首页 > 最新文献

Writing Systems Research最新文献

英文 中文
Introduction to this special issue: Reading morphologically complex words in a second language 本期专题简介:用第二语言阅读词形复杂的单词
Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2015-07-03 DOI: 10.1080/17586801.2014.988109
Min Wang, L. Verhoeven
In this age of globalisation, the need for communication in a second or foreign language has dramatically increased. Second language (L2) reading and literacy education has become one of the most important components of our educational system. Learning to read is essentially learning to map between phonological, orthographic and semantic information in written words. The key development in learning to read is the improvement of the qualities of orthographic, phonological and semantic representations of given words, and learning to read in L2 is no exception. This Special Issue is focused on one important specific aspect of L2 reading, namely, reading morphologically complex L2 words. Morphology represents an important interface between phonology, orthography and semantics. Previous L2 reading research has been largely devoted to phonological processes in reading single-morpheme words. Relatively fewer studies have investigated reading of morphologically complex words in L2. How morphologically complex words are represented and processed has been one of the central questions in first language (L1) reading literature. Recent research has shown support for interactive models that propose a direct lexical route involving access to full-form representations of the complex words along with a parsing route that allows decomposition of constituent morphemes. According to those interactive models, the mental lexicon is the storehouse for both complex words and their constituent morphemes. The meaning of a complex word can be accessed either directly or by an analysis of the meanings of its constituent morphemes. The analysis approach is affected by the features of an individual complex word and its constituent morphemes, such as frequency of both the whole word and its constituent morphemes and semantic transparency in terms of the relation between the whole word and its constituent morphemes. Individual differences in reading skill have recently been shown to play a role in the reliance of morpho-orthographic information in reading. Research to date has focused heavily on reading morphologically complex words in monolingual populations, while less is known about reading complex words by L2 readers, both in developing children and adults. Within the very limited literature on L2 reading of morphologically complex words, there is some debate on whether L2 learners are able to represent and process morphological information as much as L1 readers.
在这个全球化的时代,用第二语言或外语进行交流的需求急剧增加。第二语言阅读和读写教育已经成为我们教育体系中最重要的组成部分之一。学习阅读本质上就是学习在书面文字的音韵、正字法和语义信息之间进行映射。学习阅读的关键发展是提高给定单词的正字法、语音和语义表征的质量,学习二语阅读也不例外。这期特刊关注的是二语阅读的一个重要的具体方面,即阅读形态复杂的二语词汇。形态学是音系学、正字法和语义学之间的重要接口。以往的二语阅读研究主要集中在单语素词汇阅读中的语音过程。相对而言,对二语中词形复杂词汇阅读的研究较少。词形复杂的词汇如何被表征和处理一直是母语阅读文献中的核心问题之一。最近的研究显示了对交互式模型的支持,该模型提出了一种直接的词汇路径,包括访问复杂单词的完整形式表示,以及允许分解组成语素的解析路径。根据这些交互模型,心理词汇既是复杂词的仓库,也是构成词的语素的仓库。一个复杂词的意思既可以直接获得,也可以通过分析其组成语素的意思来获得。这种分析方法受单个复杂词及其组成语素的特征的影响,如全词及其组成语素的出现频率、全词及其组成语素关系的语义透明度等。阅读技能的个体差异最近被证明在阅读中对词形正字法信息的依赖中起作用。迄今为止的研究主要集中在单语人群中阅读词形复杂的单词,而对发展中儿童和成人的二语读者阅读复杂单词的研究却很少。在非常有限的关于词形复杂词汇的二语阅读的文献中,关于二语学习者是否能够像母语读者一样多地表征和处理词形信息存在一些争论。
{"title":"Introduction to this special issue: Reading morphologically complex words in a second language","authors":"Min Wang, L. Verhoeven","doi":"10.1080/17586801.2014.988109","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/17586801.2014.988109","url":null,"abstract":"In this age of globalisation, the need for communication in a second or foreign language has dramatically increased. Second language (L2) reading and literacy education has become one of the most important components of our educational system. Learning to read is essentially learning to map between phonological, orthographic and semantic information in written words. The key development in learning to read is the improvement of the qualities of orthographic, phonological and semantic representations of given words, and learning to read in L2 is no exception. This Special Issue is focused on one important specific aspect of L2 reading, namely, reading morphologically complex L2 words. Morphology represents an important interface between phonology, orthography and semantics. Previous L2 reading research has been largely devoted to phonological processes in reading single-morpheme words. Relatively fewer studies have investigated reading of morphologically complex words in L2. How morphologically complex words are represented and processed has been one of the central questions in first language (L1) reading literature. Recent research has shown support for interactive models that propose a direct lexical route involving access to full-form representations of the complex words along with a parsing route that allows decomposition of constituent morphemes. According to those interactive models, the mental lexicon is the storehouse for both complex words and their constituent morphemes. The meaning of a complex word can be accessed either directly or by an analysis of the meanings of its constituent morphemes. The analysis approach is affected by the features of an individual complex word and its constituent morphemes, such as frequency of both the whole word and its constituent morphemes and semantic transparency in terms of the relation between the whole word and its constituent morphemes. Individual differences in reading skill have recently been shown to play a role in the reliance of morpho-orthographic information in reading. Research to date has focused heavily on reading morphologically complex words in monolingual populations, while less is known about reading complex words by L2 readers, both in developing children and adults. Within the very limited literature on L2 reading of morphologically complex words, there is some debate on whether L2 learners are able to represent and process morphological information as much as L1 readers.","PeriodicalId":39225,"journal":{"name":"Writing Systems Research","volume":"7 1","pages":"129 - 132"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2015-07-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1080/17586801.2014.988109","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"60438149","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Exploring the effects of word features on French immersion children's ability to deconstruct morphologically complex words 探讨单词特征对法语浸泡儿童解构复杂词形单词能力的影响
Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2015-07-03 DOI: 10.1080/17586801.2014.943645
Kathleen Hipfner-Boucher, Katie Lam, Xi Chen, S. Deacon
The present study investigated factors influencing the ability to decompose multimorphemic words in French in non-francophone children educated in French. In particular, we focused on the effects of two word features: English-French cognate status and base frequency. We also examined the effect of child language background (English first language (EL1) versus English second language (ELL)) on performance. In two related studies, children in grades 1 to 3 completed a translation task requiring them to match morphologically complex words in French and English. Target words were manipulated with respect to cognate status and base frequency. Overall, performance was found to improve over time and to be influenced by cognate status and word frequency. Across all grades, EL1 and ELL children were comparable on task performance. Taken together, these results suggest that French immersion students' ability to deconstruct words and extract morphemes in French is influenced by the presence of cognates, as well as base frequency.
本研究旨在探讨法语教育下的非法语儿童分解法语多形词能力的影响因素。我们特别关注了两个单词特征的影响:英语-法语同源状态和基频。我们还研究了儿童语言背景(英语第一语言(EL1)和英语第二语言(ELL))对表现的影响。在两个相关的研究中,一到三年级的孩子完成了一项翻译任务,要求他们匹配法语和英语中词形复杂的单词。目标词在同源状态和基频方面被操纵。总的来说,人们的表现会随着时间的推移而提高,并受到同源词状态和词频的影响。在所有年级中,一年级和二年级儿童在任务表现上具有可比性。综上所述,这些结果表明,法语浸入式学生在法语中解构单词和提取语素的能力受到同源词和基频的影响。
{"title":"Exploring the effects of word features on French immersion children's ability to deconstruct morphologically complex words","authors":"Kathleen Hipfner-Boucher, Katie Lam, Xi Chen, S. Deacon","doi":"10.1080/17586801.2014.943645","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/17586801.2014.943645","url":null,"abstract":"The present study investigated factors influencing the ability to decompose multimorphemic words in French in non-francophone children educated in French. In particular, we focused on the effects of two word features: English-French cognate status and base frequency. We also examined the effect of child language background (English first language (EL1) versus English second language (ELL)) on performance. In two related studies, children in grades 1 to 3 completed a translation task requiring them to match morphologically complex words in French and English. Target words were manipulated with respect to cognate status and base frequency. Overall, performance was found to improve over time and to be influenced by cognate status and word frequency. Across all grades, EL1 and ELL children were comparable on task performance. Taken together, these results suggest that French immersion students' ability to deconstruct words and extract morphemes in French is influenced by the presence of cognates, as well as base frequency.","PeriodicalId":39225,"journal":{"name":"Writing Systems Research","volume":"7 1","pages":"157 - 168"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2015-07-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1080/17586801.2014.943645","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"60438097","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 3
Sensitivity to morpheme units in English as L2 word recognition 英语语素单位敏感性作为第二语言单词识别
Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2015-07-03 DOI: 10.1080/17586801.2014.976165
S. Casalis, Eva Commissaire, Lynne G. Duncan
Little is yet known about how L2 learners process morphology during visual word recognition. Two points of view may be contrasted: the first one suggests that L2 learners, as less proficient speakers, may be less sensitive to the computational aspects of word processing such as the morphological structure of complex words, relying more on lexical information; whereas, the second one suggests that word processing is constrained mainly by linguistic aspects, making L2 learners as sensitive to word structure as native speakers. While previous studies have mainly focused on proficient to highly proficient L2 speakers, the present study compared L2 learners of low proficiency with those of intermediate to high levels of proficiency. The role of morphological structure in word recognition and pseudoword processing was examined by manipulating the presence of embedded words and suffixes in items presented for L2 lexical decision. Contrasting patterns in L2 word recognition were observed between groups as the low-proficiency group was more sensitive to the presence of an embedded word than the higher proficiency group in both accuracy and speed. However, pseudowords made up of an embedded word and suffix were significantly more likely to be wrongly accepted as words than other pseudowords by both groups. Furthermore, correct rejection of these items as words induced longer latencies in both groups, indicating a morphological analysis of these pseudowords. Together, the results show that L2 learners, including those who are low in proficiency, are sensitive to the morphological structure of written L2 words.
关于二语学习者在视觉单词识别过程中如何处理词法,我们所知甚少。可以对比两种观点:第一种观点认为,二语学习者作为不太熟练的说话者,可能对文字处理的计算方面(如复杂单词的形态结构)不太敏感,更多地依赖词汇信息;然而,第二种观点认为,文字处理主要受到语言方面的限制,这使得二语学习者对单词结构的敏感度与母语人士一样高。虽然以前的研究主要集中在熟练到高度熟练的二语使用者,但本研究比较了低熟练程度的二语学习者和中高熟练程度的二语学习者。通过操纵二语词汇决策项目中嵌入词和后缀的存在,研究了词形结构在词识别和伪词处理中的作用。在二语单词识别方面,两组之间的对比模式是,低熟练程度组对嵌入词的存在在准确性和速度上比高熟练程度组更敏感。然而,由嵌入词和后缀组成的假词比其他假词更容易被两组人错误地接受。此外,对这些词的正确拒绝在两组中引起了更长的潜伏期,这表明对这些假词进行了形态学分析。综上所述,结果表明,二语学习者,包括那些熟练程度较低的学习者,对二语书面词汇的形态结构都很敏感。
{"title":"Sensitivity to morpheme units in English as L2 word recognition","authors":"S. Casalis, Eva Commissaire, Lynne G. Duncan","doi":"10.1080/17586801.2014.976165","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/17586801.2014.976165","url":null,"abstract":"Little is yet known about how L2 learners process morphology during visual word recognition. Two points of view may be contrasted: the first one suggests that L2 learners, as less proficient speakers, may be less sensitive to the computational aspects of word processing such as the morphological structure of complex words, relying more on lexical information; whereas, the second one suggests that word processing is constrained mainly by linguistic aspects, making L2 learners as sensitive to word structure as native speakers. While previous studies have mainly focused on proficient to highly proficient L2 speakers, the present study compared L2 learners of low proficiency with those of intermediate to high levels of proficiency. The role of morphological structure in word recognition and pseudoword processing was examined by manipulating the presence of embedded words and suffixes in items presented for L2 lexical decision. Contrasting patterns in L2 word recognition were observed between groups as the low-proficiency group was more sensitive to the presence of an embedded word than the higher proficiency group in both accuracy and speed. However, pseudowords made up of an embedded word and suffix were significantly more likely to be wrongly accepted as words than other pseudowords by both groups. Furthermore, correct rejection of these items as words induced longer latencies in both groups, indicating a morphological analysis of these pseudowords. Together, the results show that L2 learners, including those who are low in proficiency, are sensitive to the morphological structure of written L2 words.","PeriodicalId":39225,"journal":{"name":"Writing Systems Research","volume":"7 1","pages":"186 - 201"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2015-07-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1080/17586801.2014.976165","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"60438111","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 13
Young children's knowledge about the spatial layout of writing. 幼儿对书写空间布局的认识。
Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2015-07-01 DOI: 10.1080/17586801.2014.924386
Rebecca Treiman, Kevin Mulqueeny, Brett Kessler

Children who are knowledgeable about the basic properties of writing when formal literacy instruction begins are better prepared to benefit from that instruction than children who know less about this topic. In the present study, we examined U.S. preschoolers' knowledge about one aspect of writing: its spatial arrangement. Our participants, who had a mean age of 4 years, 2 months and who could not read any words in a list of simple words, were significantly above the level of chance at determining that horizontally arranged strings of letters are more like the writing in books than are letters with vertical, diagonal, or scattered arrangements. Contrary to the theory that children learn about the characteristics of writing that hold true in all writing systems before they learn about the characteristics that are specific to their own writing system, young children did not show a priority for vertical arrangements. The results are more consistent with the hypothesis that preschoolers apply their statistical learning skills to the spatial layout of writing.

当正式的识字教育开始时,那些对写作的基本特性有所了解的孩子比那些对这一主题知之甚少的孩子更能从这种教育中受益。在本研究中,我们调查了美国学龄前儿童对写作的一个方面的知识:它的空间安排。我们的参与者平均年龄为4岁零2个月,他们不会阅读简单单词列表中的任何单词,但在确定水平排列的字母串比垂直、对角线或分散排列的字母更像书中的书写方面,他们的机会明显高于这个水平。有一种理论认为,儿童在学习所有书写系统的特征之前,都会先学习适用于他们自己的书写系统的特征,与此相反,幼儿并没有表现出对垂直排列的优先次序。结果更符合假设,即学龄前儿童将他们的统计学习技能应用于写作的空间布局。
{"title":"Young children's knowledge about the spatial layout of writing.","authors":"Rebecca Treiman,&nbsp;Kevin Mulqueeny,&nbsp;Brett Kessler","doi":"10.1080/17586801.2014.924386","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/17586801.2014.924386","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Children who are knowledgeable about the basic properties of writing when formal literacy instruction begins are better prepared to benefit from that instruction than children who know less about this topic. In the present study, we examined U.S. preschoolers' knowledge about one aspect of writing: its spatial arrangement. Our participants, who had a mean age of 4 years, 2 months and who could not read any words in a list of simple words, were significantly above the level of chance at determining that horizontally arranged strings of letters are more like the writing in books than are letters with vertical, diagonal, or scattered arrangements. Contrary to the theory that children learn about the characteristics of writing that hold true in all writing systems before they learn about the characteristics that are specific to their own writing system, young children did not show a priority for vertical arrangements. The results are more consistent with the hypothesis that preschoolers apply their statistical learning skills to the spatial layout of writing.</p>","PeriodicalId":39225,"journal":{"name":"Writing Systems Research","volume":"7 2","pages":"235-244"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2015-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1080/17586801.2014.924386","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"34067993","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 13
Who are adolescents and adults who develop literacy for the first time in an L2, and why are they of research interest? 谁是第一次在第二语言中发展读写能力的青少年和成年人?为什么对他们有研究兴趣?
Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2015-01-02 DOI: 10.1080/17586801.2015.998443
M. Young-Scholten
This Special Issue concerns a special population of second-language learners: post-puberty second-language (L2) learners who are learning to read and write for the first time in their lives, in the second language they are in the process of acquiring. They are among the roughly 781 million adult illiterates worldwide (UNESCO, 2014), and among those who emigrate from politically instable and/or impoverished regions of the world to highly literate post-industrialised societies. Theirs is a unique learning situation. Children learning to read in their home language begin with a language they know. They know the phonology, morphology and syntax of that language, and they know most if not all of the words they come across in reading materials designed for them. Their linguistic competence confers a range of advantages at the initial stages of reading. They are able to develop metalinguistic knowledge and skills to facilitate learning to read based on their linguistic competence. Attempts to guess words can be based on predictions of the next consonant in a cluster or of a possible word following a transitive verb. Much has been written about children’s reading development. Much has also been written about the development of reading by second-language learners, including those learning to read in a writing system different from their native language (Koda, 2005). The older learners studied are invariably educated and can already read and write in their native language. The younger learners studied may be foreign language learners in school or immigrant children who, like their older siblings and parents, need to learn to read in a language different from their home language. Here, too, there is a good amount of research (e.g., Gersten & Geva, 2003; Grigorenko & Takanishi, 2009). Older uneducated and low-educated L2 learners have long been neglected by researchers, despite the fact that more now fit the profile of the uninstructed migrant workers in northern Europe who in the 1970s and 1980s attracted considerable research attention (see Young-Scholten, 2013). The amount of research on the language and literacy development of older non-/low-educated L2 learners is surprisingly small. Increased basic research on both the internal and external factors which correlate with language and literacy success for these severely disadvantaged adults would yield evidence on which to
本期特刊关注的是一群特殊的第二语言学习者:青春期后的第二语言学习者,他们正处于学习第二语言的过程中,这是他们一生中第一次学习阅读和写作。他们属于全球约7.81亿成年文盲(联合国教科文组织,2014年),属于从世界上政治不稳定和/或贫困地区移民到识字程度较高的后工业化社会的人。他们的学习环境很独特。孩子们从他们熟悉的语言开始学习母语阅读。他们知道这种语言的音韵、词法和句法,他们知道为他们设计的阅读材料中遇到的大部分单词,如果不是全部的话。他们的语言能力在阅读的最初阶段赋予了他们一系列的优势。他们能够发展元语言知识和技能,以促进基于他们语言能力的阅读学习。猜测单词的方法可以基于对集群中下一个辅音的预测,也可以基于对及物动词后面可能出现的单词的预测。关于儿童阅读发展的文章很多。关于第二语言学习者的阅读发展也有很多文章,包括那些学习用不同于母语的书写系统阅读的人(Koda, 2005)。被研究的年龄较大的学习者无一例外都受过教育,已经能用母语读写。被研究的年轻学习者可能是学校里的外语学习者,也可能是像他们的哥哥姐姐和父母一样需要学习用不同于母语的语言阅读的移民儿童。在这方面,也有大量的研究(例如,Gersten & Geva, 2003;Grigorenko & Takanishi, 2009)。年龄较大的未受教育和受教育程度较低的第二语言学习者长期以来一直被研究人员所忽视,尽管现在有更多的人符合北欧未受教育的移民工人的形象,他们在20世纪70年代和80年代引起了相当大的研究关注(见Young-Scholten, 2013)。关于年龄较大的非/低教育程度的第二语言学习者的语言和读写能力发展的研究数量少得惊人。加强对与这些严重处境不利的成年人的语言和读写能力成功相关的内部和外部因素的基础研究,将提供证据
{"title":"Who are adolescents and adults who develop literacy for the first time in an L2, and why are they of research interest?","authors":"M. Young-Scholten","doi":"10.1080/17586801.2015.998443","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/17586801.2015.998443","url":null,"abstract":"This Special Issue concerns a special population of second-language learners: post-puberty second-language (L2) learners who are learning to read and write for the first time in their lives, in the second language they are in the process of acquiring. They are among the roughly 781 million adult illiterates worldwide (UNESCO, 2014), and among those who emigrate from politically instable and/or impoverished regions of the world to highly literate post-industrialised societies. Theirs is a unique learning situation. Children learning to read in their home language begin with a language they know. They know the phonology, morphology and syntax of that language, and they know most if not all of the words they come across in reading materials designed for them. Their linguistic competence confers a range of advantages at the initial stages of reading. They are able to develop metalinguistic knowledge and skills to facilitate learning to read based on their linguistic competence. Attempts to guess words can be based on predictions of the next consonant in a cluster or of a possible word following a transitive verb. Much has been written about children’s reading development. Much has also been written about the development of reading by second-language learners, including those learning to read in a writing system different from their native language (Koda, 2005). The older learners studied are invariably educated and can already read and write in their native language. The younger learners studied may be foreign language learners in school or immigrant children who, like their older siblings and parents, need to learn to read in a language different from their home language. Here, too, there is a good amount of research (e.g., Gersten & Geva, 2003; Grigorenko & Takanishi, 2009). Older uneducated and low-educated L2 learners have long been neglected by researchers, despite the fact that more now fit the profile of the uninstructed migrant workers in northern Europe who in the 1970s and 1980s attracted considerable research attention (see Young-Scholten, 2013). The amount of research on the language and literacy development of older non-/low-educated L2 learners is surprisingly small. Increased basic research on both the internal and external factors which correlate with language and literacy success for these severely disadvantaged adults would yield evidence on which to","PeriodicalId":39225,"journal":{"name":"Writing Systems Research","volume":"7 1","pages":"1 - 3"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2015-01-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1080/17586801.2015.998443","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"60438261","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 14
The simultaneous development of receptive skills in an orthographically transparent second language 在拼写透明的第二语言中同时发展接受技能
Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2015-01-02 DOI: 10.1080/17586801.2014.943148
T. Tammelin-Laine, Maisa Martin
Learning to read in an orthographically very shallow language may seem easy. However, for adults who are non-literate in their first language (L1), have no experience of formal education, and have to acquire literacy in a new language (L2), learning to read at all can be a formidable task. In this article, the results of a case study of the outcome of the first 10 months of Finnish literacy training for five immigrant women (24–45 years of age) are presented. Relationships are sought between the participants' achieved reading skills, their oral receptive vocabulary, their knowledge of letters, their phonological working memory and their visual memory. The results of the study show that even when grapheme-phoneme correspondences are quite regular, mere knowledge of the letters is not enough to trigger their ability to blend sounds. Vocabulary skills are necessary for functional reading but may be difficult to acquire if the phonological working memory is poor. Deficiencies in learners' mental representations of word patterns make it difficult for them to connect strings of letters to words. A good visual memory may help with vocabulary acquisition but this too first requires cracking the alphabetic code. The main conclusion of this first study of Finnish L2 literacy acquisition by non-literate adults is that multiple pedagogical approaches are required to accommodate the needs of these learners.
学习用一种拼写非常浅显的语言阅读似乎很容易。然而,对于母语(第一语言)不识字、没有受过正规教育的成年人来说,学习阅读是一项艰巨的任务,他们必须掌握一门新语言(第二语言)的读写能力。本文介绍了对五名移民妇女(24-45岁)进行芬兰扫盲培训前10个月的结果进行个案研究的结果。研究人员寻找了参与者的阅读技巧、口头接受性词汇、字母知识、语音工作记忆和视觉记忆之间的关系。研究结果表明,即使字母和音素的对应关系非常规则,仅仅了解字母也不足以激发他们混合声音的能力。词汇技能对于功能性阅读是必要的,但如果语音工作记忆较差,词汇技能可能难以获得。学习者对单词模式的心理表征存在缺陷,这使得他们很难将字母串与单词联系起来。良好的视觉记忆可能有助于词汇习得,但这也首先需要破解字母密码。这项关于非识字成人芬兰语第二语言读写能力习得的第一项研究的主要结论是,需要多种教学方法来适应这些学习者的需求。
{"title":"The simultaneous development of receptive skills in an orthographically transparent second language","authors":"T. Tammelin-Laine, Maisa Martin","doi":"10.1080/17586801.2014.943148","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/17586801.2014.943148","url":null,"abstract":"Learning to read in an orthographically very shallow language may seem easy. However, for adults who are non-literate in their first language (L1), have no experience of formal education, and have to acquire literacy in a new language (L2), learning to read at all can be a formidable task. In this article, the results of a case study of the outcome of the first 10 months of Finnish literacy training for five immigrant women (24–45 years of age) are presented. Relationships are sought between the participants' achieved reading skills, their oral receptive vocabulary, their knowledge of letters, their phonological working memory and their visual memory. The results of the study show that even when grapheme-phoneme correspondences are quite regular, mere knowledge of the letters is not enough to trigger their ability to blend sounds. Vocabulary skills are necessary for functional reading but may be difficult to acquire if the phonological working memory is poor. Deficiencies in learners' mental representations of word patterns make it difficult for them to connect strings of letters to words. A good visual memory may help with vocabulary acquisition but this too first requires cracking the alphabetic code. The main conclusion of this first study of Finnish L2 literacy acquisition by non-literate adults is that multiple pedagogical approaches are required to accommodate the needs of these learners.","PeriodicalId":39225,"journal":{"name":"Writing Systems Research","volume":"7 1","pages":"39 - 57"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2015-01-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1080/17586801.2014.943148","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"60437943","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 12
Processing costs when reading short message service shortcuts: An eye-tracking study 阅读短信服务快捷方式时的处理成本:一项眼球追踪研究
Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2015-01-02 DOI: 10.1080/17586801.2014.943150
Sinéad McCausland, J. Kingston, F. Lyddy
Short Message Service (SMS) messaging is associated with a variety of ‘textisms’, shortened words and other nonstandard spellings. Two such shortcuts, orthographic (e.g., deleting vowels) and phonological (e.g., sound-based respellings) abbreviations, have been associated with negative effects on reading in adults. The current study used eye tracking to investigate reading of English SMS messages with and without such shortcuts. Thirty young adults read sentences containing phonological, orthographic or mixed textisms, along with standard English sentences. Reading time, average fixation duration and total fixation count were measured. Sentences in standard English were associated with significantly faster reading times compared to the three textism conditions, with no difference between the textism types. Fewer fixations were made on standard English sentences and on sentences with phonological textisms. There was no association between the reading measures and participants' use of textisms, texting frequency or keypad preference. Although the processing cost was modest, the results support the view that sentences with texting shortcuts are harder to read than standard sentences in English.
短消息服务(SMS)与各种各样的“文本”、缩短的单词和其他非标准拼写有关。两种这样的捷径,正字法缩写(例如,删除元音)和语音缩写(例如,基于声音的重新拼写),对成年人的阅读产生了负面影响。目前的研究使用眼动追踪来调查有或没有这种快捷方式的英文短信阅读。30名年轻人阅读包含音韵学、正字法或混合文本的句子,以及标准的英语句子。测量阅读时间、平均注视时间和总注视次数。与三种文本主义条件相比,标准英语句子的阅读速度明显更快,而文本主义类型之间没有差异。对标准英语句子和具有语音文本的句子的关注较少。阅读测量与参与者使用文本、发短信频率或键盘偏好之间没有关联。虽然处理成本不高,但结果支持这样的观点,即带有短信快捷方式的句子比标准英语句子更难阅读。
{"title":"Processing costs when reading short message service shortcuts: An eye-tracking study","authors":"Sinéad McCausland, J. Kingston, F. Lyddy","doi":"10.1080/17586801.2014.943150","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/17586801.2014.943150","url":null,"abstract":"Short Message Service (SMS) messaging is associated with a variety of ‘textisms’, shortened words and other nonstandard spellings. Two such shortcuts, orthographic (e.g., deleting vowels) and phonological (e.g., sound-based respellings) abbreviations, have been associated with negative effects on reading in adults. The current study used eye tracking to investigate reading of English SMS messages with and without such shortcuts. Thirty young adults read sentences containing phonological, orthographic or mixed textisms, along with standard English sentences. Reading time, average fixation duration and total fixation count were measured. Sentences in standard English were associated with significantly faster reading times compared to the three textism conditions, with no difference between the textism types. Fewer fixations were made on standard English sentences and on sentences with phonological textisms. There was no association between the reading measures and participants' use of textisms, texting frequency or keypad preference. Although the processing cost was modest, the results support the view that sentences with texting shortcuts are harder to read than standard sentences in English.","PeriodicalId":39225,"journal":{"name":"Writing Systems Research","volume":"7 1","pages":"107 - 97"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2015-01-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1080/17586801.2014.943150","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"60438067","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 3
Monolingual and multilingual discrimination of written sequences' readability 书写序列可读性的单语和多语判别
Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2015-01-02 DOI: 10.1080/17586801.2014.898574
A. Stavans
Multilingual literate landscapes are ubiquitous input for children in many places in the world. This type of input (albeit visual only) may propel literacy awareness, integration and cognitive assimilation of different writing and notational systems even before schooling. This study explores quantitatively and qualitatively the ways in which young multilingual children understand and interpret the principles underlying different writing systems. The focus is to compare how bilingual and monolingual children judge ‘readable and non-readable’ representations which are alphabetic or non-alphabetic (single, other or mixed); and whether readable strings comply with a qualitative and quantitative condition assigned to the string of signs presented. There are similarities as well as differences in the distinction as ‘readable’ between alphabetic and non-alphabetic notations among bilingual Ethiopian children and monolingual non-Ethiopian children who are pre-readers. Both groups regard as ‘readable’ sequences that contain varied and multiple combinations of alphabetic signs. There are revealing differences between the groups as to the quantity of signs in the sequence and whether it comes from a single, familiar or mixed alphabet. The Ethiopian bilingual children are more inclined to regard different alphabetic systems—whether they combine signs from within a single alphabet or from multiple alphabets—and tend to ‘detect’ them as ‘readable’ more so than non-Ethiopian monolinguals.
在世界许多地方,多语言文化景观是儿童无处不在的输入。这种类型的输入(尽管只是视觉输入)可能会在上学之前就促进读写意识、不同书写和符号系统的整合和认知同化。本研究从数量和质量上探讨了多语言儿童理解和解释不同书写系统背后的原则的方式。重点是比较双语和单语儿童如何判断字母或非字母(单一,其他或混合)的“可读和不可读”表征;以及可读字符串是否符合指定给所呈现的符号字符串的定性和定量条件。双语埃塞俄比亚儿童和单语非埃塞俄比亚学龄前儿童在字母和非字母符号的“可读”区分上既有相似之处,也有差异。这两组人都认为包含各种字母符号组合的“可读”序列。在序列中符号的数量以及它是来自单一的、熟悉的还是混合的字母方面,两组之间存在着明显的差异。埃塞俄比亚双语儿童更倾向于考虑不同的字母系统——无论他们是将单个字母中的符号组合在一起还是将多个字母组合在一起——并且比非埃塞俄比亚单语儿童更倾向于“发现”它们是“可读的”。
{"title":"Monolingual and multilingual discrimination of written sequences' readability","authors":"A. Stavans","doi":"10.1080/17586801.2014.898574","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/17586801.2014.898574","url":null,"abstract":"Multilingual literate landscapes are ubiquitous input for children in many places in the world. This type of input (albeit visual only) may propel literacy awareness, integration and cognitive assimilation of different writing and notational systems even before schooling. This study explores quantitatively and qualitatively the ways in which young multilingual children understand and interpret the principles underlying different writing systems. The focus is to compare how bilingual and monolingual children judge ‘readable and non-readable’ representations which are alphabetic or non-alphabetic (single, other or mixed); and whether readable strings comply with a qualitative and quantitative condition assigned to the string of signs presented. There are similarities as well as differences in the distinction as ‘readable’ between alphabetic and non-alphabetic notations among bilingual Ethiopian children and monolingual non-Ethiopian children who are pre-readers. Both groups regard as ‘readable’ sequences that contain varied and multiple combinations of alphabetic signs. There are revealing differences between the groups as to the quantity of signs in the sequence and whether it comes from a single, familiar or mixed alphabet. The Ethiopian bilingual children are more inclined to regard different alphabetic systems—whether they combine signs from within a single alphabet or from multiple alphabets—and tend to ‘detect’ them as ‘readable’ more so than non-Ethiopian monolinguals.","PeriodicalId":39225,"journal":{"name":"Writing Systems Research","volume":"7 1","pages":"108 - 127"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2015-01-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1080/17586801.2014.898574","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"60437632","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 3
Emerging literacy in adult second-language learners: A synthesis of research findings in the Netherlands 成人第二语言学习者的新兴读写能力:荷兰研究结果的综合
Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2015-01-02 DOI: 10.1080/17586801.2014.943149
J. Kurvers
This article synthesises the findings of research on the acquisition of literacy by unschooled adult immigrants in the Netherlands. It addresses studies on three main topics: the metalinguistic skills of these adults, their development of word recognition and predictors of success in reading. The first study compared the conceptions of spoken and written language of 25 unschooled adults with those of young pre-reading children and low-educated adult readers. The study revealed that the metalinguistic skills of unschooled adults differed more from those of low-educated adult readers than from pre-reading children. The second was a case study which focused on the reading development of adult L2 beginners and revealed that their development of word-recognition skills confirms stage models of beginning reading in which acquiring the alphabetical principle is a key step. In the third study, the literacy skills of roughly 300 adult L2 literacy students were assessed and related to their background characteristics and to instructional practices. The study revealed considerable variation in success and time needed to develop reading. Success was related to student characteristics (age, prior education, attendance and contact with speakers of Dutch) and to instructional practices (time spent on small-group or individual work, use of multimedia and portfolio, and use of students’ first language as a resource).
本文综合了对荷兰未受教育的成年移民获得读写能力的研究结果。它涉及三个主要主题的研究:这些成年人的元语言技能,他们的单词识别发展和阅读成功的预测因素。第一项研究比较了25名未受教育的成年人与学龄前儿童和低教育程度的成年读者的口语和书面语概念。研究显示,未受教育的成年人的元语言技能与受教育程度较低的成人读者相比,与学前儿童的差异更大。第二部分是对成人二语初学者的阅读发展的个案研究,发现他们的单词识别技能的发展证实了开始阅读的阶段模型,其中字母原则的习得是关键步骤。在第三项研究中,对大约300名成年二语读写学生的读写技能进行了评估,并将其与他们的背景特征和教学实践联系起来。研究显示,培养阅读能力的成功率和所需时间存在很大差异。成功与学生的特征(年龄、先前教育、出勤率和与荷兰语使用者的接触)和教学实践(花在小组或个人作业上的时间、多媒体和作品集的使用以及使用学生的第一语言作为资源)有关。
{"title":"Emerging literacy in adult second-language learners: A synthesis of research findings in the Netherlands","authors":"J. Kurvers","doi":"10.1080/17586801.2014.943149","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/17586801.2014.943149","url":null,"abstract":"This article synthesises the findings of research on the acquisition of literacy by unschooled adult immigrants in the Netherlands. It addresses studies on three main topics: the metalinguistic skills of these adults, their development of word recognition and predictors of success in reading. The first study compared the conceptions of spoken and written language of 25 unschooled adults with those of young pre-reading children and low-educated adult readers. The study revealed that the metalinguistic skills of unschooled adults differed more from those of low-educated adult readers than from pre-reading children. The second was a case study which focused on the reading development of adult L2 beginners and revealed that their development of word-recognition skills confirms stage models of beginning reading in which acquiring the alphabetical principle is a key step. In the third study, the literacy skills of roughly 300 adult L2 literacy students were assessed and related to their background characteristics and to instructional practices. The study revealed considerable variation in success and time needed to develop reading. Success was related to student characteristics (age, prior education, attendance and contact with speakers of Dutch) and to instructional practices (time spent on small-group or individual work, use of multimedia and portfolio, and use of students’ first language as a resource).","PeriodicalId":39225,"journal":{"name":"Writing Systems Research","volume":"7 1","pages":"58 - 78"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2015-01-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1080/17586801.2014.943149","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"60438005","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 27
Following Roba: What happens when a low-educated adult immigrant learns to read 跟随罗巴:当一个受教育程度低的成年移民学会阅读时会发生什么
Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2015-01-02 DOI: 10.1080/17586801.2014.987199
Nicole M. Pettitt, E. Tarone
This longitudinal study follows concurrent changes in a multilingual adult English learner's mastery of alphabetic print literacy and his oral learner language. The learner was 29 years old, and began to read and write an alphabetic script for the first time in English, his seventh language, during this study. Systematic observations were made of both his development of specific literacy skills and specific structures in his oral English over the course of six months during one-to-one literacy tutoring sessions with the first author; these occurred one to two times each week. Mixed methods were used for collection of data, including learner observations, oral language tasks, interviews and review of relevant documents. Results document the learner's development of a set of specific literacy skills during the six-month study. Findings include: knowing the names of the letters of the alphabet seemed unrelated to his decoding ability; some syntactic elements of his oral production became more complex with increasing alphabetic literacy, while oral fluency, lexis and pragmatics did not appear to be related to development of alphabetic literacy.
这项纵向研究跟踪了多语言成人英语学习者对字母印刷读写能力和口语学习语言掌握的同步变化。这名学生29岁,在这项研究中,他第一次开始用英语(他的第七语言)阅读和书写字母。在与第一作者一对一的读写辅导课程中,系统地观察了他在六个月的课程中特定读写技能和口语结构的发展;这种情况每周发生一到两次。数据收集采用混合方法,包括学习者观察、口语任务、访谈和查阅相关文献。结果记录了学习者在六个月的学习期间对一套特定识字技能的发展。研究结果包括:知道字母表中字母的名字似乎与他的解码能力无关;随着字母读写能力的提高,他的口语生产中的一些句法元素变得更加复杂,而口语流利度、词汇和语用学似乎与字母读写能力的发展无关。
{"title":"Following Roba: What happens when a low-educated adult immigrant learns to read","authors":"Nicole M. Pettitt, E. Tarone","doi":"10.1080/17586801.2014.987199","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/17586801.2014.987199","url":null,"abstract":"This longitudinal study follows concurrent changes in a multilingual adult English learner's mastery of alphabetic print literacy and his oral learner language. The learner was 29 years old, and began to read and write an alphabetic script for the first time in English, his seventh language, during this study. Systematic observations were made of both his development of specific literacy skills and specific structures in his oral English over the course of six months during one-to-one literacy tutoring sessions with the first author; these occurred one to two times each week. Mixed methods were used for collection of data, including learner observations, oral language tasks, interviews and review of relevant documents. Results document the learner's development of a set of specific literacy skills during the six-month study. Findings include: knowing the names of the letters of the alphabet seemed unrelated to his decoding ability; some syntactic elements of his oral production became more complex with increasing alphabetic literacy, while oral fluency, lexis and pragmatics did not appear to be related to development of alphabetic literacy.","PeriodicalId":39225,"journal":{"name":"Writing Systems Research","volume":"7 1","pages":"20 - 38"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2015-01-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1080/17586801.2014.987199","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"60438121","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 13
期刊
Writing Systems Research
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1