首页 > 最新文献

Vision Research最新文献

英文 中文
Photic drive response in people with epilepsy: Exploring the interaction with background alpha rhythm
IF 1.5 4区 心理学 Q4 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2025-01-27 DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2025.108548
Eman N. Ali , Christian J. Lueck , Kate L. Martin , Angela Borbelj , Ted Maddess
Photic drive responses (PDRs) are used to explore cortical hyperexcitability. We quantified PDRs and interactions with the alpha rhythm in people with epilepsy (PwE). Fifteen PwE (mean age ± SD 47.3 ± 4.6 years; 8 males), and 15 control subjects (mean age 52.7 ± 4.6 years; 9 males) underwent EEG with modified intermittent photic stimulation (IPS). The modification allowed so-called alpha-band gain to be measured. None of the PwE had demonstrated photosensitivity. The modified IPS method alternated eyes-open and eyes–closed conditions with and without IPS. The alpha-band gain appeared as N-fold changes in PDR when IPS (or its harmonics) and the alpha-bands overlapped. An epileptic attack within 1 month of testing significantly increased alpha-band gain by 1.36×. Generalised epilepsy (but not focal epilepsy) significantly decreased alpha-band gain y 0.79×. Each decade of age beyond the mean age significantly increased alpha-band gain by 1.09×. Similar significant interactions were seen between alpha and the second harmonic of IPS driving frequencies that matched alpha frequencies, i.e. for recent attack and, generalized epilepsy. The interactions thus appeared to be occurring between cortical IPS outputs and the alpha generator. These changes were most evident at electrodes O1 and O2. Investigating alpha-band gain using modified IPS offers a way to quantify cortical hyperexcitability in epilepsy and other diseases. It also provides new information about alpha and so too predictive coding, which appears to be at least partly governed by alpha.
{"title":"Photic drive response in people with epilepsy: Exploring the interaction with background alpha rhythm","authors":"Eman N. Ali ,&nbsp;Christian J. Lueck ,&nbsp;Kate L. Martin ,&nbsp;Angela Borbelj ,&nbsp;Ted Maddess","doi":"10.1016/j.visres.2025.108548","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.visres.2025.108548","url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>Photic drive responses (PDRs) are used to explore cortical hyperexcitability. We quantified PDRs and interactions with the alpha rhythm in people with epilepsy (PwE). Fifteen PwE (mean age ± SD 47.3 ± 4.6 years; 8 males), and 15 control subjects (mean age 52.7 ± 4.6 years; 9 males) underwent EEG with modified intermittent photic stimulation (IPS). The modification allowed so-called <em>alpha-band gain</em> to be measured. None of the PwE had demonstrated photosensitivity. The modified IPS method alternated eyes-open and eyes–closed conditions with and without IPS. The alpha-band gain appeared as N-fold changes in PDR when IPS (or its harmonics) and the alpha-bands overlapped. An epileptic attack within 1 month of testing significantly increased alpha-band gain by 1.36×. Generalised epilepsy (but not focal epilepsy) significantly decreased alpha-band gain y 0.79×. Each decade of age beyond the mean age significantly increased alpha-band gain by 1.09×. Similar significant interactions were seen between alpha and the second harmonic of IPS driving frequencies that matched alpha frequencies, <em>i.e.</em> for recent attack and, generalized epilepsy. The interactions thus appeared to be occurring between cortical IPS outputs and the alpha generator. These changes were most evident at electrodes O1 and O2. Investigating alpha-band gain using modified IPS offers a way to quantify cortical hyperexcitability in epilepsy and other diseases. It also provides new information about alpha and so too predictive coding, which appears to be at least partly governed by alpha.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":23670,"journal":{"name":"Vision Research","volume":"228 ","pages":"Article 108548"},"PeriodicalIF":1.5,"publicationDate":"2025-01-27","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143060852","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Face age modulates face ensemble coding
IF 1.5 4区 心理学 Q4 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2025-01-25 DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2025.108549
Shenli Peng, Tianhui Liu, Yi Yang
Research has demonstrated that humans possess the remarkable ability to swiftly extract ensemble statistics, specifically the average identity, from sets of stimuli, such as facial crowds. This phenomenon is known as ensemble perception. Although previous studies have investigated how physiognomic features like gender and race influence face ensemble perception, the impact of face age on face ensemble coding performance remains a relatively unexplored area. Here, we demonstrated ensemble coding of multiple faces in terms of an average face was impacted by face age. In both Experiment 1 and 2, adult participants viewed sets of four faces that were of either own-age or other-age and then judged whether the subsequently presented probe face was present or not in the preceding set. The other-age faces were manipulated as older faces in Experiment 1 and baby faces in Experiment 2. The results suggested participants incorrectly endorsed a morphed set average to be the member of the set, pointing to face ensemble coding ability. Furthermore, the results of Experiment 1 revealed adult participants displayed an own-age superiority when other-age faces were manipulated as older faces, however, the results of Experiment 2 found when other-age faces were manipulated as baby faces, participants displayed stronger visual averaging tendency towards other-age faces rather own-age faces, showing a babyface effect. Together, the present research provided initial evidence that face ensemble coding performance was modulated by face age.
{"title":"Face age modulates face ensemble coding","authors":"Shenli Peng,&nbsp;Tianhui Liu,&nbsp;Yi Yang","doi":"10.1016/j.visres.2025.108549","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.visres.2025.108549","url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>Research has demonstrated that humans possess the remarkable ability to swiftly extract ensemble statistics, specifically the average identity, from sets of stimuli, such as facial crowds. This phenomenon is known as ensemble perception. Although previous studies have investigated how physiognomic features like gender and race influence face ensemble perception, the impact of face age on face ensemble coding performance remains a relatively unexplored area. Here, we demonstrated ensemble coding of multiple faces in terms of an average face was impacted by face age. In both Experiment 1 and 2, adult participants viewed sets of four faces that were of either own-age or other-age and then judged whether the subsequently presented probe face was present or not in the preceding set. The other-age faces were manipulated as older faces in Experiment 1 and baby faces in Experiment 2. The results suggested participants incorrectly endorsed a morphed set average to be the member of the set, pointing to face ensemble coding ability. Furthermore, the results of Experiment 1 revealed adult participants displayed an own-age superiority when other-age faces were manipulated as older faces, however, the results of Experiment 2 found when other-age faces were manipulated as baby faces, participants displayed stronger visual averaging tendency towards other-age faces rather own-age faces, showing a babyface effect. Together, the present research provided initial evidence that face ensemble coding performance was modulated by face age.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":23670,"journal":{"name":"Vision Research","volume":"228 ","pages":"Article 108549"},"PeriodicalIF":1.5,"publicationDate":"2025-01-25","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143047746","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Visual comfort models based on coloured text and neutral background combinations 基于彩色文本和中性背景组合的视觉舒适模型
IF 1.5 4区 心理学 Q4 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-11-29 DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2024.108524
Zhenzhen Li, Yu Liu, Yuechen Zhu, Ming Ronnier Luo
Reading on mobile phones can cause visual discomfort, negatively affecting visual health. Most studies have focused on neutral text-background combinations, with limited validation for coloured text-background combinations. This study investigates the impact of coloured text on neutral backgrounds and the colour difference between text and background on visual comfort during digital reading. A psychophysical experiment was conducted, where 230 images of coloured text on neutral backgrounds were evaluated by 20 participants using a 6-point scale for visual comfort. Results showed that reading coloured text on a black background generally provided higher comfort compared to a white background. Additionally, visual comfort decreased as the text colour approached that of the background. The effect of text hue on comfort was not significant. Furthermore, several visual comfort models for mobile displays were developed and compared. The VC1-LAB model is based on Bern’s attributes, while the VC2-LAB model focuses on the lightness of text and background. The VC3-LAB model includes both lightness and chroma attributes. Comparisons revealed that VC3-LAB outperformed the others in predicting visual comfort, highlighting the importance of lightness and chroma in improving predictive accuracy. Therefore, the VC3-LAB model is useful for evaluating the visual comfort of coloured text on neutral backgrounds.
在手机上阅读会引起视觉不适,对视觉健康产生负面影响。大多数研究都集中在中性文本-背景组合上,对有色文本-背景组合的验证有限。本研究探讨了彩色文本在中性背景上的影响,以及文本和背景之间的颜色差异对数字阅读视觉舒适度的影响。研究人员进行了一项心理物理实验,其中20名参与者使用6分制的视觉舒适度来评估230张中性背景上的彩色文字图像。结果表明,在黑色背景上阅读彩色文本通常比在白色背景上阅读更舒适。此外,当文字颜色接近背景颜色时,视觉舒适度下降。文字色调对舒适度的影响不显著。此外,还开发了几种移动显示器的视觉舒适模型并进行了比较。VC1-LAB模型基于Bern的属性,而VC2-LAB模型侧重于文本和背景的亮度。VC3-LAB模型包括亮度和色度属性。比较显示,VC3-LAB在预测视觉舒适度方面优于其他方法,突出了亮度和色度在提高预测准确性方面的重要性。因此,VC3-LAB模型对于评估中性背景上彩色文本的视觉舒适度是有用的。
{"title":"Visual comfort models based on coloured text and neutral background combinations","authors":"Zhenzhen Li,&nbsp;Yu Liu,&nbsp;Yuechen Zhu,&nbsp;Ming Ronnier Luo","doi":"10.1016/j.visres.2024.108524","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.visres.2024.108524","url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>Reading on mobile phones can cause visual discomfort, negatively affecting visual health. Most studies have focused on neutral text-background combinations, with limited validation for coloured text-background combinations. This study investigates the impact of coloured text on neutral backgrounds and the colour difference between text and background on visual comfort during digital reading. A psychophysical experiment was conducted, where 230 images of coloured text on neutral backgrounds were evaluated by 20 participants using a 6-point scale for visual comfort. Results showed that reading coloured text on a black background generally provided higher comfort compared to a white background. Additionally, visual comfort decreased as the text colour approached that of the background. The effect of text hue on comfort was not significant. Furthermore, several visual comfort models for mobile displays were developed and compared. The VC<sub>1-LAB</sub> model is based on Bern’s attributes, while the VC<sub>2-LAB</sub> model focuses on the lightness of text and background. The VC<sub>3-LAB</sub> model includes both lightness and chroma attributes. Comparisons revealed that VC<sub>3-LAB</sub> outperformed the others in predicting visual comfort, highlighting the importance of lightness and chroma in improving predictive accuracy. Therefore, the VC<sub>3-LAB</sub> model is useful for evaluating the visual comfort of coloured text on neutral backgrounds.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":23670,"journal":{"name":"Vision Research","volume":"227 ","pages":"Article 108524"},"PeriodicalIF":1.5,"publicationDate":"2024-11-29","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142747178","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Trans-saccadic integration for object recognition peters out with pre-saccadic object eccentricity as target-directed saccades become more saliency-driven 当目标导向的扫视变得更加显著性驱动时,跨扫视的目标识别整合随着扫视前的目标偏心逐渐消失
IF 1.5 4区 心理学 Q4 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-11-29 DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2024.108500
Junhao Liang, Li Zhaoping
Bringing objects from peripheral locations to fovea via saccades facilitates their recognition. Human observers integrate pre- and post-saccadic information for recognition. This integration has only been investigated using instructed saccades to prescribed locations. Typically, the target has a fixed pre-saccadic location in an uncluttered scene and is viewed by a pre-determined post-saccadic duration. Consequently, whether trans-saccadic integration is limited or absent when the pre-saccadic target eccentricity is too large in cluttered scenes in unknown. Our study revealed this limit during visual exploration, when observers decided themselves when and to where to make their saccades. We asked thirty observers (400 trials each) to find and report as quickly as possible a target amongst 404 non-targets in an image spanning 57.3°×33.8° in visual angle. We measured the target’s pre-saccadic eccentricity e, the duration Tpre of the fixation before the saccade, and the post-saccadic foveal viewing duration Tpost. This Tpost increased with e before starting to saturate around eccentricity ep=10°20°. Meanwhile, Tpre increased much more slowly with e and started decreasing before ep. These observations imply the following at sufficiently large pre-saccadic eccentricities: the trans-saccadic integration ceases, target recognition relies exclusively on post-saccadic foveal vision, decision to saccade to the target relies exclusively on target saliency rather than identification. These implications should be applicable to general behavior, although ep should depend on object and scene properties. They are consistent with the Central-peripheral Dichotomy that central and peripheral vision are specialized for seeing and looking, respectively.
通过扫视将物体从外围位置带到中央凹,有助于对其进行识别。人类观察者将跳眼前和跳眼后的信息整合起来进行识别。这种整合只能通过指示扫视到指定位置来研究。通常,目标在一个整洁的场景中有一个固定的跳前位置,并在预定的跳后持续时间内被观察。因此,在未知的杂乱场景中,当跳前目标偏心过大时,跨跳积分是否受到限制或不存在。我们的研究揭示了视觉探索过程中的这种限制,即观察者自己决定何时何地进行扫视。我们要求30名观察者(每人400次试验)在一张视角为57.3°×33.8°的图像中尽快从404个非目标中找到并报告一个目标。我们测量了被试的跳前偏心率e、跳前注视持续时间Tpre和跳后中央凹注视持续时间Tpost。在离心率ep=10°- 20°附近开始饱和之前,这个post随着e的增加而增加。同时,Tpre随e的增加缓慢,在ep之前开始下降。这些观察结果表明,在足够大的跳眼前偏心时:跨跳眼整合停止,目标识别完全依赖于跳眼后中央凹视觉,跳眼到目标的决定完全依赖于目标显著性而不是识别。这些含义应该适用于一般行为,尽管ep应该取决于对象和场景属性。它们与中央-外周视觉二分法一致,即中央视觉和外周视觉分别专门用于看和看。
{"title":"Trans-saccadic integration for object recognition peters out with pre-saccadic object eccentricity as target-directed saccades become more saliency-driven","authors":"Junhao Liang,&nbsp;Li Zhaoping","doi":"10.1016/j.visres.2024.108500","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.visres.2024.108500","url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>Bringing objects from peripheral locations to fovea via saccades facilitates their recognition. Human observers integrate pre- and post-saccadic information for recognition. This integration has only been investigated using instructed saccades to prescribed locations. Typically, the target has a fixed pre-saccadic location in an uncluttered scene and is viewed by a pre-determined post-saccadic duration. Consequently, whether trans-saccadic integration is limited or absent when the pre-saccadic target eccentricity is too large in cluttered scenes in unknown. Our study revealed this limit during visual exploration, when observers decided themselves when and to where to make their saccades. We asked thirty observers (400 trials each) to find and report as quickly as possible a target amongst 404 non-targets in an image spanning <span><math><mrow><mn>57</mn><mo>.</mo><mn>3</mn><mo>°</mo><mo>×</mo><mn>33</mn><mo>.</mo><mn>8</mn><mo>°</mo></mrow></math></span> in visual angle. We measured the target’s pre-saccadic eccentricity <span><math><mi>e</mi></math></span>, the duration <span><math><msub><mrow><mi>T</mi></mrow><mrow><mi>p</mi><mi>r</mi><mi>e</mi></mrow></msub></math></span> of the fixation before the saccade, and the post-saccadic foveal viewing duration <span><math><msub><mrow><mi>T</mi></mrow><mrow><mi>p</mi><mi>o</mi><mi>s</mi><mi>t</mi></mrow></msub></math></span>. This <span><math><msub><mrow><mi>T</mi></mrow><mrow><mi>p</mi><mi>o</mi><mi>s</mi><mi>t</mi></mrow></msub></math></span> increased with <span><math><mi>e</mi></math></span> before starting to saturate around eccentricity <span><math><mrow><msub><mrow><mi>e</mi></mrow><mrow><mi>p</mi></mrow></msub><mo>=</mo><mn>10</mn><mo>°</mo><mo>−</mo><mn>20</mn><mo>°</mo></mrow></math></span>. Meanwhile, <span><math><msub><mrow><mi>T</mi></mrow><mrow><mi>p</mi><mi>r</mi><mi>e</mi></mrow></msub></math></span> increased much more slowly with <span><math><mi>e</mi></math></span> and started decreasing before <span><math><msub><mrow><mi>e</mi></mrow><mrow><mi>p</mi></mrow></msub></math></span>. These observations imply the following at sufficiently large pre-saccadic eccentricities: the trans-saccadic integration ceases, target recognition relies exclusively on post-saccadic foveal vision, decision to saccade to the target relies exclusively on target saliency rather than identification. These implications should be applicable to general behavior, although <span><math><msub><mrow><mi>e</mi></mrow><mrow><mi>p</mi></mrow></msub></math></span> should depend on object and scene properties. They are consistent with the Central-peripheral Dichotomy that central and peripheral vision are specialized for seeing and looking, respectively.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":23670,"journal":{"name":"Vision Research","volume":"226 ","pages":"Article 108500"},"PeriodicalIF":1.5,"publicationDate":"2024-11-29","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142746138","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Applying the efficient coding principle to understand encoding of multisensory and multimodality sensory signals 应用高效编码原理理解多感官和多模态感官信号的编码
IF 1.5 4区 心理学 Q4 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-11-26 DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2024.108489
Li Zhaoping
Sensory neurons often encode multisensory or multimodal signals. For example, many medial superior temporal (MST) neurons are tuned to heading direction of self-motion based on visual (optic flow) signals and vestibular signals. Middle temporal (MT) cortical neurons are tuned to object depth from signals of two visual modalities: motion parallax and binocular disparity. A MST neuron’s preferred heading directions from different senses can be congruent (matched) or opposite from each other. Similarly, the preferred depths of a MT neuron from the two modalities are congruent in some neurons and opposite in other neurons. While the congruent tuning appears natural for cue integration, the functions of the opposite tuning have been puzzling. This paper explains these tunings from the efficient coding principle that sensory encoding extracts as much sensory information as possible while minimizing neural cost. It extends the previous applications of this principle to understand neural receptive fields in retina and the primary visual cortex, particularly multimodal encoding of cone signals or binocular signals. Congruent and opposite sensory signals that excite the congruent and opposite neurons, respectively, are the decorrelated sensory components that provide a general purpose, efficient, representation of sensory inputs before task specific object segmentation and recognition. It can be extended to encoding signals from more than two sensory sources, e.g., from three cone types. This framework also predicts a wider tuning width for the opposite than congruent neurons, neurons that are neither congruent nor opposite, and how neural receptive fields adapt to statistical changes of sensory environments.
感觉神经元经常编码多感觉或多模态信号。例如,许多内侧上颞叶(MST)神经元根据视觉(视流)信号和前庭信号对自身运动的方向进行调谐。中颞叶(MT)皮层神经元根据两种视觉模式的信号(运动视差和双眼视差)对物体深度进行调谐。中颞叶神经元从不同感官获得的首选方向可能是一致的(匹配),也可能是相反的。类似地,MT 神经元在两种模式下的偏好深度在某些神经元中是一致的,而在另一些神经元中则相反。对于线索整合来说,一致的调谐似乎很自然,但相反调谐的功能却令人费解。本文从高效编码原理来解释这些调谐,即感觉编码在提取尽可能多的感觉信息的同时将神经成本降至最低。本文扩展了这一原理的应用,以理解视网膜和初级视觉皮层的神经感受野,尤其是锥体信号或双眼信号的多模态编码。分别激发同向和反向神经元的同向和反向感觉信号是装饰相关的感觉成分,它们在特定任务的物体分割和识别之前提供了通用、高效的感觉输入表征。它可以扩展到对来自两个以上感觉源的信号进行编码,例如来自三种锥体类型的信号。这一框架还预测了相反神经元比同源神经元、既非同源也非相反神经元更宽的调谐宽度,以及神经感受野如何适应感觉环境的统计变化。
{"title":"Applying the efficient coding principle to understand encoding of multisensory and multimodality sensory signals","authors":"Li Zhaoping","doi":"10.1016/j.visres.2024.108489","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.visres.2024.108489","url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>Sensory neurons often encode multisensory or multimodal signals. For example, many medial superior temporal (MST) neurons are tuned to heading direction of self-motion based on visual (optic flow) signals and vestibular signals. Middle temporal (MT) cortical neurons are tuned to object depth from signals of two visual modalities: motion parallax and binocular disparity. A MST neuron’s preferred heading directions from different senses can be congruent (matched) or opposite from each other. Similarly, the preferred depths of a MT neuron from the two modalities are congruent in some neurons and opposite in other neurons. While the congruent tuning appears natural for cue integration, the functions of the opposite tuning have been puzzling. This paper explains these tunings from the efficient coding principle that sensory encoding extracts as much sensory information as possible while minimizing neural cost. It extends the previous applications of this principle to understand neural receptive fields in retina and the primary visual cortex, particularly multimodal encoding of cone signals or binocular signals. Congruent and opposite sensory signals that excite the congruent and opposite neurons, respectively, are the decorrelated sensory components that provide a general purpose, efficient, representation of sensory inputs before task specific object segmentation and recognition. It can be extended to encoding signals from more than two sensory sources, e.g., from three cone types. This framework also predicts a wider tuning width for the opposite than congruent neurons, neurons that are neither congruent nor opposite, and how neural receptive fields adapt to statistical changes of sensory environments.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":23670,"journal":{"name":"Vision Research","volume":"226 ","pages":"Article 108489"},"PeriodicalIF":1.5,"publicationDate":"2024-11-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142699188","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Tracking cortical entrainment to stages of optic-flow processing 跟踪大脑皮层对视流处理阶段的诱导
IF 1.5 4区 心理学 Q4 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-11-26 DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2024.108523
Cai Wingfield , Andrew Soltan , Ian Nimmo-Smith , William D. Marslen-Wilson , Andrew Thwaites
In human visual processing, information from the visual field passes through numerous transformations before perceptual attributes such as motion are derived. Determining the sequence of transforms involved in the perception of visual motion has been an active field since the 1940s. One plausible family of models are the spatiotemporal energy models, based on computations of motion energy computed from the spatiotemporal features the visual field. One of the most venerated is that of Heeger (1988), which hypotheses that motion is estimated by matching the predicted spatiotemporal energy in frequency space. In this study, we investigate the plausibility of Heeger’s model by testing for evidence of cortical entrainment to its components. Entrainment of cortical activity to these components was estimated using measurements of electro- and magnetoencephalographic (EMEG) activity, recorded while healthy subjects watched videos of dots moving left and right across their visual field. We find entrainment to several components of Heeger’s model bilaterally in occipital lobe regions, including representations of motion energy at a latency of 80 ms, overall velocity at 95 ms, and acceleration at 130 ms. We find little evidence of entrainment to displacement. We contrast Heeger’s biologically inspired model with alternative baseline models, finding that Heeger’s model provides a closer fit to the observed data. These results help shed light on the processes through which perception of motion arises in the visual processing stream.
在人类的视觉处理过程中,来自视野的信息要经过无数次变换,才能得出运动等感知属性。自 20 世纪 40 年代以来,确定视觉运动感知所涉及的变换序列一直是一个活跃的领域。时空能量模型是其中一个可信的模型系列,它基于根据视场的时空特征计算出的运动能量。其中最受推崇的是 Heeger(1988 年)的模型,其假设运动是通过匹配频率空间中预测的时空能量来估计的。在本研究中,我们通过测试皮层与该模型各组成部分的相干性来研究 Heeger 模型的合理性。通过测量健康受试者在观看点在视野中左右移动的视频时记录的脑电和脑磁图(EMEG)活动,我们估算了大脑皮层活动与这些成分的夹带。我们发现,在枕叶区域的双侧,Heeger 模型的几个组成部分都存在内联现象,其中包括潜伏期为 80 毫秒的运动能量表征、95 毫秒的整体速度表征和 130 毫秒的加速度表征。我们几乎没有发现与位移相关的证据。我们将 Heeger 的生物启发模型与其他基线模型进行了对比,发现 Heeger 的模型更符合观察到的数据。这些结果有助于揭示运动感知在视觉处理流中产生的过程。
{"title":"Tracking cortical entrainment to stages of optic-flow processing","authors":"Cai Wingfield ,&nbsp;Andrew Soltan ,&nbsp;Ian Nimmo-Smith ,&nbsp;William D. Marslen-Wilson ,&nbsp;Andrew Thwaites","doi":"10.1016/j.visres.2024.108523","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.visres.2024.108523","url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>In human visual processing, information from the visual field passes through numerous transformations before perceptual attributes such as motion are derived. Determining the sequence of transforms involved in the perception of visual motion has been an active field since the 1940s. One plausible family of models are the spatiotemporal energy models, based on computations of motion energy computed from the spatiotemporal features the visual field. One of the most venerated is that of <span><span>Heeger (1988)</span></span>, which hypotheses that motion is estimated by matching the predicted spatiotemporal energy in frequency space. In this study, we investigate the plausibility of Heeger’s model by testing for evidence of cortical entrainment to its components. Entrainment of cortical activity to these components was estimated using measurements of electro- and magnetoencephalographic (EMEG) activity, recorded while healthy subjects watched videos of dots moving left and right across their visual field. We find entrainment to several components of Heeger’s model bilaterally in occipital lobe regions, including representations of motion energy at a latency of 80 ms, overall velocity at 95 ms, and acceleration at 130 ms. We find little evidence of entrainment to displacement. We contrast Heeger’s biologically inspired model with alternative baseline models, finding that Heeger’s model provides a closer fit to the observed data. These results help shed light on the processes through which perception of motion arises in the visual processing stream.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":23670,"journal":{"name":"Vision Research","volume":"226 ","pages":"Article 108523"},"PeriodicalIF":1.5,"publicationDate":"2024-11-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142699187","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Extracellular vesicles as emerging players in glaucoma: Mechanisms, biomarkers, and therapeutic targets 细胞外囊泡是青光眼的新兴参与者:机制、生物标志物和治疗目标
IF 1.5 4区 心理学 Q4 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-11-23 DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2024.108522
Maral Namdari , Fiona S. McDonnell
In recent years, extracellular vesicles (EVs) have attracted significant scientific interest due to their widespread distribution, their potential as disease biomarkers, and their promising applications in therapy. Encapsulated by lipid bilayers these nanovesicles include small extracellular vesicles (sEV) (30–150 nm), microvesicles (100–1000 nm), and apoptotic bodies (100–5000 nm) and are essential for cellular communication, immune responses, biomolecular transport, and physiological regulation. As they reflect the condition and functionality of their originating cells, EVs play critical roles in numerous physiological processes and diseases. Therefore, EVs offer valuable opportunities for uncovering disease mechanisms, enhancing drug delivery systems, and identifying novel biomarkers. In the context of glaucoma, a leading cause of irreversible blindness, the specific roles of EVs are still largely unexplored.
This review examines the emerging role of EVs in the pathogenesis of glaucoma, with a focus on their potential as diagnostic biomarkers and therapeutic agents. Through a thorough analysis of current literature, we summarize key advancements in EV research and identify areas where further investigation is needed to fully understand their function in glaucoma.
近年来,细胞外囊泡(EVs)因其广泛分布、作为疾病生物标志物的潜力以及在治疗中的应用前景而引起了科学界的极大兴趣。这些被脂质双分子层包裹的纳米囊泡包括小细胞外囊泡(sEV)(30-150 nm)、微囊泡(100-1000 nm)和凋亡体(100-5000 nm),它们对细胞通讯、免疫反应、生物分子运输和生理调节至关重要。EVs 反映了起源细胞的状况和功能,因此在许多生理过程和疾病中发挥着关键作用。因此,EVs 为揭示疾病机制、增强给药系统和鉴定新型生物标记物提供了宝贵的机会。这篇综述探讨了 EVs 在青光眼发病机制中新出现的作用,重点关注 EVs 作为诊断生物标志物和治疗药物的潜力。通过对当前文献的全面分析,我们总结了EV研究的主要进展,并确定了需要进一步研究的领域,以全面了解它们在青光眼中的功能。
{"title":"Extracellular vesicles as emerging players in glaucoma: Mechanisms, biomarkers, and therapeutic targets","authors":"Maral Namdari ,&nbsp;Fiona S. McDonnell","doi":"10.1016/j.visres.2024.108522","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.visres.2024.108522","url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>In recent years, extracellular vesicles (EVs) have attracted significant scientific interest due to their widespread distribution, their potential as disease biomarkers, and their promising applications in therapy. Encapsulated by lipid bilayers these nanovesicles include small extracellular vesicles (sEV) (30–150 nm), microvesicles (100–1000 nm), and apoptotic bodies (100–5000 nm) and are essential for cellular communication, immune responses, biomolecular transport, and physiological regulation. As they reflect the condition and functionality of their originating cells, EVs play critical roles in numerous physiological processes and diseases. Therefore, EVs offer valuable opportunities for uncovering disease mechanisms, enhancing drug delivery systems, and identifying novel biomarkers. In the context of glaucoma, a leading cause of irreversible blindness, the specific roles of EVs are still largely unexplored.</div><div>This review examines the emerging role of EVs in the pathogenesis of glaucoma, with a focus on their potential as diagnostic biomarkers and therapeutic agents. Through a thorough analysis of current literature, we summarize key advancements in EV research and identify areas where further investigation is needed to fully understand their function in glaucoma.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":23670,"journal":{"name":"Vision Research","volume":"226 ","pages":"Article 108522"},"PeriodicalIF":1.5,"publicationDate":"2024-11-23","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142699189","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Visual discomfort and chromatic flickers 视觉不适和色频闪烁
IF 1.5 4区 心理学 Q4 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-11-20 DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2024.108520
Sanae Yoshimoto , Hinako Iizuka , Tatsuto Takeuchi
Flickering patterns that shift in chromaticity can be uncomfortable and may trigger epileptic seizures, though the underlying factors are not fully understood. In the spatial domain, chromatic contrast in images is a potential predictor of visual discomfort, with higher contrast generally leading to increased discomfort. This study investigated whether chromatic contrast between two flickering colors in a uniform field influences discomfort. Participants rated their subjective discomfort for various flickering color combinations defined by the CIE L*a*b* uniform color space. Overall, discomfort increased with both chromatic and brightness contrasts. Additionally, flickers containing highly saturated red generally caused greater discomfort compared to those without red, an effect not observed with low saturation. Our findings suggest that visual discomfort induced by time-varying chromatic patterns is partly influenced by chromatic contrast over time. Furthermore, unlike the spatial domain, discomfort in the temporal domain may be specifically associated with the hue of red.
色度变化的闪烁图案会让人感到不适,并可能诱发癫痫发作,但其根本原因尚不完全清楚。在空间领域,图像中的色度对比是视觉不适的潜在预测因素,对比度越高,不适感通常越强。本研究调查了均匀视野中两种闪烁颜色之间的色度对比是否会影响不适感。受试者对 CIE L*a*b* 统一色彩空间定义的各种闪烁色彩组合进行了主观不适感评分。总体而言,不适感会随着色度和亮度对比的增加而增加。此外,与不含红色的闪烁相比,含有高饱和度红色的闪烁通常会引起更大的不适感,而低饱和度的闪烁则不会产生这种效果。我们的研究结果表明,随时间变化的色度模式所引起的视觉不适在一定程度上受到随时间变化的色度对比的影响。此外,与空间域不同,时间域的不适感可能与红色的色调有特殊关联。
{"title":"Visual discomfort and chromatic flickers","authors":"Sanae Yoshimoto ,&nbsp;Hinako Iizuka ,&nbsp;Tatsuto Takeuchi","doi":"10.1016/j.visres.2024.108520","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.visres.2024.108520","url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>Flickering patterns that shift in chromaticity can be uncomfortable and may trigger epileptic seizures, though the underlying factors are not fully understood. In the spatial domain, chromatic contrast in images is a potential predictor of visual discomfort, with higher contrast generally leading to increased discomfort. This study investigated whether chromatic contrast between two flickering colors in a uniform field influences discomfort. Participants rated their subjective discomfort for various flickering color combinations defined by the CIE <em>L*a*b*</em> uniform color space. Overall, discomfort increased with both chromatic and brightness contrasts. Additionally, flickers containing highly saturated red generally caused greater discomfort compared to those without red, an effect not observed with low saturation. Our findings suggest that visual discomfort induced by time-varying chromatic patterns is partly influenced by chromatic contrast over time. Furthermore, unlike the spatial domain, discomfort in the temporal domain may be specifically associated with the hue of red.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":23670,"journal":{"name":"Vision Research","volume":"226 ","pages":"Article 108520"},"PeriodicalIF":1.5,"publicationDate":"2024-11-20","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142688434","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Effects of cue location and object orientation on object-based attention 线索位置和物体方位对基于物体的注意力的影响
IF 1.5 4区 心理学 Q4 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-11-19 DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2024.108521
Hao Lou , Karin S. Pilz , Monicque M. Lorist
Spatial cues have previously been found to facilitate information processing not only at cued locations but also within cued objects, so-called object-based attention. We used different variants of the classic two-rectangle paradigm to investigate the interaction of cue location and object orientation on object-based attentional effects. First, we re-analyzed data from a prior study using the classical two-rectangle paradigm. We expected faster attentional shifts along the horizontal compared to the vertical meridian. Results confirmed that cue location and rectangle orientation interactively influence object-based attention, with horizontal objects combined with upper left visual field cues eliciting faster responses than other conditions. In Experiment 2, we removed object contours to examine the benefits of shifting attention based purely on cue location. The results showed that these differences remained, indicating that attentional shifts are not solely guided by object contours. In Experiment 3, we added a third possible target location to the original two-rectangle experiment to examine whether attentional shifts followed a predictable pattern across the stimulus display. Despite faster responses to cued targets, no consistent and organized visual search pattern was observed when participants searched for targets at invalidly cued locations. Our findings suggest that object-based effects are influenced by both cue location and the orientation of attentional shifts. Shifts from left to right in the upper visual field consistently demonstrated significant benefits, whereas the benefits of vertical shifts were less consistent across experiments.
以前曾有研究发现,空间线索不仅能促进提示位置的信息处理,还能促进提示对象内部的信息处理,即所谓的基于对象的注意。我们使用了经典双角范式的不同变体来研究线索位置和对象方位对基于对象的注意效应的交互作用。首先,我们重新分析了之前使用经典双矩形范式研究的数据。我们预计,与垂直经线相比,沿水平经线的注意力转移速度更快。结果证实,提示位置和矩形方向会交互影响基于物体的注意力,与其他条件相比,水平物体与左上方视野提示相结合会引起更快的反应。在实验 2 中,我们去掉了物体轮廓,以研究纯粹根据提示位置转移注意力的益处。结果显示,这些差异依然存在,表明注意力的转移并不完全受物体轮廓线的引导。在实验 3 中,我们在原来的双矩形实验中增加了第三个可能的目标位置,以考察注意力转移是否遵循整个刺激显示的可预测模式。尽管被试对提示目标的反应更快,但当被试在无效提示位置搜索目标时,并没有观察到一致且有组织的视觉搜索模式。我们的研究结果表明,基于目标的效应同时受到提示位置和注意力转移方向的影响。在上视野中从左到右的转移始终表现出显著的益处,而垂直转移的益处在不同实验中的表现则不太一致。
{"title":"Effects of cue location and object orientation on object-based attention","authors":"Hao Lou ,&nbsp;Karin S. Pilz ,&nbsp;Monicque M. Lorist","doi":"10.1016/j.visres.2024.108521","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.visres.2024.108521","url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>Spatial cues have previously been found to facilitate information processing not only at cued locations but also within cued objects, so-called object-based attention. We used different variants of the classic two-rectangle paradigm to investigate the interaction of cue location and object orientation on object-based attentional effects. First, we re-analyzed data from a prior study using the classical two-rectangle paradigm. We expected faster attentional shifts along the horizontal compared to the vertical meridian. Results confirmed that cue location and rectangle orientation interactively influence object-based attention, with horizontal objects combined with upper left visual field cues eliciting faster responses than other conditions. In Experiment 2, we removed object contours to examine the benefits of shifting attention based purely on cue location. The results showed that these differences remained, indicating that attentional shifts are not solely guided by object contours. In Experiment 3, we added a third possible target location to the original two-rectangle experiment to examine whether attentional shifts followed a predictable pattern across the stimulus display. Despite faster responses to cued targets, no consistent and organized visual search pattern was observed when participants searched for targets at invalidly cued locations. Our findings suggest that object-based effects are influenced by both cue location and the orientation of attentional shifts. Shifts from left to right in the upper visual field consistently demonstrated significant benefits, whereas the benefits of vertical shifts were less consistent across experiments.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":23670,"journal":{"name":"Vision Research","volume":"226 ","pages":"Article 108521"},"PeriodicalIF":1.5,"publicationDate":"2024-11-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142682869","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Intravitreal AAV2 gene delivery to feline retinal ganglion cells 将 AAV2 基因植入猫视网膜神经节细胞。
IF 1.5 4区 心理学 Q4 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-11-16 DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2024.108519
Kazuya Oikawa , J.Seth Eaton , Julie A. Kiland , Odalys Torné , Virginia Mathu , Robert W. Nickells , Gillian J. McLellan
Effective strategies for the neuroprotection and preservation of retinal ganglion cells (RGCs) remain elusive in the management of glaucoma. A spontaneous genetic model of glaucoma has been identified in cats and extensively characterized as a viable translational model, with eye size and anatomy similar to humans. In this study we sought to establish initial proof of concept for gene delivery to feline RGCs via intravitreal injection of AAV2 in normal cats. Pre-retinal, posterior vitreal injection of AAV2/2-CMV-GFP, was performed overlying the area centralis in 5 adult cats. Immunosuppressive oral prednisolone was administered perioperatively and gradually tapered over 6-10wks post-injection. Ophthalmic examination was performed pre- and post-injection. The GFP reporter expression and morphological effects of viral transduction on the retina were monitored in vivo using confocal scanning laser ophthalmoscopy (cSLO) and optical coherence tomography (OCT), respectively (Spectralis OCT-HRA, Heidelberg), at 1-2wk intervals over 6-10wks. Full-field electroretinograms (ERG) and visual evoked potentials (VEP) were recorded at baseline and post-injection. Retinas were examined by histology and immunolabeling for the RGC marker RBPMS and Müller cell and astrocyte marker SOX9, and GFP expression was examined in the retina, optic nerve (ON), optic tract and lateral geniculate nucleus (LGN). GFP+ retinal cells and RGC axons were visualized by cSLO at 1–2 weeks post-injection. No retinal morphological changes were observed by OCT in vivo but 3/5 eyes exhibited mild retinal inflammation on histology. Retinal and ON function were preserved in injected eyes compared to baseline and untreated eyes. GFP expression was predominantly identified in RBPMS+ RGC cells as well as SOX9+ Müller cells. GFP fluorescence was observed throughout RGC nerve fiber tract in the central visual pathway. Peak transduction in RGCs (up to ∼ 20 %) was observed in the regions with high GFP expression, but < 1 % of RGCs expressed GFP across the whole retina. Our data provide proof of concept that pre-retinal injection of AAV2/2 may represent a feasible platform for gene delivery to feline RGCs in vivo but highlight a need for further refinement to improve RGC transduction efficiency and control low-grade retinal inflammation.
在治疗青光眼的过程中,神经保护和保留视网膜神经节细胞(RGC)的有效策略仍然难以捉摸。在猫身上发现了一种自发的青光眼遗传模型,并将其广泛表征为一种可行的转化模型,其眼睛大小和解剖结构与人类相似。在这项研究中,我们试图在正常猫体内通过玻璃体内注射 AAV2 将基因传递到猫的 RGCs,并对这一概念进行初步验证。我们在 5 只成年猫的中央区上方进行了 AAV2/2-CMV-GFP 视网膜前、后玻璃体内注射。围手术期口服泼尼松龙进行免疫抑制,并在注射后 6-10 周内逐渐减量。注射前后均进行了眼科检查。在6-10周内,每隔1-2周分别使用共焦扫描激光眼底镜(cSLO)和光学相干断层扫描(OCT)(Spectralis OCT-HRA,海德堡)监测GFP报告表达和病毒转导对视网膜的形态学影响。在基线和注射后记录全场视网膜电图(ERG)和视觉诱发电位(VEP)。通过组织学和免疫标记RGC标记物RBPMS及Müller细胞和星形胶质细胞标记物SOX9检查视网膜,并检查视网膜、视神经(ON)、视束和外侧膝状核(LGN)中的GFP表达。注射后1-2周,通过cSLO观察GFP+视网膜细胞和RGC轴突。活体 OCT 未观察到视网膜形态学变化,但有 3/5 的眼睛在组织学上表现出轻微的视网膜炎症。与基线和未处理的眼睛相比,注射眼的视网膜和视网膜功能都得到了保留。GFP 主要在 RBPMS+ RGC 细胞和 SOX9+ Müller 细胞中表达。在中央视觉通路的整个 RGC 神经纤维束中都能观察到 GFP 荧光。在 GFP 高表达区域观察到 RGC 的峰值转导(高达 ∼ 20 %),但在 GFP 低表达区域观察到 RGC 的峰值转导(高达 ∼ 20 %)。
{"title":"Intravitreal AAV2 gene delivery to feline retinal ganglion cells","authors":"Kazuya Oikawa ,&nbsp;J.Seth Eaton ,&nbsp;Julie A. Kiland ,&nbsp;Odalys Torné ,&nbsp;Virginia Mathu ,&nbsp;Robert W. Nickells ,&nbsp;Gillian J. McLellan","doi":"10.1016/j.visres.2024.108519","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.visres.2024.108519","url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>Effective strategies for the neuroprotection and preservation of retinal ganglion cells (RGCs) remain elusive in the management of glaucoma. A spontaneous genetic model of glaucoma has been identified in cats and extensively characterized as a viable translational model, with eye size and anatomy similar to humans. In this study we sought to establish initial proof of concept for gene delivery to feline RGCs via intravitreal injection of AAV2 in normal cats<em>.</em> Pre-retinal, posterior vitreal injection of AAV2/2-CMV-GFP, was performed overlying the area centralis in 5 adult cats. Immunosuppressive oral prednisolone was administered perioperatively and gradually tapered over 6-10wks post-injection. Ophthalmic examination was performed pre- and post-injection. The GFP reporter expression and morphological effects of viral transduction on the retina were monitored <em>in vivo</em> using confocal scanning laser ophthalmoscopy (cSLO) and optical coherence tomography (OCT), respectively (Spectralis OCT-HRA, Heidelberg), at 1-2wk intervals over 6-10wks. Full-field electroretinograms (ERG) and visual evoked potentials (VEP) were recorded at baseline and post-injection. Retinas were examined by histology and immunolabeling for the RGC marker RBPMS and Müller cell and astrocyte marker SOX9, and GFP expression was examined in the retina, optic nerve (ON), optic tract and lateral geniculate nucleus (LGN). GFP<sup>+</sup> retinal cells and RGC axons were visualized by cSLO at 1–2 weeks post-injection. No retinal morphological changes were observed by OCT <em>in vivo</em> but 3/5 eyes exhibited mild retinal inflammation on histology. Retinal and ON function were preserved in injected eyes compared to baseline and untreated eyes. GFP expression was predominantly identified in RBPMS<sup>+</sup> RGC cells as well as SOX9<sup>+</sup> Müller cells. GFP fluorescence was observed throughout RGC nerve fiber tract in the central visual pathway. Peak transduction in RGCs (up to ∼ 20 %) was observed in the regions with high GFP expression, but &lt; 1 % of RGCs expressed GFP across the whole retina. Our data provide proof of concept that pre-retinal injection of AAV2/2 may represent a feasible platform for gene delivery to feline RGCs <em>in vivo</em> but highlight a need for further refinement to improve RGC transduction efficiency and control low-grade retinal inflammation.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":23670,"journal":{"name":"Vision Research","volume":"226 ","pages":"Article 108519"},"PeriodicalIF":1.5,"publicationDate":"2024-11-16","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142645033","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Vision Research
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1