首页 > 最新文献

Vision Research最新文献

英文 中文
Visual field asymmetries in visual word form identification 视觉词形识别中的视野不对称问题
IF 1.8 4区 心理学 Q4 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-04-13 DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2024.108413
Li-Ting Tsai , Kuo-Meng Liao , Chiun-Ho Hou , Yuh Jang , Chien-Chung Chen

Visual performance across the visual fields interacts with visual tasks and visual stimuli, and visual resolution decreases as a function of eccentricity, varying at isoeccentric locations. In this study, we investigated the extent of asymmetry and the rate of change in visual acuity threshold for visual word form (VWF) identification at horizontal and vertical azimuths across the fovea, and at eccentricities of 1°, 2°, 4°, 6° and 8° for 10%, 20%, 40%, and 80% contrast levels, to determine whether and how the eccentricities, meridians, and contrasts modulated the VWF identification acuity threshold. The stimuli were 16 traditional Chinese characters of similar legibility. Participants pressed a key to indicate the character presented, either monocularly or binocularly, at one of 21 randomly selected locations. A staircase procedure was used to determine the threshold, and a multiple linear regression model was used to fit the linear cortical magnification factor (CMF). We found that (1) the asymmetry was most pronounced on the vertical and superior azimuths, (2) the asymmetry between the right and left azimuths was not significant, (3) the CMF was significantly smaller on the vertical azimuth than on the horizontal azimuth, (4) the CMF was smaller on the superior vertical azimuth than on the inferior azimuth, and (5) monocular viewing and low contrast enhanced the CMF difference between azimuths. In conclusion, vertical and horizontal azimuths, location of eccentricity, contrast levels of word symbols, and monocular/binocular viewing have different effects on visual field asymmetry and cortical magnification factors.

整个视野的视觉表现与视觉任务和视觉刺激相互影响,视觉分辨率随偏心率的变化而降低,在等中心位置则有所不同。在这项研究中,我们研究了在整个眼窝的水平和垂直方位角,以及偏心率为1°、2°、4°、6°和8°,对比度为10%、20%、40%和80%时,视觉字形(VWF)识别视敏度阈值的不对称程度和变化率,以确定偏心率、经线和对比度是否以及如何调节VWF识别视敏度阈值。刺激物是 16 个可读性相似的繁体字。受试者在 21 个随机选择的位置中按下一个键来指示单眼或双眼显示的字符。我们使用阶梯程序来确定阈值,并使用多元线性回归模型来拟合线性皮层放大系数(CMF)。我们发现:(1) 垂直方位角和上方位角的不对称性最明显;(2) 左右方位角的不对称性不明显;(3) 垂直方位角的 CMF 明显小于水平方位角;(4) 垂直方位角上端的 CMF 小于方位角下端的 CMF;(5) 单眼观察和低对比度增强了方位角之间的 CMF 差异。总之,垂直和水平方位角、偏心位置、单词符号的对比度以及单眼/双眼观看对视野不对称和皮层放大系数有不同的影响。
{"title":"Visual field asymmetries in visual word form identification","authors":"Li-Ting Tsai ,&nbsp;Kuo-Meng Liao ,&nbsp;Chiun-Ho Hou ,&nbsp;Yuh Jang ,&nbsp;Chien-Chung Chen","doi":"10.1016/j.visres.2024.108413","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2024.108413","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Visual performance across the visual fields interacts with visual tasks and visual stimuli, and visual resolution decreases as a function of eccentricity, varying at isoeccentric locations. In this study, we investigated the extent of asymmetry and the rate of change in visual acuity threshold for visual word form (VWF) identification at horizontal and vertical azimuths across the fovea, and at eccentricities of 1°, 2°, 4°, 6° and 8° for 10%, 20%, 40%, and 80% contrast levels, to determine whether and how the eccentricities, meridians, and contrasts modulated the VWF identification acuity threshold. The stimuli were 16 traditional Chinese characters of similar legibility. Participants pressed a key to indicate the character presented, either monocularly or binocularly, at one of 21 randomly selected locations. A staircase procedure was used to determine the threshold, and a multiple linear regression model was used to fit the linear cortical magnification factor (CMF). We found that (1) the asymmetry was most pronounced on the vertical and superior azimuths, (2) the asymmetry between the right and left azimuths was not significant, (3) the CMF was significantly smaller on the vertical azimuth than on the horizontal azimuth, (4) the CMF was smaller on the superior vertical azimuth than on the inferior azimuth, and (5) monocular viewing and low contrast enhanced the CMF difference between azimuths. In conclusion, vertical and horizontal azimuths, location of eccentricity, contrast levels of word symbols, and monocular/binocular viewing have different effects on visual field asymmetry and cortical magnification factors.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":23670,"journal":{"name":"Vision Research","volume":"220 ","pages":"Article 108413"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-04-13","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140549633","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Two-photon vision – Seeing colors in infrared 双光子视觉 - 用红外线观察颜色
IF 1.8 4区 心理学 Q4 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-04-11 DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2024.108404
Katarzyna Komar

This review discusses the current state of knowledge regarding the phenomenon called two-photon vision. It involves the visual perception of pulsed infrared beams in the range of 850–1200 nm as having colors corresponding to one-half of the IR wavelengths. It is caused by two-photon absorption (TPA), which occurs when the visual photopigment interacts simultaneously with two infrared photons.

The physical mechanism of TPA is described, and implications about the efficiency of the process are considered. The spectral range of two-photon vision is defined, along with a detailed discussion of the known differences in color perception between normal and two-photon vision. The quadratic dependence of the luminance of two-photon stimuli on the power of the stimulating beam is also explained. Examples of recording two-photon vision in the retinas of mice and monkeys are provided from the literature. Finally, applications of two-photon vision are discussed, particularly two-photon microperimetry, which has been under development for several years; and the potential advantages of two-photon retinal displays are explained.

本综述讨论了有关双光子视觉现象的知识现状。双光子视觉是指视觉感知到 850-1200 纳米范围内的脉冲红外线光束具有与二分之一红外线波长相对应的颜色。它是由双光子吸收(TPA)引起的,当视觉光色素同时与两个红外光子相互作用时,就会发生双光子吸收。对双光子视觉的光谱范围进行了定义,并详细讨论了正常视觉和双光子视觉在色彩感知方面的已知差异。此外,还解释了双光子刺激亮度与刺激光束功率的二次关系。文献中提供了在小鼠和猴子视网膜上记录双光子视觉的例子。最后,讨论了双光子视觉的应用,特别是双光子显微瞳孔测量法,该技术已开发数年;并解释了双光子视网膜显示器的潜在优势。
{"title":"Two-photon vision – Seeing colors in infrared","authors":"Katarzyna Komar","doi":"10.1016/j.visres.2024.108404","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2024.108404","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>This review discusses the current state of knowledge regarding the phenomenon called two-photon vision. It involves the visual perception of pulsed infrared beams in the range of 850–1200 nm as having colors corresponding to one-half of the IR wavelengths. It is caused by two-photon absorption (TPA), which occurs when the visual photopigment interacts simultaneously with two infrared photons.</p><p>The physical mechanism of TPA is described, and implications about the efficiency of the process are considered. The spectral range of two-photon vision is defined, along with a detailed discussion of the known differences in color perception between normal and two-photon vision. The quadratic dependence of the luminance of two-photon stimuli on the power of the stimulating beam is also explained. Examples of recording two-photon vision in the retinas of mice and monkeys are provided from the literature. Finally, applications of two-photon vision are discussed, particularly two-photon microperimetry, which has been under development for several years; and the potential advantages of two-photon retinal displays are explained.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":23670,"journal":{"name":"Vision Research","volume":"220 ","pages":"Article 108404"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-04-11","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0042698924000488/pdfft?md5=71bd85ed9386356a140bd2016bf7aa73&pid=1-s2.0-S0042698924000488-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140545966","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The role of perceptual processing in the oddball effect revealed by the Thatcher illusion 撒切尔幻觉揭示的怪人效应中的知觉加工作用
IF 1.8 4区 心理学 Q4 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-04-10 DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2024.108399
Akira Sarodo , Kentaro Yamamoto , Katsumi Watanabe

When a novel stimulus (oddball) appears after repeated presentation of an identical stimulus, the oddball is perceived to last longer than the repeated stimuli, a phenomenon known as the oddball effect. We investigated whether the perceptual or physical differences between the repeated and oddball stimuli are more important for the oddball effect. To manipulate the perceptual difference while keeping their physical visual features constant, we used the Thatcher illusion, in which an inversion of a face hinders recognition of distortion in its facial features. We found that the Thatcherized face presented after repeated presentation of an intact face induced a stronger oddball effect when the faces were upright than when they were inverted (Experiment 1). However, the difference in the oddball effect between face orientations was not observed when the intact face was presented as the oddball after repeated presentation of a Thatcherized face (Experiment 2). These results were replicated when participants performed both the intact-repeated and Thatcherized-repeated conditions in a single experiment (Experiment 3). Two control experiments confirmed that the repeated presentation of the preceding stimuli is necessary for the difference in duration distortion to occur (Experiments 4 and 5). The results suggest the considerable role of perceptual processing in the oddball effect. We discuss the discrepancy in the results between the intact-repeated and Thatcherized-repeated conditions in terms of predictive coding.

当重复出现一个相同的刺激后,又出现一个新的刺激(奇异球)时,奇异球被认为比重复刺激持续的时间更长,这种现象被称为奇异球效应。我们研究了重复刺激和怪球刺激之间的知觉差异还是物理差异对怪球效应更为重要。为了在保持物理视觉特征不变的情况下操纵知觉上的差异,我们使用了撒切尔幻觉,在这种幻觉中,倒置的人脸会阻碍对其面部特征扭曲的识别。我们发现,在重复呈现一张完整的脸之后,再呈现一张柴契尔幻觉化的脸,当这些脸直立时比倒置时引起的怪球效应更强(实验 1)。然而,当重复呈现一张 Thatcherized 人脸后,再呈现一张完整的人脸作为奇异球时,却观察不到不同人脸方向的奇异球效应差异(实验 2)。当被试在一次实验中同时进行完整重复和 Thatcherized 重复两个条件时,上述结果得到了重复(实验 3)。两个对照实验证实,重复呈现前面的刺激是发生时长失真差异的必要条件(实验 4 和 5)。这些结果表明,知觉加工在奇异球效应中起着相当重要的作用。我们从预测编码的角度讨论了完整重复和茅塞顿开重复条件下的结果差异。
{"title":"The role of perceptual processing in the oddball effect revealed by the Thatcher illusion","authors":"Akira Sarodo ,&nbsp;Kentaro Yamamoto ,&nbsp;Katsumi Watanabe","doi":"10.1016/j.visres.2024.108399","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2024.108399","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>When a novel stimulus (oddball) appears after repeated presentation of an identical stimulus, the oddball is perceived to last longer than the repeated stimuli, a phenomenon known as the oddball effect. We investigated whether the perceptual or physical differences between the repeated and oddball stimuli are more important for the oddball effect. To manipulate the perceptual difference while keeping their physical visual features constant, we used the Thatcher illusion, in which an inversion of a face hinders recognition of distortion in its facial features. We found that the Thatcherized face presented after repeated presentation of an intact face induced a stronger oddball effect when the faces were upright than when they were inverted (Experiment 1). However, the difference in the oddball effect between face orientations was not observed when the intact face was presented as the oddball after repeated presentation of a Thatcherized face (Experiment 2). These results were replicated when participants performed both the intact-repeated and Thatcherized-repeated conditions in a single experiment (Experiment 3). Two control experiments confirmed that the repeated presentation of the preceding stimuli is necessary for the difference in duration distortion to occur (Experiments 4 and 5). The results suggest the considerable role of perceptual processing in the oddball effect. We discuss the discrepancy in the results between the intact-repeated and Thatcherized-repeated conditions in terms of predictive coding.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":23670,"journal":{"name":"Vision Research","volume":"220 ","pages":"Article 108399"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-04-10","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0042698924000439/pdfft?md5=988ab9e9736a1bc36fcaa47725a5ffae&pid=1-s2.0-S0042698924000439-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140540868","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Similarity in motion binds and bends judgments of aspect ratio 运动中的相似性约束和弯曲了对长宽比的判断
IF 1.8 4区 心理学 Q4 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-04-10 DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2024.108400
Görkem Er, Timothy D. Sweeny

It is well known that objects become grouped in perceptual organization when they share some visual feature, like a common direction of motion. Less well known is that grouping can change how people perceive a set of objects. For example, when a pair of shapes consistently share a common region of space, their aspect ratios tend to be perceived as more similar (are attracted toward each other). Conversely, when shapes are assigned to different regions in space their aspect ratios repel from each other. Here we examine whether the visual system produce both attractive and repulsive distortions when the state of grouping between a pair of shapes changes on a moment-to-moment basis. Observers viewed a pair of ellipses that differed in terms of how flat or tall they were and reported the aspect ratio of one ellipse from the pair. Each ellipse was defined by a cloud of coherently-moving dots, and the dots within the two ellipses had either the same or different directions of motion, varying from trial-to-trial. We found that the cued ellipse's aspect ratio was reported to be repelled from the aspect ratio of the uncued ellipse when the shapes had different directions of motion compared to when they had the same direction of motion. These results suggest that the visual system can adaptively alter visual experience based on grouping, in particular, repelling the appearance of objects when they do not appear to go together, and it can do so quickly and flexibly.

众所周知,当物体具有某些共同的视觉特征(如共同的运动方向)时,它们就会在知觉组织中被分组。鲜为人知的是,分组可以改变人们对一组物体的感知方式。例如,当一对形状持续共享一个共同的空间区域时,它们的长宽比往往会被认为更加相似(相互吸引)。相反,当形状被分配到不同的空间区域时,它们的长宽比就会相互排斥。在此,我们研究了当一对图形的分组状态在瞬间发生变化时,视觉系统是否会同时产生吸引和排斥失真。观察者看到的是一对椭圆,它们在扁平或高度上存在差异,观察者报告这对椭圆中一个椭圆的长宽比。每个椭圆都由一团连贯运动的圆点所定义,两个椭圆内的圆点的运动方向或相同或不同,在不同的试验中各不相同。我们发现,与运动方向相同的椭圆形相比,当运动方向不同时,提示椭圆形的长宽比与未提示椭圆形的长宽比相斥。这些结果表明,视觉系统可以在分组的基础上适应性地改变视觉体验,特别是当物体看起来并不在一起时,视觉系统会排斥它们的出现,而且可以快速灵活地做到这一点。
{"title":"Similarity in motion binds and bends judgments of aspect ratio","authors":"Görkem Er,&nbsp;Timothy D. Sweeny","doi":"10.1016/j.visres.2024.108400","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2024.108400","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>It is well known that objects become grouped in perceptual organization when they share some visual feature, like a common direction of motion. Less well known is that grouping can change how people perceive a set of objects. For example, when a pair of shapes consistently share a common region of space, their aspect ratios tend to be perceived as more similar (are attracted toward each other). Conversely, when shapes are assigned to different regions in space their aspect ratios repel from each other. Here we examine whether the visual system produce both attractive and repulsive distortions when the state of grouping between a pair of shapes changes on a moment-to-moment basis. Observers viewed a pair of ellipses that differed in terms of how flat or tall they were and reported the aspect ratio of one ellipse from the pair. Each ellipse was defined by a cloud of coherently-moving dots, and the dots within the two ellipses had either the same or different directions of motion, varying from trial-to-trial. We found that the cued ellipse's aspect ratio was reported to be repelled from the aspect ratio of the uncued ellipse when the shapes had different directions of motion compared to when they had the same direction of motion. These results suggest that the visual system can adaptively alter visual experience based on grouping, in particular, repelling the appearance of objects when they do not appear to go together, and it can do so quickly and flexibly.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":23670,"journal":{"name":"Vision Research","volume":"220 ","pages":"Article 108400"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-04-10","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140540841","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Selective microRNA expression of exosomes from retinal pigment epithelial cells by oxidative stress 氧化应激对视网膜色素上皮细胞外泌体微RNA表达的选择性影响
IF 1.8 4区 心理学 Q4 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-04-08 DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2024.108388
Zhengyu Zhang , Qinyuan Gu , Lu Chen , Dongqing Yuan , Xunyi Gu , Huiming Qian , Ping Xie , Qinghuai Liu , Zizhong Hu

The function of exosomal miRNAs (miRs) in retinal degeneration is largely unclear. We were aimed to investigate the functions of exosomes as well as their miRs derived from retinal pigment epithelial (RPE) cells following exposure to oxidative stress (OS). After the OS by lipopolysaccharide and rotenone on RPE cells, interleukin-1 beta (IL-1β), Interleukin-6 (IL-6), Tumor Necrosis Factor-alpha (TNF-α) were upregulated, along with the decreased mitochondrial membrane potential and upregulated oxidative damage marker 8-OH-dG in RPE cells. RPE-derived exosomes were then isolated, identified, injected into the subretinal space in mice. After subretinal injection, RPE-exosomes after OS not only induced higher ROS level and apoptotic retinal cells, but also elevated IL-1β, IL-6 alongside TNF-α expressions among retina/RPE/choroidal complex. Next, miRs inside the exosomes were sequenced by the next generation sequencing (NGS) technology. NGS revealed that certain miRs were abundant in exosomes, while others were selectively kept by RPE cells. Further, downregulated miRs, like miR-125b-5p, miR-125a-5p, alongside miR-128-3p, and upregulated miR, such as miR-7-5p were validated byRT-qPCR. Finally, Gene Ontology (GO) annotation and Kyoto Encyclopedia of Genes and Genomes (KEGG) pathway enrichment analysis were used to find the possible target genes of those selective exosomal miRs. Our results proved that the RPE-derived exosomes after OS selectively express certain miRs, providing novel insights into the pathogenesis of age-related macular degeneration (AMD) in future.

外泌体 miRNA(miRs)在视网膜变性中的功能尚不清楚。我们的目的是研究视网膜色素上皮细胞(RPE)暴露于氧化应激(OS)后产生的外泌体及其 miRs 的功能。RPE细胞受到脂多糖和鱼藤酮氧化应激后,白细胞介素-1β(IL-1β)、白细胞介素-6(IL-6)和肿瘤坏死因子-α(TNF-α)上调,线粒体膜电位下降,氧化损伤标志物8-OH-dG上调。然后分离、鉴定 RPE 衍生的外泌体,并将其注入小鼠视网膜下间隙。视网膜下注射后,OS后的RPE外泌体不仅诱导了更高的ROS水平和视网膜细胞凋亡,还升高了视网膜/RPE/脉络膜复合体中IL-1β、IL-6和TNF-α的表达。接着,利用新一代测序(NGS)技术对外泌体中的miRs进行了测序。NGS 发现,某些 miRs 在外泌体中含量丰富,而另一些则被 RPE 细胞选择性地保留下来。此外,下调的 miR(如 miR-125b-5p、miR-125a-5p 和 miR-128-3p)和上调的 miR(如 miR-7-5p)也通过 RT-qPCR 得到了验证。最后,通过基因本体(GO)注释和京都基因组百科全书(KEGG)通路富集分析,找到了这些选择性外泌体 miRs 的可能靶基因。我们的研究结果证明,OS后RPE衍生的外泌体选择性地表达了某些miRs,这为今后研究老年性黄斑变性(AMD)的发病机制提供了新的视角。
{"title":"Selective microRNA expression of exosomes from retinal pigment epithelial cells by oxidative stress","authors":"Zhengyu Zhang ,&nbsp;Qinyuan Gu ,&nbsp;Lu Chen ,&nbsp;Dongqing Yuan ,&nbsp;Xunyi Gu ,&nbsp;Huiming Qian ,&nbsp;Ping Xie ,&nbsp;Qinghuai Liu ,&nbsp;Zizhong Hu","doi":"10.1016/j.visres.2024.108388","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2024.108388","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>The function of exosomal miRNAs (miRs) in retinal degeneration is largely unclear. We were aimed to investigate the functions of exosomes as well as their miRs derived from retinal pigment epithelial (RPE) cells following exposure to oxidative stress (OS). After the OS by lipopolysaccharide and rotenone on RPE cells, interleukin-1 beta (IL-1β), Interleukin-6 (IL-6), Tumor Necrosis Factor-alpha (TNF-α) were upregulated, along with the decreased mitochondrial membrane potential and upregulated oxidative damage marker 8-OH-dG in RPE cells. RPE-derived exosomes were then isolated, identified, injected into the subretinal space in mice. After subretinal injection, RPE-exosomes after OS not only induced higher ROS level and apoptotic retinal cells, but also elevated IL-1β, IL-6 alongside TNF-α expressions among retina/RPE/choroidal complex. Next, miRs inside the exosomes were sequenced by the next generation sequencing (NGS) technology. NGS revealed that certain miRs were abundant in exosomes, while others were selectively kept by RPE cells. Further, downregulated miRs, like miR-125b-5p, miR-125a-5p, alongside miR-128-3p, and upregulated miR, such as miR-7-5p were validated byRT-qPCR. Finally, Gene Ontology (GO) annotation and Kyoto Encyclopedia of Genes and Genomes (KEGG) pathway enrichment analysis were used to find the possible target genes of those selective exosomal miRs. Our results proved that the RPE-derived exosomes after OS selectively express certain miRs, providing novel insights into the pathogenesis of age-related macular degeneration (AMD) in future.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":23670,"journal":{"name":"Vision Research","volume":"220 ","pages":"Article 108388"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-04-08","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140535011","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Localization of multiple opsins in ocular and non-ocular tissues of deep-sea shrimps and the first evidence of co-localization in a rhabdomeric R8 cell (Caridea: Oplophoroidea) 多种蛋白在深海对虾眼部和非眼部组织中的定位以及在横纹肌R8细胞中共同定位的首个证据(鲤形目:Oplophoroidea)
IF 1.8 4区 心理学 Q4 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-04-05 DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2024.108403
Tom Iwanicki , Mireille Steck , Heather Bracken-Grissom , Megan L. Porter

Bioluminescence is a prevalent phenomenon throughout the marine realm and is often the dominant source of light in mesophotic and aphotic depth horizons. Shrimp belonging to the superfamily Oplophoroidea are mesopelagic, perform diel vertical migration, and secrete a bright burst of bioluminescent mucous when threatened. Species in the family Oplophoridae also possess cuticular light-emitting photophores presumably for camouflage via counter-illumination. Many species within the superfamily express a single visual pigment in the retina, consistent with most other large-bodied mesopelagic crustaceans studied to date. Photophore-bearing species have an expanded visual opsin repertoire and dual-sensitivity visual systems, as evidenced by transcriptomes and electroretinograms. In this study, we used immunohistochemistry to describe opsin protein localization in the retinas of four species of Oplophoroidea and non-ocular tissues of Janicella spinicauda. Our results show that Acanthephyra purpurea (Acanthephyridae) retinas possess LWS-only photoreceptors, consistent with the singular peak sensitivity previously reported. Oplophoridae retinas contain two opsin clades (LWS and MWS) consistent with dual-sensitivity. Oplophorus gracilirostris and Systellaspis debilis have LWS in the proximal rhabdom (R1-7 cells) and MWS2 localized in the distal rhabdom (R8 cell). Surprisingly, Janicella spinicauda has LWS in the proximal rhabdom (R1-7) and co-localized MWS1 and MWS2 opsin paralogs in the distal rhabdom, providing the first evidence of co-localization of opsins in a crustacean rhabdomeric R8 cell. Furthermore, opsins were found in multiple non-ocular tissues of J. spinicauda, including nerve, tendon, and photophore. These combined data demonstrate evolutionary novelty and opsin duplication within Oplophoridae, with implications for visual ecology, evolution in mesophotic environments, and a mechanistic understanding of adaptive counter-illumination using photophore bioluminescence.

生物发光是整个海洋领域的普遍现象,通常是中层和噬水深层的主要光源。对虾超科(Oplophoroidea)的对虾属于中上层鱼类,进行日夜垂直洄游,受到威胁时会分泌出明亮的生物发光粘液。Oplophoridae 科的物种还具有角质发光的光生器,可能是为了通过反照明进行伪装。该超科中的许多物种在视网膜上表达单一的视觉色素,这与迄今为止研究的大多数其他大型中上层甲壳动物一致。从转录组和视网膜电图中可以看出,携带光团的物种具有更多的视觉蛋白和双敏感性视觉系统。在这项研究中,我们使用免疫组化方法描述了四种栉水母目动物视网膜和尖嘴雉非眼组织中的视蛋白定位。我们的研究结果表明,Acanthephyra purpurea(Acanthephyridae)视网膜只具有LWS感光器,这与之前报道的单峰灵敏度一致。Oplophoridae的视网膜包含两个感光蛋白支系(LWS和MWS),与双重敏感性一致。Oplophorus gracilirostris 和 Systellaspis debilis 的 LWS 位于近端横纹域(R1-7 细胞),而 MWS2 则位于远端横纹域(R8 细胞)。令人惊讶的是,Janicella spinicauda 的近端横纹肌(R1-7)中有 LWS,远端横纹肌中则有共定位的 MWS1 和 MWS2 蛋白旁系,首次证明了蛋白在甲壳类横纹肌 R8 细胞中的共定位。此外,在棘尾鱼的多个非眼组织中也发现了蛋白酶,包括神经、肌腱和感光体。这些综合数据证明了Oplophoridae内部进化的新颖性和眼蛋白的复制,对视觉生态学、中生环境中的进化以及利用光团生物发光进行适应性反照明的机理理解具有重要意义。
{"title":"Localization of multiple opsins in ocular and non-ocular tissues of deep-sea shrimps and the first evidence of co-localization in a rhabdomeric R8 cell (Caridea: Oplophoroidea)","authors":"Tom Iwanicki ,&nbsp;Mireille Steck ,&nbsp;Heather Bracken-Grissom ,&nbsp;Megan L. Porter","doi":"10.1016/j.visres.2024.108403","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2024.108403","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Bioluminescence is a prevalent phenomenon throughout the marine realm and is often the dominant source of light in mesophotic and aphotic depth horizons. Shrimp belonging to the superfamily Oplophoroidea are mesopelagic, perform diel vertical migration, and secrete a bright burst of bioluminescent mucous when threatened. Species in the family Oplophoridae also possess cuticular light-emitting photophores presumably for camouflage via counter-illumination. Many species within the superfamily express a single visual pigment in the retina, consistent with most other large-bodied mesopelagic crustaceans studied to date. Photophore-bearing species have an expanded visual opsin repertoire and dual-sensitivity visual systems, as evidenced by transcriptomes and electroretinograms. In this study, we used immunohistochemistry to describe opsin protein localization in the retinas of four species of Oplophoroidea and non-ocular tissues of <em>Janicella spinicauda.</em> Our results show that <em>Acanthephyra purpurea</em> (Acanthephyridae) retinas possess LWS-only photoreceptors, consistent with the singular peak sensitivity previously reported. Oplophoridae retinas contain two opsin clades (LWS and MWS) consistent with dual-sensitivity. <em>Oplophorus gracilirostris</em> and <em>Systellaspis debilis</em> have LWS in the proximal rhabdom (R1-7 cells) and MWS2 localized in the distal rhabdom (R8 cell). Surprisingly, <em>Janicella spinicauda</em> has LWS in the proximal rhabdom (R1-7) and co-localized MWS1 and MWS2 opsin paralogs in the distal rhabdom, providing the first evidence of co-localization of opsins in a crustacean rhabdomeric R8 cell. Furthermore, opsins were found in multiple non-ocular tissues of <em>J. spinicauda</em>, including nerve, tendon, and photophore. These combined data demonstrate evolutionary novelty and opsin duplication within Oplophoridae, with implications for visual ecology, evolution in mesophotic environments, and a mechanistic understanding of adaptive counter-illumination using photophore bioluminescence.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":23670,"journal":{"name":"Vision Research","volume":"219 ","pages":"Article 108403"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-04-05","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0042698924000476/pdfft?md5=43adf96f314e6527bd2750376394c6f0&pid=1-s2.0-S0042698924000476-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140350222","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Glossiness perception and its pupillary response 光泽感及其瞳孔反应
IF 1.8 4区 心理学 Q4 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-04-04 DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2024.108393
Hideki Tamura, Shigeki Nakauchi, Tetsuto Minami

Recent studies have revealed that pupillary response changes depend on perceptual factors such as subjective brightness caused by optical illusions and luminance. However, the manner in which the perceptual factor that is derived from the glossiness perception of object surfaces affects the pupillary response remains unclear. We investigated the relationship between the glossiness perception and pupillary response through a glossiness rating experiment that included recording the pupil diameter. We prepared general object images (original) and randomized images (shuffled) that comprised the same images with randomized small square regions as stimuli. The image features were controlled by matching the luminance histogram. The observers were asked to rate the perceived glossiness of the stimuli presented for 3,000 ms and the changes in their pupil diameters were recorded. Images with higher glossiness ratings constricted the pupil size more than those with lower glossiness ratings at the peak constriction of the pupillary responses during the stimulus duration. The linear mixed-effects model demonstrated that the glossiness rating, image category (original/shuffled), variance of the luminance histogram, and stimulus area were most effective in predicting the pupillary responses. These results suggest that the illusory brightness obtained by the image regions of high-glossiness objects, such as specular highlights, induce pupil constriction.

最近的研究表明,瞳孔反应的变化取决于感知因素,如光学幻觉和亮度引起的主观亮度。然而,源自物体表面光泽度感知的感知因素是如何影响瞳孔反应的仍不清楚。我们通过一项包括记录瞳孔直径在内的光泽度评级实验来研究光泽度感知与瞳孔反应之间的关系。我们准备了一般物体图像(原始图像)和随机图像(洗牌图像),后者由带有随机小方块区域的相同图像作为刺激物。图像特征通过匹配亮度直方图来控制。观察者被要求对呈现 3,000 毫秒的刺激物的光泽度进行评分,并记录其瞳孔直径的变化。在刺激持续期间瞳孔反应收缩的峰值,光泽度评级较高的图像比光泽度评级较低的图像更能收缩瞳孔。线性混合效应模型表明,光泽度评级、图像类别(原始图像/混淆图像)、亮度直方图方差和刺激面积对预测瞳孔反应最有效。这些结果表明,高光泽度物体图像区域获得的虚幻亮度(如镜面高光)会诱发瞳孔收缩。
{"title":"Glossiness perception and its pupillary response","authors":"Hideki Tamura,&nbsp;Shigeki Nakauchi,&nbsp;Tetsuto Minami","doi":"10.1016/j.visres.2024.108393","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2024.108393","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Recent studies have revealed that pupillary response changes depend on perceptual factors such as subjective brightness caused by optical illusions and luminance. However, the manner in which the perceptual factor that is derived from the glossiness perception of object surfaces affects the pupillary response remains unclear. We investigated the relationship between the glossiness perception and pupillary response through a glossiness rating experiment that included recording the pupil diameter. We prepared general object images (original) and randomized images (shuffled) that comprised the same images with randomized small square regions as stimuli. The image features were controlled by matching the luminance histogram. The observers were asked to rate the perceived glossiness of the stimuli presented for 3,000 ms and the changes in their pupil diameters were recorded. Images with higher glossiness ratings constricted the pupil size more than those with lower glossiness ratings at the peak constriction of the pupillary responses during the stimulus duration. The linear mixed-effects model demonstrated that the glossiness rating, image category (original/shuffled), variance of the luminance histogram, and stimulus area were most effective in predicting the pupillary responses. These results suggest that the illusory brightness obtained by the image regions of high-glossiness objects, such as specular highlights, induce pupil constriction.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":23670,"journal":{"name":"Vision Research","volume":"219 ","pages":"Article 108393"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-04-04","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0042698924000373/pdfft?md5=490cc345c0db6cdeb11157def7f36ce1&pid=1-s2.0-S0042698924000373-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140348193","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Performance on a contour integration task as a function of contour shape in schizophrenia and controls 精神分裂症患者和对照组在轮廓整合任务中的表现与轮廓形状的函数关系
IF 1.8 4区 心理学 Q4 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-04-04 DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2024.108394
Samyukta Jayakumar , Anthony O. Ahmed , Pamela D. Butler , Steven M. Silverstein , Judy L. Thompson , Aaron R. Seitz

Contour Integration (CI) is the ability to integrate elemental features into objects and is a basic visual process essential for object perception and recognition, and for functioning in visual environments. It is now well documented that people with schizophrenia (SZ), in addition to having cognitive impairments, also have several visual perceptual deficits, including in CI. Here, we retrospectively characterize the performance of both SZ and neurotypical individuals (NT) on a series of contour shapes, made up of Gabor elements, that varied in terms of closure and curvature. Participants in both groups performed a CI training task that included 7 different families of shapes (Lines, Ellipse, Blobs, Squiggles, Spiral, Circle and Letters) for up to 40 sessions. Two parameters were manipulated in the training task: Orientation Jitter (OJ, i.e., orientation deviations of individual Gabor elements from ideal for each shape) and Inducer Number (IN, i.e., number of Gabor elements defining the shape). Results show that both OJ and IN thresholds significantly differed between the groups, with higher (OJ) and lower (IN) thresholds observed in the controls. Furthermore, we found significant effects as a function of the contour shapes, with differences between groups emerging with contours that were considered more complex, e.g., due to having a higher degree of curvature (Blobs, Spiral, Letters). These data can inform future work that aims to characterize visual integration impairments in schizophrenia.

轮廓整合(CI)是将元素特征整合到物体中的能力,是感知和识别物体以及在视觉环境中发挥作用所必需的基本视觉过程。目前有大量资料表明,精神分裂症(SZ)患者除了有认知障碍外,还存在多种视觉感知缺陷,包括轮廓整合(CI)。在这里,我们回顾性地描述了精神分裂症患者和神经畸形患者(NT)在一系列由 Gabor 元素组成的轮廓形状上的表现,这些轮廓形状的闭合度和曲率各不相同。两组参与者都进行了一项 CI 训练任务,其中包括 7 个不同的形状系列(直线、椭圆、团块状、方形、螺旋状、圆形和字母),共进行了多达 40 次训练。在训练任务中对两个参数进行了处理:方向抖动(OJ,即每个形状的单个 Gabor 元素与理想方向的偏差)和诱导数(IN,即定义形状的 Gabor 元素的数量)。结果显示,OJ阈值和IN阈值在各组之间存在显著差异,对照组的OJ阈值更高,IN阈值更低。此外,我们还发现,轮廓形状的函数也会产生明显的影响,当轮廓被认为更复杂时,组间的差异就会出现,例如,由于具有更高的曲率(Blobs、Spiral、Letters),组间的差异就会出现。这些数据可以为今后研究精神分裂症患者视觉整合障碍的工作提供参考。
{"title":"Performance on a contour integration task as a function of contour shape in schizophrenia and controls","authors":"Samyukta Jayakumar ,&nbsp;Anthony O. Ahmed ,&nbsp;Pamela D. Butler ,&nbsp;Steven M. Silverstein ,&nbsp;Judy L. Thompson ,&nbsp;Aaron R. Seitz","doi":"10.1016/j.visres.2024.108394","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2024.108394","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Contour Integration (CI) is the ability to integrate elemental features into objects and is a basic visual process essential for object perception and recognition, and for functioning in visual environments. It is now well documented that people with schizophrenia (SZ), in addition to having cognitive impairments, also have several visual perceptual deficits, including in CI. Here, we retrospectively characterize the performance of both SZ and neurotypical individuals (NT) on a series of contour shapes, made up of Gabor elements, that varied in terms of closure and curvature. Participants in both groups performed a CI training task that included 7 different families of shapes (Lines, Ellipse, Blobs, Squiggles, Spiral, Circle and Letters) for up to 40 sessions. Two parameters were manipulated in the training task: Orientation Jitter (OJ, i.e., orientation deviations of individual Gabor elements from ideal for each shape) and Inducer Number (IN, i.e., number of Gabor elements defining the shape). Results show that both OJ and IN thresholds significantly differed between the groups, with higher (OJ) and lower (IN) thresholds observed in the controls. Furthermore, we found significant effects as a function of the contour shapes, with differences between groups emerging with contours that were considered more complex, e.g., due to having a higher degree of curvature (Blobs, Spiral, Letters). These data can inform future work that aims to characterize visual integration impairments in schizophrenia.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":23670,"journal":{"name":"Vision Research","volume":"219 ","pages":"Article 108394"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-04-04","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0042698924000385/pdfft?md5=4a495f98beed8e5a7bcfcb8075d65211&pid=1-s2.0-S0042698924000385-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140344254","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
A new mouse-fixation device for IOP measurement in awake mice 用于测量清醒小鼠眼压的新型小鼠固定装置
IF 1.8 4区 心理学 Q4 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-04-04 DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2024.108397
Jiaxin Guo , Jialiang Yang , Haotian Huang , Kai Wu , Xiawei Wu , Haolin Wang , Yu He , Fanfeng Bi , Chenyazhuo Hu , Bo Gong , Houbin Zhang

Glaucoma is an irreversible blinding eye disease. The mechanisms underlying glaucoma are complex. Up to now, no successful remedy has been found to completely cure the condition. High intraocular pressure (IOP) is an established risk factor for glaucoma and the only known modifiable factor for glaucoma treatment. Mice have been widely used to study glaucoma pathogenesis. IOP measurement is an important tool for monitoring the potential development of glaucomatous phenotypes in glaucoma mouse models. Currently, there are two methods of IOP measurement in mice: invasive and non-invasive. As the invasive method can cause corneal damage and inflammation, and most of the noninvasive method involves the use of anesthetics. In the course of our research, we designed a mouse fixation device to facilitate non-invasive measurements of mouse IOPs. Using this device, mouse IOPs can be accurately measured in awake mice. This device will help researchers to accurately assess mouse IOP without the use of anesthetics.

青光眼是一种不可逆的致盲性眼病。青光眼的发病机制十分复杂。迄今为止,还没有找到能够彻底治愈这种疾病的成功疗法。高眼压是青光眼的既定风险因素,也是治疗青光眼的唯一已知可调节因素。小鼠已被广泛用于研究青光眼的发病机制。眼压测量是监测青光眼小鼠模型中青光眼表型潜在发展的重要工具。目前,小鼠眼压测量有两种方法:有创和无创。由于侵入式方法会造成角膜损伤和炎症,而大多数非侵入式方法需要使用麻醉剂。在研究过程中,我们设计了一种小鼠固定装置,以方便对小鼠眼压进行无创测量。使用该装置,可以准确测量清醒小鼠的眼压。该装置将帮助研究人员在不使用麻醉剂的情况下准确评估小鼠的眼压。
{"title":"A new mouse-fixation device for IOP measurement in awake mice","authors":"Jiaxin Guo ,&nbsp;Jialiang Yang ,&nbsp;Haotian Huang ,&nbsp;Kai Wu ,&nbsp;Xiawei Wu ,&nbsp;Haolin Wang ,&nbsp;Yu He ,&nbsp;Fanfeng Bi ,&nbsp;Chenyazhuo Hu ,&nbsp;Bo Gong ,&nbsp;Houbin Zhang","doi":"10.1016/j.visres.2024.108397","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2024.108397","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Glaucoma is an irreversible blinding eye disease. The mechanisms underlying glaucoma are complex. Up to now, no successful remedy has been found to completely cure the condition. High intraocular pressure (IOP) is an established risk factor for glaucoma and the only known modifiable factor for glaucoma treatment. Mice have been widely used to study glaucoma pathogenesis. IOP measurement is an important tool for monitoring the potential development of glaucomatous phenotypes in glaucoma mouse models. Currently, there are two methods of IOP measurement in mice: invasive and non-invasive. As the invasive method can cause corneal damage and inflammation, and most of the noninvasive method involves the use of anesthetics. In the course of our research, we designed a mouse fixation device to facilitate non-invasive measurements of mouse IOPs. Using this device, mouse IOPs can be accurately measured in awake mice. This device will help researchers to accurately assess mouse IOP without the use of anesthetics.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":23670,"journal":{"name":"Vision Research","volume":"219 ","pages":"Article 108397"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-04-04","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140344229","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Sustaining attention in visuomotor timing is associated with location-based binding 视觉运动计时中的持续注意与位置绑定有关
IF 1.8 4区 心理学 Q4 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-04-03 DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2024.108405
Yingyu Huang , Liying Zhan , Shengqi Zhong , Mi Sun , Chaolun Wang , Chengbin Yang , Xiang Wu

Maintaining focus of attention over prolonged periods can be challenging, especially when the target stimulus is absent from the temporal sequence. Prior research has shown that a temporal attentional cue filling in the temporal blank can improve sustained attention: in a sustained visual attention task requiring synchronizing finger tapping with a temporally regular sequence composed of brief flash disks interleaved with blank periods, task performance was improved when a continuous fixation point that served as a temporal attentional cue was presented superimposed on the disk stimulus. To test the hypothesis that binding the temporal attentional cue with the target temporal sequence by spatial overlapping is crucial for enhancing sustained attention, the present study conducted a series of three experiments that deconstructed the bound connection between the cue and the sequence stimulus. In Experiment 1, the cue was placed above or below a flash disk. In Experiment 2, the cue was between two vertically arranged flash disks. In Experiment 3, the cue was in a flash ring. No significant effect of sustained attention improvement was found in any of the three experiments. Experiment 4 further replicated these null results and the previously observed effect of sustained attention improvement when the temporal cue was superimposed on the sequence stimulus. Our finding demonstrates that binding by spatial overlapping during the temporal blank when the sequence stimulus is absent is critical for enhancing sustained attention, which should be beneficial for improving performance across a broader range of tasks that require prolonged maintenance of attention.

长时间保持注意力集中是一项挑战,尤其是当目标刺激在时间序列中缺失时。先前的研究表明,填补时间空白的时间注意线索可以提高持续注意:在一项持续视觉注意任务中,要求手指敲击与由短暂闪烁的圆盘和空白期交错组成的时间规律序列同步,当作为时间注意线索的连续固定点叠加在圆盘刺激上时,任务表现会有所提高。为了验证这样一个假设,即通过空间重叠将时间注意线索与目标时间序列结合在一起对增强持续注意至关重要,本研究进行了一系列共三个实验,以解构线索与序列刺激之间的绑定联系。在实验 1 中,提示被放置在闪盘的上方或下方。在实验 2 中,提示位于两个垂直排列的闪光盘之间。在实验 3 中,提示位于一个闪光环中。在这三个实验中,都没有发现持续注意力改善的明显效果。当时间线索叠加在序列刺激上时,实验 4 进一步复制了这些无效结果以及之前观察到的持续注意力改善效果。我们的研究结果表明,当序列刺激缺失时,在时空空白期间通过空间重叠进行绑定对于增强持续注意力至关重要,这应该有利于提高需要长时间保持注意力的更广泛任务的成绩。
{"title":"Sustaining attention in visuomotor timing is associated with location-based binding","authors":"Yingyu Huang ,&nbsp;Liying Zhan ,&nbsp;Shengqi Zhong ,&nbsp;Mi Sun ,&nbsp;Chaolun Wang ,&nbsp;Chengbin Yang ,&nbsp;Xiang Wu","doi":"10.1016/j.visres.2024.108405","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2024.108405","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Maintaining focus of attention over prolonged periods can be challenging, especially when the target stimulus is absent from the temporal sequence. Prior research has shown that a temporal attentional cue filling in the temporal blank can improve sustained attention: in a sustained visual attention task requiring synchronizing finger tapping with a temporally regular sequence composed of brief flash disks interleaved with blank periods, task performance was improved when a continuous fixation point that served as a temporal attentional cue was presented superimposed on the disk stimulus. To test the hypothesis that binding the temporal attentional cue with the target temporal sequence by spatial overlapping is crucial for enhancing sustained attention, the present study conducted a series of three experiments that deconstructed the bound connection between the cue and the sequence stimulus. In Experiment 1, the cue was placed above or below a flash disk. In Experiment 2, the cue was between two vertically arranged flash disks. In Experiment 3, the cue was in a flash ring. No significant effect of sustained attention improvement was found in any of the three experiments. Experiment 4 further replicated these null results and the previously observed effect of sustained attention improvement when the temporal cue was superimposed on the sequence stimulus. Our finding demonstrates that binding by spatial overlapping during the temporal blank when the sequence stimulus is absent is critical for enhancing sustained attention, which should be beneficial for improving performance across a broader range of tasks that require prolonged maintenance of attention.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":23670,"journal":{"name":"Vision Research","volume":"219 ","pages":"Article 108405"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-04-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140342240","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Vision Research
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1