Pub Date : 2018-03-12DOI: 10.1080/02134748.2018.1439691
Rodrigo A. Asún, Claudia Zúñiga, J. Morales
Abstract With the objective of theoretically and psychometrically updating the Regional Identity Scale (RIS) proposed by Zúñiga and Asún in 2004 and expanding the population for which it shows evidence of validity, a revision and revalidation of this instrument was performed via two studies. The first study included 1,113 university students from four regions of Chile, and the second included 1,200 adults from the same regions. Both samples were obtained via a random design through multistage conglomerates. The results enable us to propose a new version of the original scale (RIS-2) with suitable reliability indexes, solid evidence of both convergent validity and internal structure validity, and items with satisfactory psychometric quality. Furthermore, we verify the presence of a multifactor structure with three highly correlated dimensions. We conclude that the RIS-2 is a reliable, valid and conceptually broader instrument than the preceding version, so it is a useful tool to measure the intensity of regional identity in both university students and the adult population.
{"title":"Design and validation of the revised Regional Identity Scale (RIS-2) / Diseño y validación de la Escala de Identidad Regional revisada (RIS-2)","authors":"Rodrigo A. Asún, Claudia Zúñiga, J. Morales","doi":"10.1080/02134748.2018.1439691","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/02134748.2018.1439691","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract With the objective of theoretically and psychometrically updating the Regional Identity Scale (RIS) proposed by Zúñiga and Asún in 2004 and expanding the population for which it shows evidence of validity, a revision and revalidation of this instrument was performed via two studies. The first study included 1,113 university students from four regions of Chile, and the second included 1,200 adults from the same regions. Both samples were obtained via a random design through multistage conglomerates. The results enable us to propose a new version of the original scale (RIS-2) with suitable reliability indexes, solid evidence of both convergent validity and internal structure validity, and items with satisfactory psychometric quality. Furthermore, we verify the presence of a multifactor structure with three highly correlated dimensions. We conclude that the RIS-2 is a reliable, valid and conceptually broader instrument than the preceding version, so it is a useful tool to measure the intensity of regional identity in both university students and the adult population.","PeriodicalId":42024,"journal":{"name":"Revista De Psicologia Social","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2018-03-12","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1080/02134748.2018.1439691","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"42534431","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 2018-03-08DOI: 10.1080/02134748.2018.1446393
J. Barrientos, Jòrdi Escartin, Lara Longares, Álvaro Rodríguez-Carballeira
Abstract Psychological abuse in same-sex couples has hardly been studied in Spain and Latin America, despite its importance in society and public health. The objective of this study is to examine the prevalence of psychological abuse in same-sex couples in Spain and Latin America and analyse the associated sociodemographic characteristics. An online survey was conducted with 663 gay and lesbian respondents from Spain and Latin America. Of the total sample, 10.6% reported that they perceived themselves as victims of psychological abuse from their partner (9.6% in gay men and 10.7% in lesbians). In gay men, differences were found between victims and non-victims on the sociodemographic variables of age, professional status, and daily alcohol consumption. In lesbians, significant differences were found between victims and non-victims in the country and professional status variables. When comparing gay and lesbian victims, significant differences were found in professional status and alcohol consumption. This study contributes to better understanding of this phenomenon, and it reveals the need for further research.
{"title":"Sociodemographic characteristics of gay and lesbian victims of intimate partner psychological abuse in Spain and Latin America / Características sociodemográficas de gais y lesbianas víctimas de abuso psicológico en pareja en España e Hispanoamérica","authors":"J. Barrientos, Jòrdi Escartin, Lara Longares, Álvaro Rodríguez-Carballeira","doi":"10.1080/02134748.2018.1446393","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/02134748.2018.1446393","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract Psychological abuse in same-sex couples has hardly been studied in Spain and Latin America, despite its importance in society and public health. The objective of this study is to examine the prevalence of psychological abuse in same-sex couples in Spain and Latin America and analyse the associated sociodemographic characteristics. An online survey was conducted with 663 gay and lesbian respondents from Spain and Latin America. Of the total sample, 10.6% reported that they perceived themselves as victims of psychological abuse from their partner (9.6% in gay men and 10.7% in lesbians). In gay men, differences were found between victims and non-victims on the sociodemographic variables of age, professional status, and daily alcohol consumption. In lesbians, significant differences were found between victims and non-victims in the country and professional status variables. When comparing gay and lesbian victims, significant differences were found in professional status and alcohol consumption. This study contributes to better understanding of this phenomenon, and it reveals the need for further research.","PeriodicalId":42024,"journal":{"name":"Revista De Psicologia Social","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2018-03-08","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1080/02134748.2018.1446393","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"48864193","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 2018-03-05DOI: 10.1080/02134748.2018.1435106
R. Lobato, Miguel Moya, H. Trujillo
Abstract Based on the effects of framing on audience opinion, the attitudes towards Palestinians and Israelis are evaluated when a news item reports on a violent event between the two sides. To do so, a short inter-group experiment was conducted. A 3 (manipulation of the news item) x 2 (group evaluated) design was applied. Three news formats were presented: (1) about a Palestinian attack on Israel; (2) about an Israeli attack on Palestine; (3) no news item. Half of the participants evaluated the Israelis while the other half evaluated the Palestinians. The results of the study, which was performed with a Spanish sample (N = 319), showed that there were no differences in the evaluation of the group according to the group evaluated, although there were differences according to the manipulation of the news item: in both conditions in which one group attacked another, the group evaluated was rated more negatively than the control group. The results also showed that both emotions and stereotypes mediated the effects of framing on prejudice. These results suggest that the participants’ attitude seems to depend more on the violent framing of the news items than on the role assigned to each group and that emotional processes and stereotypes are what regulate these attitudes.
{"title":"Effect of the content of a news item on Spaniards’ attitudes towards Palestinians and Israelis / Efecto del contenido de una noticia sobre la actitud de los españoles hacia palestinos e israelíes","authors":"R. Lobato, Miguel Moya, H. Trujillo","doi":"10.1080/02134748.2018.1435106","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/02134748.2018.1435106","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract Based on the effects of framing on audience opinion, the attitudes towards Palestinians and Israelis are evaluated when a news item reports on a violent event between the two sides. To do so, a short inter-group experiment was conducted. A 3 (manipulation of the news item) x 2 (group evaluated) design was applied. Three news formats were presented: (1) about a Palestinian attack on Israel; (2) about an Israeli attack on Palestine; (3) no news item. Half of the participants evaluated the Israelis while the other half evaluated the Palestinians. The results of the study, which was performed with a Spanish sample (N = 319), showed that there were no differences in the evaluation of the group according to the group evaluated, although there were differences according to the manipulation of the news item: in both conditions in which one group attacked another, the group evaluated was rated more negatively than the control group. The results also showed that both emotions and stereotypes mediated the effects of framing on prejudice. These results suggest that the participants’ attitude seems to depend more on the violent framing of the news items than on the role assigned to each group and that emotional processes and stereotypes are what regulate these attitudes.","PeriodicalId":42024,"journal":{"name":"Revista De Psicologia Social","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2018-03-05","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1080/02134748.2018.1435106","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"43960188","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 2018-03-02DOI: 10.1080/02134748.2018.1435218
Carmen Gómez-Martínez, Maria de la Villa Moral-Jimenez
Abstract The dehumanization of certain outgroups is manifested in the deligitimization of the other and generates exclusion, especially in conditions perceived as potentially threatening. The purpose of this study is to analyse the perceptions of Syrian refugees and image of the Muslim population among young people between the ages of 15 and 18 (n = 205) and people over the age of 60 (n = 117). To do so, scales of dehumanization, modern racism and Islamophobia were applied and analysed according to certain variables such as age and degree of perceived religiosity. The results show that there are significant differences in terms of age in dehumanization, modern racism and Islamophobia, but the dehumanization of both Muslims and Syrian refugees seems to appear at all levels, with the perception of threat as a crucial factor. Religiosity also seems to be a significant variable in the study of racism and Islamophobia, although not in dehumanization. The psychosocial implications of these findings are discussed.
{"title":"Dehumanization and Islamophobia: attitudes towards the Syrian refugee crisis / Deshumanización e islamofobia: actitudes ante la crisis de los refugiados sirios","authors":"Carmen Gómez-Martínez, Maria de la Villa Moral-Jimenez","doi":"10.1080/02134748.2018.1435218","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/02134748.2018.1435218","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract The dehumanization of certain outgroups is manifested in the deligitimization of the other and generates exclusion, especially in conditions perceived as potentially threatening. The purpose of this study is to analyse the perceptions of Syrian refugees and image of the Muslim population among young people between the ages of 15 and 18 (n = 205) and people over the age of 60 (n = 117). To do so, scales of dehumanization, modern racism and Islamophobia were applied and analysed according to certain variables such as age and degree of perceived religiosity. The results show that there are significant differences in terms of age in dehumanization, modern racism and Islamophobia, but the dehumanization of both Muslims and Syrian refugees seems to appear at all levels, with the perception of threat as a crucial factor. Religiosity also seems to be a significant variable in the study of racism and Islamophobia, although not in dehumanization. The psychosocial implications of these findings are discussed.","PeriodicalId":42024,"journal":{"name":"Revista De Psicologia Social","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2018-03-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1080/02134748.2018.1435218","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"46672784","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 2018-02-28DOI: 10.1080/02134748.2018.1435217
R. Díaz-Loving, C. Cruz-Torres, Carolina Armenta-Huarte, Norma E. Reyes-Ruiz
Abstract Cultures can be analysed through cultural syndromes (shared patterns of beliefs, attitudes, norms, roles and values organized around a theme). One of these syndromes, individualism-collectivism, distinguishes collectivist cultures, which prioritize collective over individual interests, from individualist cultures, which value individual autonomy, achievement and freedom. These dimensions intersect with the horizontal-vertical syndrome, where verticality means the preference for a society organized by hierarchies, in contrast to a horizontal culture, which privileges equality. This paper explores these dimensions using a new instrument in a sample of 990 participants from four regions in Mexico. The results of exploratory and confirmatory factorial analyses show four theoretically congruent factors with adequate levels of internal consistency reaching metric equivalence criteria in three of the four samples. The scores show greater support for horizontal individualism and less support for vertical collectivism. These results are discussed within the context of a cultural shift towards individualism.
{"title":"Variations in horizontal and vertical individualism-collectivism in four regions of Mexico / Variaciones en el individualismo-colectivismo horizontal y vertical en cuatro regiones de México","authors":"R. Díaz-Loving, C. Cruz-Torres, Carolina Armenta-Huarte, Norma E. Reyes-Ruiz","doi":"10.1080/02134748.2018.1435217","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/02134748.2018.1435217","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract Cultures can be analysed through cultural syndromes (shared patterns of beliefs, attitudes, norms, roles and values organized around a theme). One of these syndromes, individualism-collectivism, distinguishes collectivist cultures, which prioritize collective over individual interests, from individualist cultures, which value individual autonomy, achievement and freedom. These dimensions intersect with the horizontal-vertical syndrome, where verticality means the preference for a society organized by hierarchies, in contrast to a horizontal culture, which privileges equality. This paper explores these dimensions using a new instrument in a sample of 990 participants from four regions in Mexico. The results of exploratory and confirmatory factorial analyses show four theoretically congruent factors with adequate levels of internal consistency reaching metric equivalence criteria in three of the four samples. The scores show greater support for horizontal individualism and less support for vertical collectivism. These results are discussed within the context of a cultural shift towards individualism.","PeriodicalId":42024,"journal":{"name":"Revista De Psicologia Social","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2018-02-28","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1080/02134748.2018.1435217","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"47216093","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 2018-02-21DOI: 10.1080/02134748.2018.1435219
R. Zein
Abstract Due to its diverse nature, being an Indonesian is sometimes confusing as it entails overlapping national, ethnic and religious identity. By using a social representation approach, online survey research involving 114 Indonesians was carried out to explore these questions: How do Indonesians negotiate their ethnic, religious and national identity? What identity markers should one possess to claim being an Indonesian? And lastly, who is the nationalist? Research findings suggest that national and ethnic identity as well as ethnic and religious identity were consensual, while the relation between religious and national identity was emancipated. It is also found that being a Muslim and possessing a stronger sense of ethnic identity would increase the likelihood of being a prouder Indonesian, while being a female, living abroad for 5–10 years, being a nationalist Muslim and embracing transnational Islamic movements reduced the probability of having a stronger sense of national identity.
{"title":"What’s the matter with being Indonesian? A social representation approach to unravelling Indonesian national identity / ¿Qué significa ser indonesio? Una perspectiva de la representación social para desentrañar la identidad nacional indonesia","authors":"R. Zein","doi":"10.1080/02134748.2018.1435219","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/02134748.2018.1435219","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract Due to its diverse nature, being an Indonesian is sometimes confusing as it entails overlapping national, ethnic and religious identity. By using a social representation approach, online survey research involving 114 Indonesians was carried out to explore these questions: How do Indonesians negotiate their ethnic, religious and national identity? What identity markers should one possess to claim being an Indonesian? And lastly, who is the nationalist? Research findings suggest that national and ethnic identity as well as ethnic and religious identity were consensual, while the relation between religious and national identity was emancipated. It is also found that being a Muslim and possessing a stronger sense of ethnic identity would increase the likelihood of being a prouder Indonesian, while being a female, living abroad for 5–10 years, being a nationalist Muslim and embracing transnational Islamic movements reduced the probability of having a stronger sense of national identity.","PeriodicalId":42024,"journal":{"name":"Revista De Psicologia Social","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2018-02-21","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1080/02134748.2018.1435219","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"48085852","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 2018-01-02DOI: 10.1080/02134748.2017.1385239
Ana Urbiola, G. Willis, Josefa Ruiz-Romero, Miguel Moya
Abstract The aim of this research was to adapt and validate the Concern with Acting Prejudiced (CAP) scale for a Spanish sample. This measure evaluates concern about appearing prejudiced to others or oneself and the motivation not to deviate from personal egalitarian standards. First, we completed the translation and an item content validity analysis. Then, in Study 1 (N = 198), we conducted an exploratory factor analysis. In Study 2 (N = 383), we conducted a confirmatory analysis of the unifactorial structure of the Spanish version of the scale and examined its validity. In Study 3 (N = 89), we explored the moderating role of participants’ concern with acting prejudiced in the relation between implicit prejudice and explicit prejudice. The results confirmed the unifactorial structure of the scale, its appropriate psychometric properties and its predictive validity. Moreover, we confirmed that the CAP moderated the relation between implicit and explicit prejudice. In participants with a low concern with acting prejudiced, implicit prejudice was positively related with modern prejudice; in participants with a high concern with acting prejudiced, these variables were not related.
{"title":"When implicit prejudice does not predict explicit prejudice: Spanish version of the Concern with Acting Prejudiced scale / Cuando el prejuicio implícito no predice el explícito: versión española de la escala Concern with Acting Prejudiced","authors":"Ana Urbiola, G. Willis, Josefa Ruiz-Romero, Miguel Moya","doi":"10.1080/02134748.2017.1385239","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/02134748.2017.1385239","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract The aim of this research was to adapt and validate the Concern with Acting Prejudiced (CAP) scale for a Spanish sample. This measure evaluates concern about appearing prejudiced to others or oneself and the motivation not to deviate from personal egalitarian standards. First, we completed the translation and an item content validity analysis. Then, in Study 1 (N = 198), we conducted an exploratory factor analysis. In Study 2 (N = 383), we conducted a confirmatory analysis of the unifactorial structure of the Spanish version of the scale and examined its validity. In Study 3 (N = 89), we explored the moderating role of participants’ concern with acting prejudiced in the relation between implicit prejudice and explicit prejudice. The results confirmed the unifactorial structure of the scale, its appropriate psychometric properties and its predictive validity. Moreover, we confirmed that the CAP moderated the relation between implicit and explicit prejudice. In participants with a low concern with acting prejudiced, implicit prejudice was positively related with modern prejudice; in participants with a high concern with acting prejudiced, these variables were not related.","PeriodicalId":42024,"journal":{"name":"Revista De Psicologia Social","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2018-01-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1080/02134748.2017.1385239","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"48220486","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 2018-01-02DOI: 10.1080/02134748.2017.1385214
L. Cervantes, I. Cuadrado, Marisol Navas
This study tests two serial models of intergroup contact in intergroup relations between users of Spanish public libraries and the directionality of the contact-prejudice relationship. Participants were selected in libraries that meet IFLA Guidelines in Barcelona (NSpaniards = 138 and NMoroccans = 89) and libraries that do not meet these guidelines in Almería (NSpaniards = 116 and NMoroccans = 100). They responded to a survey about the quantity of intergroup contact, anxiety, ethnocultural empathy, outgroup knowledge and prejudice. Several serial mediation analyses reveal a model where the indirect effect of quantity of contact in the library on prejudice was mediated by intergroup anxiety and ethnocultural empathy in Moroccans in Almería and Spaniards and Moroccans in Barcelona, and it found that the direction of this relationship is from contact to attitudes, but not attitudes to contact. These results show that the mediating power of affective variables is stronger than that of cognitive variables. Explanations about the absence of significant results in Spaniards in the Almería group are provided.
{"title":"Intergroup contact and prejudice in Moroccan and Spanish users of Spanish public libraries: mediating variables and directionality of the process / Contacto intergrupal y prejuicio en usuarios marroquíes y españoles de bibliotecas públicas españolas: variables mediadoras y direccionalidad del proces","authors":"L. Cervantes, I. Cuadrado, Marisol Navas","doi":"10.1080/02134748.2017.1385214","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/02134748.2017.1385214","url":null,"abstract":"This study tests two serial models of intergroup contact in intergroup relations between users of Spanish public libraries and the directionality of the contact-prejudice relationship. Participants were selected in libraries that meet IFLA Guidelines in Barcelona (NSpaniards = 138 and NMoroccans = 89) and libraries that do not meet these guidelines in Almería (NSpaniards = 116 and NMoroccans = 100). They responded to a survey about the quantity of intergroup contact, anxiety, ethnocultural empathy, outgroup knowledge and prejudice. Several serial mediation analyses reveal a model where the indirect effect of quantity of contact in the library on prejudice was mediated by intergroup anxiety and ethnocultural empathy in Moroccans in Almería and Spaniards and Moroccans in Barcelona, and it found that the direction of this relationship is from contact to attitudes, but not attitudes to contact. These results show that the mediating power of affective variables is stronger than that of cognitive variables. Explanations about the absence of significant results in Spaniards in the Almería group are provided.","PeriodicalId":42024,"journal":{"name":"Revista De Psicologia Social","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2018-01-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1080/02134748.2017.1385214","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"43902975","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 2018-01-02DOI: 10.1080/02134748.2017.1394003
Raimundo Aguayo, M. P. Alves, M. Alzate, Elena Andrade, Camilo José Cela, N. Basabe, J. Berenguer, Marcin Bukowski, J. Canto, J. Corraliza, S. D. Lemus, Miguel Hernández, Itziar Fernández, Ismael Gallardo, Efraín García-Sánchez, L. Montes, Cristina Gómez-Román, J. González-Castro, B. Hernández, Javier Horcajo, T. Jiménez, U. Zaragoza, Gloria Jiménez-Moya, Susana Llorens, A. Manassero, A. Molina, Juan Moriano, Borja Paredes, A. M. Pérez-García, Katerina Petkanopoulou, Anna Wlodarczyk
Revista de Psicología Social wants to thank the following people for their help in manuscript reviewing/Revista de Psicología Social agradece a las siguientes personas su trabajo en la evaluación de originales Raimundo Aguayo (Universidad de Granada) Marta P. Alves (Universidade da Beira Interior) Mónica Alzate (Instituto Tecnológico Metropolitano) Elena Ma Andrade (Universidade de Santiago de Compostela) María Aranda (Universidad de Jaén) Alicia Arenas (Universidad de Sevilla) Frédérique Autin (Université de Poitiers) Laurent Auzoult (Université Bourgogne Franche-Comté) Jorge Barraca (Universidad Camilo José Cela) Nekane Basabe (Universidad del País Vasco) Jaime Berenguer (Universidad Autónoma de Madrid) Magdalena Bobowik (Universidad del País Vasco) Gilbert Brenes (Universidad de Costa Rica) Elena Briones (Universidad de Cantabria) Marcin Bukowski (Jagiellonian University) Amparo Caballero (Universidad Autónoma de Madrid) Jesús M. Canto (Universidad de Málaga) Hugo Carretero-Dios (Universidad de Granada) Héctor Carvacho (Pontificia Universidad Católica de Chile) José Antonio Corraliza (Universidad Autónoma de Madrid) Soledad de Lemus (Universidad de Granada) Ma Carmen Delgado (Universidad Pontificia de Salamanca) Carlos Díaz-Lázaro (Walden University) Mar Durán (Universidade de Santiago de Compostela) Carlos Falces (Universidad Miguel Hernández) Itziar Fernández (Universidad Nacional de Educación a Distancia) Ana Figueiredo (Pontificia Universidad Católica de Chile) Ismael Gallardo (Universidad de Talca) Carlos García-Guiu (Universidad de Granada) Efraín García-Sánchez (Universidad de Granada) Monica Gerber (Universidad Diego Portales) Francisco Gil (Universidad Complutense de Madrid) Lorena Gil de Montes (Universidad del País Vasco) Cristina Gómez-Román (Universidade de Santiago de Compostela) Jose Luis González-Castro (Universidad de Burgos) Revista de Psicología Social / International Journal of Social Psychology, 2018 Vol. 33, No. 1, 213–214, https://doi.org/10.1080/02134748.2017.1394003
社会心理学杂志想感谢以下人士在手写体审查/社会心理学杂志中的帮助,感谢以下人士在评估原创Raimundo Aguayo(格拉纳达大学)Marta P。阿尔维斯(贝拉内政大学)莫妮卡·阿尔扎特(大都会理工学院)埃琳娜·马安德拉德(圣地亚哥-德孔波斯特拉大学)玛丽亚·阿兰达(哈恩大学)艾丽西亚·阿里纳斯(塞维利亚大学)弗雷德里克·奥廷(普瓦捷大学)劳伦特·奥祖尔特(布尔戈涅-科姆特大学)豪尔赫·巴拉卡(卡米洛·何塞·塞拉大学)内坎·巴萨贝(巴斯克地区大学)海梅·贝伦格(马德里自治大学)马格达莱纳·博维克(巴斯克地区大学)吉尔伯特·布伦斯(哥斯达黎加大学)埃琳娜·布里奥内斯(坎塔布里亚大学)马辛·布科夫斯基(贾吉洛尼安大学)安帕罗·卡瓦列罗(马德里自治大学)耶稣M。Canto(马拉加大学)Hugo Carretero-Dios(格拉纳达大学)Hector Carvacho(智利天主教大学)JoséAntonio Corraliza(马德里自治大学)Soledad de Lemus(格拉纳达大学)Ma Carmen Delgado(萨拉曼卡教皇大学)Carlos Díaz-Lazaro(瓦尔登大学)Mar Duran(圣地亚哥-德孔波斯特拉大学)Carlos Falces(米格尔埃尔南德斯大学)Itziar Fernández(国家远程教育大学)Ana Figueiredo(智利天主教大学)Ismael Gallardo(塔尔卡大学)Carlos García-Guiu(格拉纳达大学)Efraín García-Sánchez(格拉纳达大学)Monica Gerber(迭戈门户大学)Francisco Gil(马德里康普顿斯大学)Lorena Gil de Montes(巴斯克地区大学)Cristina Gomez-Roman(圣地亚哥-德孔波斯特拉大学)Jose Luis Gonzalez-Castro(布尔戈斯大学)社会心理学/国际杂志《社会心理学杂志》,2018年第33卷,第1期,213-214,https://doi.org/10.1080/02134748.2017.1394003
{"title":"Reviewers for 2017/Evaluadores del año 2017","authors":"Raimundo Aguayo, M. P. Alves, M. Alzate, Elena Andrade, Camilo José Cela, N. Basabe, J. Berenguer, Marcin Bukowski, J. Canto, J. Corraliza, S. D. Lemus, Miguel Hernández, Itziar Fernández, Ismael Gallardo, Efraín García-Sánchez, L. Montes, Cristina Gómez-Román, J. González-Castro, B. Hernández, Javier Horcajo, T. Jiménez, U. Zaragoza, Gloria Jiménez-Moya, Susana Llorens, A. Manassero, A. Molina, Juan Moriano, Borja Paredes, A. M. Pérez-García, Katerina Petkanopoulou, Anna Wlodarczyk","doi":"10.1080/02134748.2017.1394003","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/02134748.2017.1394003","url":null,"abstract":"Revista de Psicología Social wants to thank the following people for their help in manuscript reviewing/Revista de Psicología Social agradece a las siguientes personas su trabajo en la evaluación de originales Raimundo Aguayo (Universidad de Granada) Marta P. Alves (Universidade da Beira Interior) Mónica Alzate (Instituto Tecnológico Metropolitano) Elena Ma Andrade (Universidade de Santiago de Compostela) María Aranda (Universidad de Jaén) Alicia Arenas (Universidad de Sevilla) Frédérique Autin (Université de Poitiers) Laurent Auzoult (Université Bourgogne Franche-Comté) Jorge Barraca (Universidad Camilo José Cela) Nekane Basabe (Universidad del País Vasco) Jaime Berenguer (Universidad Autónoma de Madrid) Magdalena Bobowik (Universidad del País Vasco) Gilbert Brenes (Universidad de Costa Rica) Elena Briones (Universidad de Cantabria) Marcin Bukowski (Jagiellonian University) Amparo Caballero (Universidad Autónoma de Madrid) Jesús M. Canto (Universidad de Málaga) Hugo Carretero-Dios (Universidad de Granada) Héctor Carvacho (Pontificia Universidad Católica de Chile) José Antonio Corraliza (Universidad Autónoma de Madrid) Soledad de Lemus (Universidad de Granada) Ma Carmen Delgado (Universidad Pontificia de Salamanca) Carlos Díaz-Lázaro (Walden University) Mar Durán (Universidade de Santiago de Compostela) Carlos Falces (Universidad Miguel Hernández) Itziar Fernández (Universidad Nacional de Educación a Distancia) Ana Figueiredo (Pontificia Universidad Católica de Chile) Ismael Gallardo (Universidad de Talca) Carlos García-Guiu (Universidad de Granada) Efraín García-Sánchez (Universidad de Granada) Monica Gerber (Universidad Diego Portales) Francisco Gil (Universidad Complutense de Madrid) Lorena Gil de Montes (Universidad del País Vasco) Cristina Gómez-Román (Universidade de Santiago de Compostela) Jose Luis González-Castro (Universidad de Burgos) Revista de Psicología Social / International Journal of Social Psychology, 2018 Vol. 33, No. 1, 213–214, https://doi.org/10.1080/02134748.2017.1394003","PeriodicalId":42024,"journal":{"name":"Revista De Psicologia Social","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2018-01-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1080/02134748.2017.1394003","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"47509111","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 2018-01-02DOI: 10.1080/02134748.2017.1385243
Karima El Ghoudani, M. Pulido-Martos, Esther Lopez-Zafra
Abstract Emotional intelligence is a widespread concept that has been analysed in different countries. However, it has been barely measured in African-Arab societies’ cultures due to their scarce tradition in validating instruments. In Morocco, the mere translation of instruments is a common practice that should be avoided to incorporate cultural and linguistic singularities. Thus, the aim of our study was to culturally adapt the Wong and Law Emotional Intelligence Scale to Morocco. This version could be widely used in other Arab North African countries and among immigrants in hosting countries. Two hundred and seventy-three Arab undergraduates (178 women and 95 men; age M = 22.17, SD = 4.09) from Morocco completed a questionnaire adapted to their culture following the back translation method. To determine construct validity, data were analysed by means of confirmatory factor analysis. The final scale resulted in a second-order factorial model with four first-order factors compounded by 16 items. This structure supports the original for Moroccan Arabic and other countries in which Arabic is spoken. Likewise, the results confirm an adequate criterion validity of the instrument.
{"title":"Measuring emotional intelligence in Moroccan Arabic: the Wong and Law Emotional Intelligence Scale / Medidas de la inteligencia emocional en árabe marroquí: la escala de inteligencia emocional de Wong y Law","authors":"Karima El Ghoudani, M. Pulido-Martos, Esther Lopez-Zafra","doi":"10.1080/02134748.2017.1385243","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/02134748.2017.1385243","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract Emotional intelligence is a widespread concept that has been analysed in different countries. However, it has been barely measured in African-Arab societies’ cultures due to their scarce tradition in validating instruments. In Morocco, the mere translation of instruments is a common practice that should be avoided to incorporate cultural and linguistic singularities. Thus, the aim of our study was to culturally adapt the Wong and Law Emotional Intelligence Scale to Morocco. This version could be widely used in other Arab North African countries and among immigrants in hosting countries. Two hundred and seventy-three Arab undergraduates (178 women and 95 men; age M = 22.17, SD = 4.09) from Morocco completed a questionnaire adapted to their culture following the back translation method. To determine construct validity, data were analysed by means of confirmatory factor analysis. The final scale resulted in a second-order factorial model with four first-order factors compounded by 16 items. This structure supports the original for Moroccan Arabic and other countries in which Arabic is spoken. Likewise, the results confirm an adequate criterion validity of the instrument.","PeriodicalId":42024,"journal":{"name":"Revista De Psicologia Social","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2018-01-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1080/02134748.2017.1385243","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"45482017","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}