首页 > 最新文献

Vision Research最新文献

英文 中文
Individual differences in direction-selective motion adaptation revealed by change-detection performance 通过变化检测性能揭示方向选择性运动适应的个体差异。
IF 1.5 4区 心理学 Q4 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-10-02 DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2024.108490
Kristina Zeljic, Joshua A. Solomon, Michael J. Morgan
The motion aftereffect (MAE) and motion adaptation in general are usually considered to be universal phenomena. However, in a preliminary study using a bias-free measure of the MAE we found some individuals who showed at best a weak effect of adaptation. These same individuals also performed poorly in a “change detection“ test of motion adaptation based on visual search, leading to the conjecture that there is a bimodality in the population with respect to motion adaptation. The present study tested this possibility by screening 102 participants on two versions of the change-detection task while also considering potential confounding factors including eye movements, practice-based improvements, and deficits in visual search ability. The 5 strongest and the 5 weakest change detectors were selected for further testing of motion detection and contrast detection after adaptation. Data showed an inverse association between change-detection ability and performance in the motion-detection task. We extend previous findings by also showing i) the weakest change detectors exhibit less direction selectivity in their contrast thresholds after adapting to drifting gratings and ii) the ability to detect change in motion direction correlates with the ability to detect change in spatial orientation. Group differences between the strongest and weakest change detectors cannot be attributed to a lack of practice, nor can they be explained by poor fixation ability. Our results suggest genuine individual differences in the degree to which adaptation is specific to stimulus orientation and direction of motion.
运动后遗效应(MAE)和运动适应通常被认为是一种普遍现象。然而,在一项使用无偏差的 MAE 测量方法进行的初步研究中,我们发现有些人最多表现出微弱的适应效应。这些人在基于视觉搜索的运动适应性 "变化检测 "测试中也表现不佳,因此我们推测人群中存在运动适应性双峰现象。本研究通过对 102 名参与者进行两个版本的变化检测任务筛选,同时考虑了潜在的混杂因素,包括眼球运动、基于练习的改进和视觉搜索能力的缺陷,从而检验了这种可能性。研究人员选出了变化检测能力最强的 5 人和最弱的 5 人,进一步测试他们在适应后的运动检测能力和对比度检测能力。数据显示,变化检测能力与运动检测任务中的表现呈反比。我们扩展了之前的研究结果,还发现 i) 最弱的变化检测器在适应漂移光栅后,其对比度阈值的方向选择性较低;ii) 检测运动方向变化的能力与检测空间方位变化的能力相关。最强和最弱变化检测器之间的群体差异不能归因于缺乏练习,也不能用固定能力差来解释。我们的研究结果表明,个体对刺激方位和运动方向的适应程度确实存在差异。
{"title":"Individual differences in direction-selective motion adaptation revealed by change-detection performance","authors":"Kristina Zeljic,&nbsp;Joshua A. Solomon,&nbsp;Michael J. Morgan","doi":"10.1016/j.visres.2024.108490","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.visres.2024.108490","url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>The motion aftereffect (MAE) and motion adaptation in general are usually considered to be universal phenomena. However, in a preliminary study using a bias-free measure of the MAE we found some individuals who showed at best a weak effect of adaptation. These same individuals also performed poorly in a “change detection“ test of motion adaptation based on visual search, leading to the conjecture that there is a bimodality in the population with respect to motion adaptation. The present study tested this possibility by screening 102 participants on two versions of the change-detection task while also considering potential confounding factors including eye movements, practice-based improvements, and deficits in visual search ability. The 5 strongest and the 5 weakest change detectors were selected for further testing of motion detection and contrast detection after adaptation. Data showed an inverse association between change-detection ability and performance in the motion-detection task. We extend previous findings by also showing <em>i</em>) the weakest change detectors exhibit less direction selectivity in their contrast thresholds after adapting to drifting gratings and <em>ii</em>) the ability to detect change in motion direction correlates with the ability to detect change in spatial orientation. Group differences between the strongest and weakest change detectors cannot be attributed to a lack of practice, nor can they be explained by poor fixation ability. Our results suggest genuine individual differences in the degree to which adaptation is specific to stimulus orientation and direction of motion.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":23670,"journal":{"name":"Vision Research","volume":"225 ","pages":"Article 108490"},"PeriodicalIF":1.5,"publicationDate":"2024-10-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142373071","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Time course and neural locus of the Flashed Face Distortion Effect 闪烁脸部失真效应的时间过程和神经定位
IF 1.5 4区 心理学 Q4 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-09-30 DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2024.108492
Yi Gao , Kamilla N. Miller , Michael A. Webster , Michael A. Crognale , Fang Jiang
Viewing a rapid sequence of face images shown in the periphery can lead to large caricature-like distortions in the perceived images, a phenomenon known as the Flashed Face Distortion Effect (FFDE). The mechanisms underlying FFDE are poorly understood. Here we examined the timing and sites of the adaptation processes giving rise to the FFDE. To investigate the effects of presentation rate, we maintained consistent trial lengths while assessing how variations in the temporal frequencies of face presentation influenced the magnitude of face distortion and the averaging of facial expressions. Over a wide range of temporal frequencies (1.2–60 Hz) tested, we observed a decrease in FFDE strength as the presentation rate increased. To probe the neural sites of FFDE, we varied whether successive faces were presented to the same or different eyes using a dichoptic display. Distortion effects were comparable for monocular, binocular, and interocular conditions, yet much larger than a control condition where faces were presented with a temporal interval between successive images, suggesting a cortical locus for FFDE.
观看外围显示的一连串快速人脸图像会导致感知图像出现类似漫画的大幅扭曲,这种现象被称为 "闪光人脸扭曲效应"(FFDE)。人们对 FFDE 的内在机制知之甚少。在这里,我们研究了产生 FFDE 的适应过程的时间和部位。为了研究呈现率的影响,我们保持了一致的试验长度,同时评估了人脸呈现的时间频率变化如何影响人脸失真的程度和面部表情的平均化。在测试的广泛时间频率(1.2-60 Hz)范围内,我们观察到随着呈现率的增加,FFDE 的强度有所下降。为了探究 FFDE 的神经部位,我们使用二向色显示屏来改变连续的人脸是呈现给同一只眼睛还是不同的眼睛。在单眼、双眼和双眼间条件下,失真效应相当,但比在连续图像之间有时间间隔的情况下呈现人脸的对照条件下的失真效应要大得多,这表明 FFDE 存在于大脑皮层。
{"title":"Time course and neural locus of the Flashed Face Distortion Effect","authors":"Yi Gao ,&nbsp;Kamilla N. Miller ,&nbsp;Michael A. Webster ,&nbsp;Michael A. Crognale ,&nbsp;Fang Jiang","doi":"10.1016/j.visres.2024.108492","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.visres.2024.108492","url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>Viewing a rapid sequence of face images shown in the periphery can lead to large caricature-like distortions in the perceived images, a phenomenon known as the Flashed Face Distortion Effect (FFDE). The mechanisms underlying FFDE are poorly understood. Here we examined the timing and sites of the adaptation processes giving rise to the FFDE. To investigate the effects of presentation rate, we maintained consistent trial lengths while assessing how variations in the temporal frequencies of face presentation influenced the magnitude of face distortion and the averaging of facial expressions. Over a wide range of temporal frequencies (1.2–60 Hz) tested, we observed a decrease in FFDE strength as the presentation rate increased. To probe the neural sites of FFDE, we varied whether successive faces were presented to the same or different eyes using a dichoptic display. Distortion effects were comparable for monocular, binocular, and interocular conditions, yet much larger than a control condition where faces were presented with a temporal interval between successive images, suggesting a cortical locus for FFDE.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":23670,"journal":{"name":"Vision Research","volume":"224 ","pages":"Article 108492"},"PeriodicalIF":1.5,"publicationDate":"2024-09-30","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142354997","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
No matter what you do, travel is travel in visual foraging 无论你做什么,旅行就是视觉觅食旅行
IF 1.5 4区 心理学 Q4 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-09-27 DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2024.108491
Injae Hong , Grace Yan , Jeremy M. Wolfe
In visual foraging, foragers collect multiple items from a series of visual displays (or “patches”). When the goal is to maximize the total or the rate of collection of target items, foragers must decide when to leave a depleted patch given that “traveling” from one patch to another incurs a temporal cost. In three experiments, we investigated whether the interposition of a secondary task during travel between patches in visual foraging altered patch-leaving behavior. Over the course of 10- or 30-minute experiments, participants foraged in simulated “berry patches” and traveled to the next patch at will. While they traveled, they either actively performed a secondary task or simply observed passing visual stimuli. Travel time was varied across conditions. The addition of a secondary task, regardless of its relevance to visual foraging, to traveling, or to both, did not impact patch-leaving times in the primary visual foraging task. In Experiment 1 and more weakly in Experiment 2, the patch-leaving decision was based on how long the travel took as predicted by the Marginal Value Theorem (MVT). In Experiment 3, however, patch-leaving did not depend on travel time. Participants ‘overharvested’ in a manner that suggests that they may have adopted rules different from those of MVT. Across all three experiments, patch-leaving did not depend on the nature of the travel.
在视觉觅食中,觅食者从一系列视觉显示(或 "斑块")中收集多种物品。当目标是最大限度地提高目标物品的总收集量或收集率时,觅食者必须决定何时离开一个耗尽的斑块,因为从一个斑块 "旅行 "到另一个斑块需要时间成本。在三项实验中,我们研究了在视觉觅食的斑块间旅行过程中插入一项次要任务是否会改变离开斑块的行为。在 10 或 30 分钟的实验过程中,参与者在模拟的 "浆果斑块 "中觅食,并随意前往下一个斑块。在行进过程中,他们要么积极执行第二任务,要么只是观察经过的视觉刺激。不同条件下的旅行时间各不相同。无论次要任务与视觉觅食、旅行或两者是否相关,增加次要任务都不会影响主要视觉觅食任务中离开补丁的时间。在实验 1 和实验 2 中,根据边际值定理(MVT)的预测,离开补丁的决定是基于行进所需的时间。然而,在实验 3 中,斑块的离开并不取决于行进时间。参与者 "过度收获 "的方式表明,他们可能采用了与边际价值定理不同的规则。在所有三个实验中,斑块的离开并不取决于旅行的性质。
{"title":"No matter what you do, travel is travel in visual foraging","authors":"Injae Hong ,&nbsp;Grace Yan ,&nbsp;Jeremy M. Wolfe","doi":"10.1016/j.visres.2024.108491","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.visres.2024.108491","url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>In visual foraging, foragers collect multiple items from a series of visual displays (or “patches”). When the goal is to maximize the total or the rate of collection of target items, foragers must decide when to leave a depleted patch given that “traveling” from one patch to another incurs a temporal cost. In three experiments, we investigated whether the interposition of a secondary task during travel between patches in visual foraging altered patch-leaving behavior. Over the course of 10- or 30-minute experiments, participants foraged in simulated “berry patches” and traveled to the next patch at will. While they traveled, they either actively performed a secondary task or simply observed passing visual stimuli. Travel time was varied across conditions. The addition of a secondary task, regardless of its relevance to visual foraging, to traveling, or to both, did not impact patch-leaving times in the primary visual foraging task. In Experiment 1 and more weakly in Experiment 2, the patch-leaving decision was based on how long the travel took as predicted by the Marginal Value Theorem (MVT). In Experiment 3, however, patch-leaving did not depend on travel time. Participants ‘overharvested’ in a manner that suggests that they may have adopted rules different from those of MVT. Across all three experiments, patch-leaving did not depend on the nature of the travel.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":23670,"journal":{"name":"Vision Research","volume":"224 ","pages":"Article 108491"},"PeriodicalIF":1.5,"publicationDate":"2024-09-27","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142326683","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Dynamics of the perceptive field size in human adults 人类成年人感知场大小的动态变化
IF 1.5 4区 心理学 Q4 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-09-20 DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2024.108488
Marzouk Yassin, Maria Lev, Uri Polat

The receptive field (RF) is the fundamental processing unit of human vision; both masking and crowding depend on its size. The RF has a psychophysical corresponding term, the perceptive field (PF); whereas the RF is measured physiologically, the PF is measured psychophysically (a perceptual response). We investigated how spatial (lateral interactions), temporal (the stimulus presentation time), and the procedure affect the PF size for both monocular and binocular viewing. The stimuli consisted of a central vertically oriented Gabor target and high-contrast Gabor flankers positioned in two configurations (orthogonal or collinear) with target-flanker separations of either 2 or 3 wavelengths (λ). We used two main methods to control the monocular and binocular vision: mono-optic glasses vs. stereo glasses. The presentation order was either mixed or non-mixed for the presentation time and the eye condition. We estimated the PF size for both monocular and binocular viewing at 4 different presentation times (40, 80,120, and 200 ms) with different orders of presentation in each experiment (mono-optic glasses vs. stereo glasses, utilizing the lateral masking paradigm). In each experiment we explored one variable: how changing one parameter would affect the PF size in both monocular and binocular viewing (the temporal duration, the testing order of conditions, and the spatial distance) while keeping the others constant. We found that both the monocular and binocular PF size were dynamic and were significantly affected by the presentation order, leading to reduced lateral suppression under the collinear 2λ condition. Hence, both the monocular and binocular PF size depended on the sequence of the stimulus presentation time and the testing order of the conditions. Furthermore, we found that the binocular PF size was significantly larger than the monocular PF size.

感受野(RF)是人类视觉的基本处理单元;遮挡和拥挤都取决于感受野的大小。感受野(RF)有一个心理物理对应项,即感受野(PF);感受野是生理测量的,而感受野是心理物理测量的(一种知觉反应)。我们研究了单眼和双眼观看时,空间(横向相互作用)、时间(刺激物呈现时间)和程序对 PF 大小的影响。刺激物包括一个垂直方向的中心 Gabor 目标和高对比度的 Gabor 侧翼,侧翼有两种配置(正交或平行),目标与侧翼之间的距离为 2 或 3 个波长(λ)。我们使用两种主要方法来控制单眼和双眼视觉:单光眼镜和立体眼镜。在呈现时间和眼睛条件方面,呈现顺序为混合或非混合。我们在 4 个不同的呈现时间(40、80、120 和 200 毫秒)和不同的呈现顺序(单光眼镜与立体眼镜,利用侧向遮蔽范例)下估算了单眼和双眼观看的 PF 大小。在每个实验中,我们都探索了一个变量:在其他参数保持不变的情况下,改变一个参数(时间长度、测试条件顺序和空间距离)会如何影响单眼和双眼观看时的 PF 大小。我们发现,单眼和双眼的PF大小都是动态的,并受到呈现顺序的显著影响,从而导致在共线2λ条件下的横向抑制减少。因此,单眼和双眼的PF大小都取决于刺激的呈现时间顺序和条件的测试顺序。此外,我们还发现双目PF大小明显大于单目PF大小。
{"title":"Dynamics of the perceptive field size in human adults","authors":"Marzouk Yassin,&nbsp;Maria Lev,&nbsp;Uri Polat","doi":"10.1016/j.visres.2024.108488","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.visres.2024.108488","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>The receptive field (RF) is the fundamental processing unit of human vision; both masking and crowding depend on its size. The RF has a psychophysical corresponding term, the perceptive field (PF); whereas the RF is measured physiologically, the PF is measured psychophysically (a perceptual response). We investigated how spatial (lateral interactions), temporal (the stimulus presentation time), and the procedure affect the PF size for both monocular and binocular viewing. The stimuli consisted of a central vertically oriented Gabor target and high-contrast Gabor flankers positioned in two configurations (orthogonal or collinear) with target-flanker separations of either 2 or 3 wavelengths (λ). We used two main methods to control the monocular and binocular vision: mono-optic glasses vs. stereo glasses. The presentation order was either mixed or non-mixed for the presentation time and the eye condition. We estimated the PF size for both monocular and binocular viewing at 4 different presentation times (40, 80,120, and 200 ms) with different orders of presentation in each experiment (mono-optic glasses vs. stereo glasses, utilizing the lateral masking paradigm). In each experiment we explored one variable: how changing one parameter would affect the PF size in both monocular and binocular viewing (the temporal duration, the testing order of conditions, and the spatial distance) while keeping the others constant. We found that both the monocular and binocular PF size were dynamic and were significantly affected by the presentation order, leading to reduced lateral suppression under the collinear 2λ condition. Hence, both the monocular and binocular PF size depended on the sequence of the stimulus presentation time and the testing order of the conditions. Furthermore, we found that the binocular PF size was significantly larger than the monocular PF size.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":23670,"journal":{"name":"Vision Research","volume":"224 ","pages":"Article 108488"},"PeriodicalIF":1.5,"publicationDate":"2024-09-20","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142271168","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Resting trabecular meshwork cells experience constitutive cation influx 静止的小梁网状细胞经历构成性阳离子流入
IF 1.5 4区 心理学 Q4 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-09-19 DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2024.108487
Oleg Yarishkin , Monika Lakk , Christopher N. Rudzitis , Jordan E. Searle , Denisa Kirdajova , David Križaj

A quintessential sentinel of cell health, the membrane potential in nonexcitable cells integrates biochemical and biomechanical inputs, determines the driving force for ionic currents activated by input signals and plays critical functions in cellular differentiation, signaling, and pathology. The identity and properties of ion channels that subserve the resting potential in trabecular meshwork (TM) cells is poorly understood, which impairs our understanding of intraocular pressure regulation in healthy and diseased eyes. Here, we identified a powerful cationic conductance that subserves the TM resting potential. It disappears following Na+ removal or substitution with choline or NMDG+, is insensitive to TTX, verapamil, phenamil methanesulfonate, amiloride and GsMTx4, is substituted by Li+ and Cs+, and inhibited by Gd3+ and Ruthenium Red. Constitutive cation influx is thus not mediated by voltage-operated Na+, Ca2+, epithelial Na+ (ENaC) channels, Piezo channels or Na+/H+ exchange but may involve TRP-like channels. Transcriptional analysis detected expression of many TRP genes, with the transcriptome pool dominated by TRPC1 followed by expression of TRPV1, TRPC3, TRPV4 and TRPC5. Pyr3 and Pico1,4,5 did not affect the standing current whereas SKF96365 promoted rather than suppressed, Na+ influx. SEA-0400 induced a modest hyperpolarization, indicating residual contribution from Na+/Ca2+ exchange. The resting membrane potential in human TM cells is thus maintained by a constitutive monovalent cation leak current with properties not unlike those of TRP channels. This conductance is likely to influence conventional outflow by setting the homeostatic steady-state and by regulating the magnitude of pressure-induced currents in normotensive and hypertensive eyes.

作为细胞健康的典型哨兵,非可兴奋细胞的膜电位整合了生化和生物力学输入,决定了输入信号激活离子电流的驱动力,并在细胞分化、信号传导和病理过程中发挥着关键作用。我们对小梁网眼(TM)细胞中支持静息电位的离子通道的特性和属性知之甚少,这影响了我们对健康和患病眼睛眼压调节的理解。在这里,我们发现了一种支持小梁网静息电位的强大阳离子电导。它在 Na+ 移除或被胆碱或 NMDG+ 替代后消失,对 TTX、维拉帕米、甲磺酸苯海拉明、阿米洛利和 GsMTx4 不敏感,被 Li+ 和 Cs+ 替代,并被 Gd3+ 和钌红抑制。因此,组成性阳离子流入不是由电压操作的 Na+、Ca2+、上皮 Na+(ENaC)通道、Piezo 通道或 Na+/H+交换通道介导,而是可能涉及 TRP 样通道。转录分析检测到许多 TRP 基因的表达,转录组以 TRPC1 为主,其次是 TRPV1、TRPC3、TRPV4 和 TRPC5。Pyr3 和 Pico1,4,5 不影响驻留电流,而 SKF96365 则促进而不是抑制 Na+ 流入。SEA-0400 可诱导适度的超极化,这表明 Na+/Ca2+ 交换仍在发挥作用。因此,人类 TM 细胞的静息膜电位是由组成型单价阳离子泄漏电流维持的,其特性与 TRP 通道并无二致。在正常血压和高血压眼球中,这种传导很可能通过设定稳态和调节压力诱导电流的大小来影响常规外流。
{"title":"Resting trabecular meshwork cells experience constitutive cation influx","authors":"Oleg Yarishkin ,&nbsp;Monika Lakk ,&nbsp;Christopher N. Rudzitis ,&nbsp;Jordan E. Searle ,&nbsp;Denisa Kirdajova ,&nbsp;David Križaj","doi":"10.1016/j.visres.2024.108487","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.visres.2024.108487","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>A quintessential sentinel of cell health, the membrane potential in nonexcitable cells integrates biochemical and biomechanical inputs, determines the driving force for ionic currents activated by input signals and plays critical functions in cellular differentiation, signaling, and pathology. The identity and properties of ion channels that subserve the resting potential in trabecular meshwork (TM) cells is poorly understood, which impairs our understanding of intraocular pressure regulation in healthy and diseased eyes. Here, we identified a powerful cationic conductance that subserves the TM resting potential. It disappears following Na<sup>+</sup> removal or substitution with choline or NMDG<sup>+</sup>, is insensitive to TTX, verapamil, phenamil methanesulfonate, amiloride and GsMTx4, is substituted by Li<sup>+</sup> and Cs<sup>+</sup>, and inhibited by Gd<sup>3+</sup> and Ruthenium Red. Constitutive cation influx is thus not mediated by voltage-operated Na<sup>+</sup>, Ca<sup>2+</sup>, epithelial Na<sup>+</sup> (ENaC) channels, Piezo channels or Na<sup>+</sup>/H<sup>+</sup> exchange but may involve TRP-like channels. Transcriptional analysis detected expression of many TRP genes, with the transcriptome pool dominated by TRPC1 followed by expression of TRPV1, TRPC3, TRPV4 and TRPC5. Pyr3 and Pico1,4,5 did not affect the standing current whereas SKF96365 promoted rather than suppressed, Na<sup>+</sup> influx. SEA-0400 induced a modest hyperpolarization, indicating residual contribution from Na<sup>+</sup>/Ca<sup>2+</sup> exchange. The resting membrane potential in human TM cells is thus maintained by a constitutive monovalent cation leak current with properties not unlike those of TRP channels. This conductance is likely to influence conventional outflow by setting the homeostatic steady-state and by regulating the magnitude of pressure-induced currents in normotensive and hypertensive eyes.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":23670,"journal":{"name":"Vision Research","volume":"224 ","pages":"Article 108487"},"PeriodicalIF":1.5,"publicationDate":"2024-09-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142243236","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Optical phase nullification partially restores visual and stereo acuity lost to simulated blur from higher-order wavefront aberrations of keratoconic eyes 光学相位归零可部分恢复角膜炎患者因高阶波前像差造成的模拟模糊而丧失的视觉和立体视力
IF 1.5 4区 心理学 Q4 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-09-18 DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2024.108486
Bhagya Lakshmi Marella , Miriam L. Conway , Pravin K. Vaddavalli , Catherine M. Suttle , Shrikant R. Bharadwaj

Contrast demodulation and phase distortions are exaggerated in retinal images blurred by the higher-order wavefront aberrations of keratoconic eyes. While the performance loss from the former parameter is well understood, little is known about the impact of the latter on visual functions in this disease condition. The present study investigated the impact of phase distortions on the monocular logMAR visual acuity, letter discriminability and random-dot stereoacuity of seventeen visually healthy adults (ten for visual acuity and letter discriminability; ten for stereoacuity and three common to both experiments) using images that were computationally blurred by four different higher-order wavefront aberration profiles of keratoconic eyes that showed significant distortions in the phase spectrum. Participants viewed these images through 2 mm artificial pupils to negate their native ocular wavefront aberrations. The results showed progressive losses in visual acuity and stereoacuity with increasing blur, a third of which could be recovered following phase nullification. Letter discriminability also improved following phase nullification, more so for smaller than larger optotypes. Stereoacuity loss and, consequently, its recovery following phase nullification was more prominent for profiles simulating unilateral asymmetric keratoconus than for profiles simulating bilateral symmetric keratoconus. These results agree with previous reports obtained from blur induced with lower-order aberrations and indicate that a similar trend may be observed for more complex patterns of blur like keratoconus. Overall, both contrast demodulation and misalignment of the local features of the blurred image may contribute to losses of spatial and depth vision in keratoconus. Phase nullification may partially mitigate these losses, thereby allowing the processing of finer spatial details and veridical disparity estimations for improved depth perception.

角膜病变眼睛的高阶波前像差会使视网膜图像的对比度解调和相位失真加剧。虽然前一种参数造成的性能损失已广为人知,但后一种参数对这种疾病条件下视觉功能的影响却知之甚少。本研究调查了相位畸变对 17 名视力健康的成年人(视力和字母辨别力为 10 人;立体视力为 10 人,两项实验共用 3 人)的单眼 logMAR 视敏度、字母辨别力和随机点立体视力的影响。受试者通过 2 毫米的人工瞳孔观看这些图像,以抵消其眼球波前像差。结果显示,随着模糊度的增加,视敏度和立体清晰度逐渐下降,其中三分之一的视敏度和立体清晰度在相位失效后可以恢复。字母辨别力也在相位失效后得到改善,小视窗比大视窗的改善幅度更大。对于模拟单侧非对称角膜屈光不正的轮廓来说,立体视力的损失比模拟双侧对称角膜屈光不正的轮廓更明显,因此,相位归零后立体视力的恢复也更明显。这些结果与之前从低阶像差引起的模糊中获得的报告一致,并表明对于更复杂的模糊模式,如角膜塑形镜,也可能观察到类似的趋势。总之,对比度解调和模糊图像局部特征的错位可能会导致角膜塑形镜的空间和深度视觉损失。相位无效化可以部分缓解这些损失,从而允许处理更精细的空间细节和进行真实的差距估计,以改善深度知觉。
{"title":"Optical phase nullification partially restores visual and stereo acuity lost to simulated blur from higher-order wavefront aberrations of keratoconic eyes","authors":"Bhagya Lakshmi Marella ,&nbsp;Miriam L. Conway ,&nbsp;Pravin K. Vaddavalli ,&nbsp;Catherine M. Suttle ,&nbsp;Shrikant R. Bharadwaj","doi":"10.1016/j.visres.2024.108486","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.visres.2024.108486","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Contrast demodulation and phase distortions are exaggerated in retinal images blurred by the higher-order wavefront aberrations of keratoconic eyes. While the performance loss from the former parameter is well understood, little is known about the impact of the latter on visual functions in this disease condition. The present study investigated the impact of phase distortions on the monocular logMAR visual acuity, letter discriminability and random-dot stereoacuity of seventeen visually healthy adults (ten for visual acuity and letter discriminability; ten for stereoacuity and three common to both experiments) using images that were computationally blurred by four different higher-order wavefront aberration profiles of keratoconic eyes that showed significant distortions in the phase spectrum. Participants viewed these images through 2 mm artificial pupils to negate their native ocular wavefront aberrations. The results showed progressive losses in visual acuity and stereoacuity with increasing blur, a third of which could be recovered following phase nullification. Letter discriminability also improved following phase nullification, more so for smaller than larger optotypes. Stereoacuity loss and, consequently, its recovery following phase nullification was more prominent for profiles simulating unilateral asymmetric keratoconus than for profiles simulating bilateral symmetric keratoconus. These results agree with previous reports obtained from blur induced with lower-order aberrations and indicate that a similar trend may be observed for more complex patterns of blur like keratoconus. Overall, both contrast demodulation and misalignment of the local features of the blurred image may contribute to losses of spatial and depth vision in keratoconus. Phase nullification may partially mitigate these losses, thereby allowing the processing of finer spatial details and veridical disparity estimations for improved depth perception.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":23670,"journal":{"name":"Vision Research","volume":"224 ","pages":"Article 108486"},"PeriodicalIF":1.5,"publicationDate":"2024-09-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142243235","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Two different visual stimuli that cause axial eye shortening have no additive effect 导致眼轴缩短的两种不同视觉刺激没有叠加效应
IF 1.5 4区 心理学 Q4 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-09-12 DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2024.108485
Lea Ingrassia , Barbara Swiatczak , Frank Schaeffel

Previous studies identified two visual stimuli that can shorten the human eye by thickening the choroid after short-term visual stimulation, potentially inhibiting myopia: (1) watching digitally filtered movies where the red plane has full spatial resolution while green and blue are low-pass filtered according to the human longitudinal chromatic aberration (LCA) function (the “red in focus” filter), and (2) reading text with inverted contrast. This study aimed to determine whether combining these two stimuli would have an additive effect on axial length. Twenty-two emmetropic subjects were recruited to read text (standard and inverted contrast) for 30 min from a large screen, 2 m away, either unfiltered or filtered with the “red in focus” filter. Axial length was measured before and after each reading episode using low-coherence interferometry (Lenstar LS 900, Haag Streit). Reading text with conventional contrast polarity (dark letters on a bright background) resulted in no significant axial length change. Adding the “red in focus” filter did not alter the outcome. Consistent with previous findings, reading inverted contrast text made emmetropic eyes shorter. Surprisingly, when the text was combined with the “red in focus” filter, eyes became longer rather than shorter. A possible explanation for this contradictory result is that, for the text stimulus, the “red in focus” filter removes spatial information in the blue channel needed by the retina to use LCA analysis to thicken the choroid.

之前的研究发现,有两种视觉刺激可以在短期视觉刺激后通过增厚脉络膜来缩短人眼的长度,从而有可能抑制近视:(1)观看数字滤波电影,其中红色平面具有完整的空间分辨率,而绿色和蓝色则根据人类纵向色差(LCA)函数("聚焦红色 "滤波器)进行了低通滤波;(2)阅读对比度倒置的文本。本研究旨在确定将这两种刺激结合起来是否会对轴向长度产生叠加效应。研究人员招募了 22 名散光受试者,让他们在 30 分钟内阅读 2 米外大屏幕上的文字(标准对比度和反差对比度),阅读时可以不加滤镜,也可以使用 "聚焦红色 "滤镜。使用低相干干涉仪(Lenstar LS 900,Haag Streit)在每次阅读前后测量轴长。在阅读传统对比度极性(亮背景上的深色字母)的文字时,轴长没有明显变化。添加 "聚焦红色 "滤光片也不会改变结果。与之前的研究结果一致,阅读倒置对比度的文字会使散光眼睛变短。令人惊讶的是,当文字与 "聚焦红色 "滤镜结合使用时,眼睛不但没有变短,反而变长了。对这一矛盾结果的一种可能解释是,对于文字刺激,"聚焦红色 "滤镜去除了视网膜利用 LCA 分析来增厚脉络膜所需的蓝色通道空间信息。
{"title":"Two different visual stimuli that cause axial eye shortening have no additive effect","authors":"Lea Ingrassia ,&nbsp;Barbara Swiatczak ,&nbsp;Frank Schaeffel","doi":"10.1016/j.visres.2024.108485","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.visres.2024.108485","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Previous studies identified two visual stimuli that can shorten the human eye by thickening the choroid after short-term visual stimulation, potentially inhibiting myopia: (1) watching digitally filtered movies where the red plane has full spatial resolution while green and blue are low-pass filtered according to the human longitudinal chromatic aberration (LCA) function (the “red in focus” filter), and (2) reading text with inverted contrast. This study aimed to determine whether combining these two stimuli would have an additive effect on axial length. Twenty-two emmetropic subjects were recruited to read text (standard and inverted contrast) for 30 min from a large screen, 2 m away, either unfiltered or filtered with the “red in focus” filter. Axial length was measured before and after each reading episode using low-coherence interferometry (Lenstar LS 900, Haag Streit). Reading text with conventional contrast polarity (dark letters on a bright background) resulted in no significant axial length change. Adding the “red in focus” filter did not alter the outcome. Consistent with previous findings, reading inverted contrast text made emmetropic eyes shorter. Surprisingly, when the text was combined with the “red in focus” filter, eyes became longer rather than shorter. A possible explanation for this contradictory result is that, for the text stimulus, the “red in focus” filter removes spatial information in the blue channel needed by the retina to use LCA analysis to thicken the choroid.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":23670,"journal":{"name":"Vision Research","volume":"224 ","pages":"Article 108485"},"PeriodicalIF":1.5,"publicationDate":"2024-09-12","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0042698924001299/pdfft?md5=25f92cb2513b50b87cd528396e732a2e&pid=1-s2.0-S0042698924001299-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142167480","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Scene context and attention independently facilitate MEG decoding of object category 场景背景和注意力可独立促进 MEG 对物体类别的解码
IF 1.5 4区 心理学 Q4 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-09-10 DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2024.108484
Olga Leticevscaia , Talia Brandman , Marius V. Peelen

Many of the objects we encounter in our everyday environments would be hard to recognize without any expectations about these objects. For example, a distant silhouette may be perceived as a car because we expect objects of that size, positioned on a road, to be cars. Reflecting the influence of such expectations on visual processing, neuroimaging studies have shown that when objects are poorly visible, expectations derived from scene context facilitate the representations of these objects in visual cortex from around 300 ms after scene onset. The current magnetoencephalography (MEG) study tested whether this facilitation occurs independently of attention and task relevance. Participants viewed degraded objects alone or within scene context while they either attended the scenes (attended condition) or the fixation cross (unattended condition), also temporally directing attention away from the scenes. Results showed that at 300 ms after stimulus onset, multivariate classifiers trained to distinguish clearly visible animate vs inanimate objects generalized to distinguish degraded objects in scenes better than degraded objects alone, despite the added clutter of the scene background. Attention also modulated object representations at this latency, with better category decoding in the attended than the unattended condition. The modulatory effects of context and attention were independent of each other. Finally, data from the current study and a previous study were combined (N = 51) to provide a more detailed temporal characterization of contextual facilitation. These results extend previous work by showing that facilitatory scene-object interactions are independent of the specific task performed on the visual input.

我们在日常环境中遇到的许多物体,如果没有对这些物体的预期,是很难识别的。例如,一个远处的剪影可能会被认为是一辆汽车,因为我们预期在道路上的这种大小的物体就是汽车。神经影像学研究表明,当物体的可见度较低时,从场景背景中获得的预期会在场景开始后的 300 毫秒左右促进这些物体在视觉皮层中的表征,这反映了这种预期对视觉处理的影响。目前的脑磁图(MEG)研究测试了这种促进作用是否与注意力和任务相关性无关。受试者单独或在场景背景下观看退化对象,同时注意场景(注意条件)或固定十字架(不注意条件),并在时间上将注意力从场景上转移开。结果表明,在刺激开始后的 300 毫秒,尽管增加了杂乱的场景背景,但为区分清晰可见的有生命物体和无生命物体而训练的多元分类器仍能更好地区分场景中的退化物体。在这一潜伏期,注意力也对物体表征产生了调节作用,有注意力条件下的类别解码效果优于无注意力条件下的类别解码效果。情境和注意力的调节作用是相互独立的。最后,本研究和之前的一项研究(N = 51)的数据相结合,提供了更详细的情境促进的时间特征。这些结果扩展了之前的研究,表明场景与物体之间的交互促进作用与视觉输入的具体任务无关。
{"title":"Scene context and attention independently facilitate MEG decoding of object category","authors":"Olga Leticevscaia ,&nbsp;Talia Brandman ,&nbsp;Marius V. Peelen","doi":"10.1016/j.visres.2024.108484","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.visres.2024.108484","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Many of the objects we encounter in our everyday environments would be hard to recognize without any expectations about these objects. For example, a distant silhouette may be perceived as a car because we expect objects of that size, positioned on a road, to be cars. Reflecting the influence of such expectations on visual processing, neuroimaging studies have shown that when objects are poorly visible, expectations derived from scene context facilitate the representations of these objects in visual cortex from around 300 ms after scene onset. The current magnetoencephalography (MEG) study tested whether this facilitation occurs independently of attention and task relevance. Participants viewed degraded objects alone or within scene context while they either attended the scenes (attended condition) or the fixation cross (unattended condition), also temporally directing attention away from the scenes. Results showed that at 300 ms after stimulus onset, multivariate classifiers trained to distinguish clearly visible animate vs inanimate objects generalized to distinguish degraded objects in scenes better than degraded objects alone, despite the added clutter of the scene background. Attention also modulated object representations at this latency, with better category decoding in the attended than the unattended condition. The modulatory effects of context and attention were independent of each other. Finally, data from the current study and a previous study were combined (N = 51) to provide a more detailed temporal characterization of contextual facilitation. These results extend previous work by showing that facilitatory scene-object interactions are independent of the specific task performed on the visual input.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":23670,"journal":{"name":"Vision Research","volume":"224 ","pages":"Article 108484"},"PeriodicalIF":1.5,"publicationDate":"2024-09-10","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0042698924001287/pdfft?md5=71069484f081043895f18f372fa0d24a&pid=1-s2.0-S0042698924001287-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142162526","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Special issue: Seeing colors in nature 特刊:从自然中看色彩
IF 1.5 4区 心理学 Q4 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-09-03 DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2024.108476
Sérgio M.C. Nascimento, David H. Foster, Almut Kelber, Hidehiko Komatsu
{"title":"Special issue: Seeing colors in nature","authors":"Sérgio M.C. Nascimento,&nbsp;David H. Foster,&nbsp;Almut Kelber,&nbsp;Hidehiko Komatsu","doi":"10.1016/j.visres.2024.108476","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.visres.2024.108476","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":23670,"journal":{"name":"Vision Research","volume":"224 ","pages":"Article 108476"},"PeriodicalIF":1.5,"publicationDate":"2024-09-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142128574","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Effects of elevated intraocular pressure on alpha ganglion cells in experimental glaucoma mice 眼压升高对实验性青光眼小鼠α神经节细胞的影响
IF 1.5 4区 心理学 Q4 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-08-31 DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2024.108475
Samuel M. Wu, Ji-Jie Pang

Glaucoma is a leading cause of blindness worldwide and glaucoma patients exhibit an early diffuse loss of retinal sensitivity followed by focal loss of RGCs. Combining some previous published results and some new data, this paper provides our current view on how high IOP (H-IOP) affects the light response sensitivity of a subset of RGCs, the alpha-ganglion cells (αGCs), as well as their presynaptic bipolar cells (DBCs and HBCs) and A2 amacrine cells (AIIACs) in dark-adapted mouse retinas. Our data demonstrate that H-IOP in experimental glaucoma mice significantly decreases light-evoked spike response sensitivity of sONαGCs and sOFFαGCs (i.e., raises thresholds by 1.5–2.5 log units), but not that of the tONαGCs and tOFFαGCs. The sensitivity loss in sONαGCs and sOFFαGCs is mediated by a H-IOP induced suppression of AIIAC response which is caused by a decrease of transmission efficacy of the DBCR→AIIAC synapse. We also provide evidence supporting the hypothesis that BK channels in the A17AC→DBCR feedback synapse are the H-IOP sensor that regulates the DBCR→AIIAC synaptic efficacy, as BK channel blocker IBTX mimics the action of H-IOP. Our results provide useful information for designing strategies for early detection and possible treatments of glaucoma as physiological changes occur before irreversible structural damage.

青光眼是导致全球失明的主要原因之一,青光眼患者早期会出现弥漫性视网膜灵敏度下降,随后会出现局灶性 RGC 损失。结合之前发表的一些结果和一些新数据,本文就高眼压(H-IOP)如何影响暗适应小鼠视网膜中的RGCs亚群--α-神经节细胞(αGCs)及其突触前双极细胞(DBCs和HBCs)和A2羊膜细胞(AIIACs)的光反应灵敏度提出了我们目前的看法。我们的数据表明,实验性青光眼小鼠的高眼压会显著降低 sONαGCs 和 sOFFαGCs 的光诱发尖峰反应灵敏度(即阈值升高 1.5-2.5 对数单位),但不会降低 tONαGCs 和 tOFFαGCs 的灵敏度。sONαGCs和sOFFαGCs的敏感性下降是由H-IOP诱导的AIIAC反应抑制介导的,而AIIAC反应抑制是由DBCR→AIIAC突触的传导效率下降引起的。我们还提供了支持以下假设的证据:A17AC→DBCR反馈突触中的BK通道是调节DBCR→AIIAC突触功效的H-IOP传感器,因为BK通道阻断剂IBTX能模拟H-IOP的作用。我们的研究结果为设计青光眼早期检测和可能的治疗策略提供了有用的信息,因为生理变化发生在不可逆转的结构损伤之前。
{"title":"Effects of elevated intraocular pressure on alpha ganglion cells in experimental glaucoma mice","authors":"Samuel M. Wu,&nbsp;Ji-Jie Pang","doi":"10.1016/j.visres.2024.108475","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.visres.2024.108475","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Glaucoma is a leading cause of blindness worldwide and glaucoma patients exhibit an early diffuse loss of retinal sensitivity followed by focal loss of RGCs. Combining some previous published results and some new data, this paper provides our current view on how high IOP (H-IOP) affects the light response sensitivity of a subset of RGCs, the alpha-ganglion cells (αGCs), as well as their presynaptic bipolar cells (DBCs and HBCs) and A2 amacrine cells (AIIACs) in dark-adapted mouse retinas. Our data demonstrate that H-IOP in experimental glaucoma mice significantly decreases light-evoked spike response sensitivity of sONαGCs and sOFFαGCs (i.e., raises thresholds by 1.5–2.5 log units), but not that of the tONαGCs and tOFFαGCs. The sensitivity loss in sONαGCs and sOFFαGCs is mediated by a H-IOP induced suppression of AIIAC response which is caused by a decrease of transmission efficacy of the DBC<sub>R</sub>→AIIAC synapse. We also provide evidence supporting the hypothesis that BK channels in the A17AC→DBC<sub>R</sub> feedback synapse are the H-IOP sensor that regulates the DBC<sub>R</sub>→AIIAC synaptic efficacy, as BK channel blocker IBTX mimics the action of H-IOP. Our results provide useful information for designing strategies for early detection and possible treatments of glaucoma as physiological changes occur before irreversible structural damage.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":23670,"journal":{"name":"Vision Research","volume":"224 ","pages":"Article 108475"},"PeriodicalIF":1.5,"publicationDate":"2024-08-31","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142095426","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Vision Research
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1