition. Thus, familiarisation and repetition reduced the NA effect but did not eliminate it, suggesting speakers activate a range of plausible names.

名称一致度(NA)指说话者对图片名称的一致程度。一个可靠的发现是,说话者在命名一致度高(HA)的图片时比命名一致度低(LA)的图片时更快。这种 NA 效应被认为是由于 LA 图片能强烈激活多个名称,因此说话者需要时间来选择一个名称。与此相反,HA 图片能强烈激活单个名称,因此无需从多个备选名称中选择一个。新近的词汇访问模型表明,心理词典的结构会随着经验的变化而变化。因此,在给 LA 图片命名时,说话者应该考虑一系列名称,但他们考虑每个名称的程度应该随着经验的变化而变化。我们在两个图片命名实验中测试了这些假设。在实验 1 中,当参与者对每幅图片命名一次时,他们对 LA 图片的命名速度要快于 HA 图片。重要的是,对于 LA 图片,他们更快地说出模式名称(由大多数参与者提供),而不是替代名称,这与说话者对 LA 图片激活多个名称的观点一致。在实验 2 中,参与者在实验前熟悉了模式名称,并对每幅图片命名三次。因此,熟悉和重复减少了 NA 效应,但并没有消除 NA 效应,这表明说话者激活了一系列似是而非的名称。
{"title":"The influence of familiarisation and item repetition on the name agreement effect in picture naming.","authors":"Ruth E Corps, Antje S Meyer","doi":"10.1177/17470218241274661","DOIUrl":"10.1177/17470218241274661","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Name agreement (NA) refers to the degree to which speakers agree on a picture's name. A robust finding is that speakers are faster to name pictures with high agreement (HA) than those with low agreement (LA). This NA effect is thought to occur because LA pictures strongly activate several names, so speakers need time to select one. HA pictures, in contrast, strongly activate a single name, so there is no need to select one name out of several alternatives. Recent models of lexical access suggest that the structure of the mental lexicon changes with experience. Thus, speakers should consider a range of names when naming LA pictures, but the extent to which they consider each of these names should change with experience. We tested these hypotheses in two picture-naming experiments. In Experiment 1, participants were faster to name LA than HA pictures when they named each picture once. Importantly, they were faster to produce modal names (provided by most participants) than alternative names for LA pictures, consistent with the view that speakers activate multiple names for LA pictures. In Experiment 2, participants were familiarised with the modal name before the experiment and named each picture three times. Although there was still an NA effect when participants named the pictures the first time, it was reduced in comparison to Experiment 1 and was further reduced with each picture repetition. Thus, familiarisation and repetition reduced the NA effect but did not eliminate it, suggesting speakers activate a range of plausible names.</p>","PeriodicalId":20869,"journal":{"name":"Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology","volume":" ","pages":"17470218241274661"},"PeriodicalIF":1.5,"publicationDate":"2024-09-12","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141913760","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1