首页 > 最新文献

Vial-Vigo International Journal of Applied Linguistics最新文献

英文 中文
Teachers’ oral corrective feedback and learners’ uptake in high school CLIL and EFL classrooms 高中CLIL和EFL课堂中教师口头纠正反馈与学习者吸收
IF 0.9 4区 文学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2021-01-18 DOI: 10.35869/VIAL.V0I18.3368
Ruth Milla, M. Mayo
espanolEstudios previos han hallado que la retroalimentacion correctiva oral (RCO) puede variar dependiendo de factores tales como la edad y el nivel de conocimiento de lengua de los aprendices o los tipos de RCO que facilita el profesor. Sin embargo, existe escasa investigacion sobre la variable del contexto de aprendizaje, aunque la RCO y la respuesta de los aprendices difiere en cantidad y tipo en contextos de segunda lengua y contextos de lengua extranjera (LE). Ademas, la RCO apenas ha sido investigada en aulas que siguen un enfoque de aprendizaje integrado de contenido y lengua extranjera (AICLE). Debido a que los programas de AICLE se estan implementando ampliamente en contextos europeos y que las diferencias entre los contextos sugieren posibles variaciones en cuanto a la RCO y la respuesta de los aprendices, este trabajo tiene como objetivo comparar la interaccion oral grabada (22 horas 43 minutos) en un aula intacta (N=26) de segundo curso de bachillerato (edad=17-18) en las clases de ingles como LE con otra de estudios empresariales (AICLE). Los resultados muestran diferencias significativas en cuanto a los tipos de RCO utilizados y la respuesta de los aprendices ante las reformulaciones. Se presentan implicaciones pedagogicas relativas a como obtener el maximo beneficio de la RCO en aulas de LE EnglishOral corrective feedback (OCF) has been reported to be affected by several factors such as learners’ age, level of proficiency or the OCF types provided by the teacher. However, little research has been carried out on the variable learning context, even though OCF and uptake vary in rates and types in second language (SL) and foreign language (FL) settings. Moreover, OCF has been clearly under-researched in classrooms that follow a content and language integrated learning (CLIL) approach. As CLIL programs are being widely implemented mainly in European settings and differences in context characteristics suggest variations in OCF and learners’ uptake, the present study aimed to compare the recorded classroom interaction data (22 hours 43 minutes) from an intact class of learners (N=26) in their last year of secondary education (age 17-18), attending the lessons of an English as a FL (EFL) teacher and a Business Studies (CLIL) teacher. Findings show significant differences as to the proportion and OCF types used, as well as different learners’ behavior regarding the rates of uptake and repair and the uptake after the use of recasts. Pedagogical implications are offered as to how to maximize the potential benefits of OCF in FL classrooms
以前的研究发现,口头纠正反馈(RCO)可能会因年龄、学习者的语言知识水平或教师提供的RCO类型等因素而有所不同。然而,关于学习环境变量的研究很少,尽管在第二语言和外语环境中,学习者的RCO和反应在数量和类型上有所不同。此外,在采用内容和外语综合学习(clil)方法的教室中,对RCO的研究很少。由于AICLE方案正在实施广泛的背景、欧洲人和环境之间的差异说明潜在至于RCO变化和反应学徒,这项工作旨在比较interaccion口头记录(22小时43分钟)在一个教室,完好无损(N = 26)大二(年龄= 17-18)英语类中他与另一个(AICLE工商管理学院)。结果表明,在使用的RCO类型和学习者对重新配方的反应方面存在显著差异。本文提出了如何从英语纠正反馈(OCF)课堂中获得最大效益的教学意义。据报道,OCF受到学习者年龄、熟练程度或教师提供的OCF类型等几个因素的影响。然而,关于可变学习环境的研究很少,尽管在第二语言(SL)和外语(FL)设置中OCF和接受不同的比率和类型。此外,在采用内容和语言综合学习(CLIL)方法的课堂中,OCF的研究明显不足。As CLIL programs are being宣传实行mainly in European settings and差异in context分离器suggest variations in the吸收OCF和一项‘目前研究旨在to compare the recorded教室interaction data(22个小时43 minutes) from an intact class of项(N = 26)名单教育(age 17-18), attending the lessons of an English As a前线成就人生(teacher and a Business Studies (CLIL) teacher。研究结果显示,在使用的比例和OCF类型方面,以及不同学习者在接受和修复率以及使用后接受方面的行为存在显著差异。提供了教学意义,以最大限度地发挥OCF在FL课堂的潜在效益
{"title":"Teachers’ oral corrective feedback and learners’ uptake in high school CLIL and EFL classrooms","authors":"Ruth Milla, M. Mayo","doi":"10.35869/VIAL.V0I18.3368","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.35869/VIAL.V0I18.3368","url":null,"abstract":"espanolEstudios previos han hallado que la retroalimentacion correctiva oral (RCO) puede variar dependiendo de factores tales como la edad y el nivel de conocimiento de lengua de los aprendices o los tipos de RCO que facilita el profesor. Sin embargo, existe escasa investigacion sobre la variable del contexto de aprendizaje, aunque la RCO y la respuesta de los aprendices difiere en cantidad y tipo en contextos de segunda lengua y contextos de lengua extranjera (LE). Ademas, la RCO apenas ha sido investigada en aulas que siguen un enfoque de aprendizaje integrado de contenido y lengua extranjera (AICLE). Debido a que los programas de AICLE se estan implementando ampliamente en contextos europeos y que las diferencias entre los contextos sugieren posibles variaciones en cuanto a la RCO y la respuesta de los aprendices, este trabajo tiene como objetivo comparar la interaccion oral grabada (22 horas 43 minutos) en un aula intacta (N=26) de segundo curso de bachillerato (edad=17-18) en las clases de ingles como LE con otra de estudios empresariales (AICLE). Los resultados muestran diferencias significativas en cuanto a los tipos de RCO utilizados y la respuesta de los aprendices ante las reformulaciones. Se presentan implicaciones pedagogicas relativas a como obtener el maximo beneficio de la RCO en aulas de LE EnglishOral corrective feedback (OCF) has been reported to be affected by several factors such as learners’ age, level of proficiency or the OCF types provided by the teacher. However, little research has been carried out on the variable learning context, even though OCF and uptake vary in rates and types in second language (SL) and foreign language (FL) settings. Moreover, OCF has been clearly under-researched in classrooms that follow a content and language integrated learning (CLIL) approach. As CLIL programs are being widely implemented mainly in European settings and differences in context characteristics suggest variations in OCF and learners’ uptake, the present study aimed to compare the recorded classroom interaction data (22 hours 43 minutes) from an intact class of learners (N=26) in their last year of secondary education (age 17-18), attending the lessons of an English as a FL (EFL) teacher and a Business Studies (CLIL) teacher. Findings show significant differences as to the proportion and OCF types used, as well as different learners’ behavior regarding the rates of uptake and repair and the uptake after the use of recasts. Pedagogical implications are offered as to how to maximize the potential benefits of OCF in FL classrooms","PeriodicalId":42598,"journal":{"name":"Vial-Vigo International Journal of Applied Linguistics","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.9,"publicationDate":"2021-01-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"73949142","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 5
A study of L2 vocabulary acquisition under incidental and intentional conditions 偶然和有意条件下二语词汇习得的研究
IF 0.9 4区 文学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2020-01-20 DOI: 10.35869/vial.v0i17.1467
Sarah Sok, ZhaoHong Han
Vocabulary development is indisputably a vital aspect of second language acquisition. In spite of the abundant attention it has garnered over the past few decades, it remains unclear how adult learners fare with intentional and incidental ways of learning. The current study investigated the effects of intentional learning (via studying a word list), incidental learning (via reading), and combined intentional-andincidental learning (via studying a word list followed by reading) conditions on 30 adult learners’ second language vocabulary acquisition. Vocabulary acquisition was measured in terms of percentage gains as well as changes in the depth of vocabulary knowledge. Results showed that while both the intentional and incidental modes of learning led to vocabulary gains, the combined intentional-and-incidental condition resulted in significantly greater gains than either the intentional-only or the incidentalonly condition. No significant differences were found between the incidental-only and intentional-only conditions.
词汇发展无疑是第二语言习得的一个重要方面。尽管在过去的几十年里,它已经获得了大量的关注,但成人学习者在有意和偶然的学习方式中表现如何仍不清楚。本研究调查了有意学习(通过学习单词表)、附带学习(通过阅读)和有意与附带学习(通过学习单词表再阅读)相结合的条件对30名成人学习者第二语言词汇习得的影响。词汇习得以百分比增益和词汇知识深度的变化来衡量。结果表明,虽然有意和偶然的学习模式都会导致词汇量的增加,但有意和偶然结合的学习模式比单纯的有意和偶然学习模式取得的效果要大得多。在仅偶然和仅故意条件之间没有发现显着差异。
{"title":"A study of L2 vocabulary acquisition under incidental and intentional conditions","authors":"Sarah Sok, ZhaoHong Han","doi":"10.35869/vial.v0i17.1467","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.35869/vial.v0i17.1467","url":null,"abstract":"Vocabulary development is indisputably a vital aspect of second language acquisition. In spite of the abundant attention it has garnered over the past few decades, it remains unclear how adult learners fare with intentional and incidental ways of learning. The current study investigated the effects of intentional learning (via studying a word list), incidental learning (via reading), and combined intentional-andincidental learning (via studying a word list followed by reading) conditions on 30 adult learners’ second language vocabulary acquisition. Vocabulary acquisition was measured in terms of percentage gains as well as changes in the depth of vocabulary knowledge. Results showed that while both the intentional and incidental modes of learning led to vocabulary gains, the combined intentional-and-incidental condition resulted in significantly greater gains than either the intentional-only or the incidentalonly condition. No significant differences were found between the incidental-only and intentional-only conditions.","PeriodicalId":42598,"journal":{"name":"Vial-Vigo International Journal of Applied Linguistics","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.9,"publicationDate":"2020-01-20","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"88459894","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 6
Attention-direction versus retrieval practice: which fosters the productive recall of German formulaic sequences best? 注意方向与检索练习:哪个最能促进德语公式化序列的生产性回忆?
IF 0.9 4区 文学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2020-01-20 DOI: 10.35869/vial.v0i17.1463
Griet Boone, June Eyckmans
This article reports on a classroom-based (quasi)-experiment with a pre-test post-test design that explored the effect of two types of activities on the productive recall of German formulaic sequences (FS): 1) attention-directing activities and 2) retrieval practice. Two intact classes of Dutch-speaking university students of German participated in the study. One class was randomly assigned to the attention-directing condition (n=18), the other one to the retrieval condition (n=11). Twenty-two target FS were selected as learning items. Each group processed the FS in a different condition. In the attention-directing condition, students had to 1) re-read a video transcript with the FS in bold typeface and 2) translate the targets into Dutch. In the retrieval condition, students had to 1) complete a transcript in which the FS were deleted and 2) translate the targets into German. Results indicate that the retrieval condition led to better productive phrase learning than the attention-directing condition.
本文报道了一项基于课堂的准实验,采用前测后测设计,探讨了两种类型的活动对德国公式化序列(FS)生产性回忆的影响:1)注意引导活动和2)检索练习。两个完整的荷兰语德语大学生班级参与了这项研究。一组随机分配到注意引导组(n=18),另一组随机分配到检索组(n=11)。选取22个目标FS作为学习项目。各组在不同条件下处理FS。在注意力引导条件下,学生们必须1)重读一段粗体的视频文本,2)将目标翻译成荷兰语。在检索条件下,学生必须1)完成一份删除FS的成绩单,2)将目标翻译成德语。结果表明,检索条件比注意引导条件更能促进生产性短语学习。
{"title":"Attention-direction versus retrieval practice: which fosters the productive recall of German formulaic sequences best?","authors":"Griet Boone, June Eyckmans","doi":"10.35869/vial.v0i17.1463","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.35869/vial.v0i17.1463","url":null,"abstract":"This article reports on a classroom-based (quasi)-experiment with a pre-test post-test design that explored the effect of two types of activities on the productive recall of German formulaic sequences (FS): 1) attention-directing activities and 2) retrieval practice. Two intact classes of Dutch-speaking university students of German participated in the study. One class was randomly assigned to the attention-directing condition (n=18), the other one to the retrieval condition (n=11). Twenty-two target FS were selected as learning items. Each group processed the FS in a different condition. In the attention-directing condition, students had to 1) re-read a video transcript with the FS in bold typeface and 2) translate the targets into Dutch. In the retrieval condition, students had to 1) complete a transcript in which the FS were deleted and 2) translate the targets into German. Results indicate that the retrieval condition led to better productive phrase learning than the attention-directing condition.","PeriodicalId":42598,"journal":{"name":"Vial-Vigo International Journal of Applied Linguistics","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.9,"publicationDate":"2020-01-20","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"81500697","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
L2 reading and vocabulary development after a short Study Abroad experience 在短暂的海外学习经历后的第二语言阅读和词汇发展
IF 0.9 4区 文学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2020-01-20 DOI: 10.35869/vial.v0i17.1464
Judith Borràs, Ángels Llanes
This study explores the development of L2 reading and vocabulary as a result of a short (3-week) SA experience. Given the growing literature on Study Abroad (SA) research, this investigation attempts to shed some light on two rather under-researched areas such as L2 reading and vocabulary, and it does so with a group of adolescents (n= 52), a population often neglected by the SA research despite being one of the most common participants in SA programmes. The participants, Spanish learners of English (L2), were administered a placement test to determine their initial L2 level, a reading text (from which fluency and comprehension were calculated), the Updated Vocabulary Level Test to measure their receptive vocabulary knowledge, and a written essay to capture their productive vocabulary development in terms of lexical accuracy, fluency, density and sophistication. The results indicate that short SA experiences are positive for both reading fluency and comprehension as well as for receptive vocabulary development. Findings are not so positive in terms of productive vocabulary skills. Finally, results failed to show a direct relationship between gains in reading, and initial proficiency and vocabulary level.
本研究探讨了二语阅读和词汇的发展,作为一个短(3周)的辅助语言体验的结果。鉴于关于出国留学(SA)研究的文献越来越多,本调查试图揭示两个研究不足的领域,如第二语言阅读和词汇,并以一组青少年(n= 52)为研究对象,尽管他们是出国留学项目中最常见的参与者之一,但却经常被出国留学研究所忽视。研究人员对西班牙语英语学习者进行了分班测试,以确定他们的初始L2水平,阅读文本(从中计算流利性和理解力),更新词汇水平测试,以衡量他们的接受性词汇知识,以及一篇书面文章,以记录他们在词汇准确性、流利性、密度和复杂性方面的生产性词汇发展。结果表明,短时间的SA体验对阅读流畅性和理解以及接受性词汇发展都有积极作用。在有效的词汇技能方面,研究结果并不那么积极。最后,研究结果没有显示阅读能力的提高与最初的熟练程度和词汇水平之间的直接关系。
{"title":"L2 reading and vocabulary development after a short Study Abroad experience","authors":"Judith Borràs, Ángels Llanes","doi":"10.35869/vial.v0i17.1464","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.35869/vial.v0i17.1464","url":null,"abstract":"This study explores the development of L2 reading and vocabulary as a result of a short (3-week) SA experience. Given the growing literature on Study Abroad (SA) research, this investigation attempts to shed some light on two rather under-researched areas such as L2 reading and vocabulary, and it does so with a group of adolescents (n= 52), a population often neglected by the SA research despite being one of the most common participants in SA programmes. The participants, Spanish learners of English (L2), were administered a placement test to determine their initial L2 level, a reading text (from which fluency and comprehension were calculated), the Updated Vocabulary Level Test to measure their receptive vocabulary knowledge, and a written essay to capture their productive vocabulary development in terms of lexical accuracy, fluency, density and sophistication. The results indicate that short SA experiences are positive for both reading fluency and comprehension as well as for receptive vocabulary development. Findings are not so positive in terms of productive vocabulary skills. Finally, results failed to show a direct relationship between gains in reading, and initial proficiency and vocabulary level.","PeriodicalId":42598,"journal":{"name":"Vial-Vigo International Journal of Applied Linguistics","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.9,"publicationDate":"2020-01-20","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"80747463","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 3
Developing interactional repertoires in the classroom through dynamic strategic interactio scenarios 通过动态策略互动场景,在课堂上发展互动技能
IF 0.9 4区 文学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2020-01-20 DOI: 10.35869/vial.v0i17.1468
R. A. V. Compernolle, Nuria Ballesteros Soria
In this article, we report on the implementation of Dynamic Strategic Interaction Scenario tasks as one approach to developing L2 learners’ interactional repertoires. The tasks involve pre-task planning, a performance, and immediate feedback during a whole-class debriefing discussion. We focus specifically on the appropriation of turn allocation devices within a single class meeting in which three small groups performed the same scenario. We show how the first group’s performance prompted a focus on turn allocation during their debriefing, and how the subsequent groups were able to build on the feedback in their own performances. We discuss our findings and their implications for research and pedagogy along three dimensions: 1) the role of feedback as mediation in the debriefing discussions; 2) the contribution of task repetition from a group-as-collective perspective; and 3) the documentation of interactional repertoire development over time.
在这篇文章中,我们报告了动态战略互动场景任务的实施,作为发展第二语言学习者互动技能的一种方法。这些任务包括任务前计划、表演和在全班汇报讨论期间的即时反馈。我们特别关注在三个小组执行相同场景的单个班级会议中轮换设备的拨款。我们展示了第一组的表现如何促使他们在汇报过程中关注回合分配,以及随后的组如何能够在他们自己的表现中建立反馈。我们从三个方面讨论了我们的发现及其对研究和教学的影响:1)反馈在述职讨论中的中介作用;2)小组作为集体视角下任务重复的贡献;3)随着时间的推移,交互功能表开发的文档。
{"title":"Developing interactional repertoires in the classroom through dynamic strategic interactio scenarios","authors":"R. A. V. Compernolle, Nuria Ballesteros Soria","doi":"10.35869/vial.v0i17.1468","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.35869/vial.v0i17.1468","url":null,"abstract":"In this article, we report on the implementation of Dynamic Strategic Interaction Scenario tasks as one approach to developing L2 learners’ interactional repertoires. The tasks involve pre-task planning, a performance, and immediate feedback during a whole-class debriefing discussion. We focus specifically on the appropriation of turn allocation devices within a single class meeting in which three small groups performed the same scenario. We show how the first group’s performance prompted a focus on turn allocation during their debriefing, and how the subsequent groups were able to build on the feedback in their own performances. We discuss our findings and their implications for research and pedagogy along three dimensions: 1) the role of feedback as mediation in the debriefing discussions; 2) the contribution of task repetition from a group-as-collective perspective; and 3) the documentation of interactional repertoire development over time.","PeriodicalId":42598,"journal":{"name":"Vial-Vigo International Journal of Applied Linguistics","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.9,"publicationDate":"2020-01-20","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"73965750","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
Does younger mean better? Age of onset, learning rate and shortterm L2 proficiency in young Danish learners of English 年轻就意味着更好吗?丹麦年轻英语学习者的起学年龄、学习率和短期第二语言熟练程度
IF 0.9 4区 文学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2020-01-20 DOI: 10.35869/vial.v0i17.1465
Teresa Cadierno, Mikkel Hansen, J. Lauridsen, S. Eskildsen, K. Fenyvesi, S. Jensen, Maria Vanessa aus der Wieschen
espanolEste trabajo presenta los resultados de un estudio semi-longitudinal que investigo el papel de la edad en el aprendizaje del ingles como lengua extranjera. Se compararon dos grupos de ninos daneses (N = 276) que, siguiendo una reforma educativa del ano 2014, diferian con respecto a la edad en la que comenzaron a recibir ensenanza en ingles. Un grupo comenzo en el primer grado (a la edad de 7-8 anos) mientras que el otro grupo empezo en el tercer grado (a la edad de 9-10 anos). Se utilizaron tres tests receptivos, uno de vocabulario, otro de gramatica y otro que media habilidades de discriminacion fonetica. Estos tests se administraron anualmente permitiendo comparar la rapidez de aprendizaje y el grado de aprendizaje de los dos grupos de ninos despues de uno y dos anos de instruccion. Los resultados del estudio mostraron que los ninos que empezaron mas tarde realizaron la mayor parte de los tests mejor que los ninos que empezaron mas temprano. En relacion a la velocidad de aprendizaje, los tests mostraron que los ninos que empezaron mas tarde tenian una ventaja con respecto al test gramatical mientras que el test de habilidad de discriminacion fonetica mostro un resultado mas difuso con un paron en el desarrollo para los ninos que empezaron mas tarde y un mayor avance de los ninos que empezaron mas temprano. Finalmente, el estudio mostro que los ninos alcanzaron un mayor nivel de dominio linguistico y una mayor velocidad de aprendizaje que las ninas. EnglishThis paper reports the results of a semi-longitudinal study investigating the role of age of onset in early foreign language (English) learning. We compared two groups of Danish school children (N = 276) who, following an educational reform in 2014, started their first English classes the same year but at different ages. One group (the early starters) was introduced to English in the 1st grade (age 7-8) and the other group (the late starters) in the 3rd grade (age 9-10). Children’s receptive vocabulary, receptive grammar, and phonetic discrimination skills were followed for three years, allowing comparisons across groups and time and tracking learning rate and shortterm proficiency (after one and two years of instruction). Results showed that the late starters outperformed the early starters in most tests. With respect to learning rate, the tests also revealed that the late starters had an advantage in the receptive grammar test, whereas the phonetic discrimination test showed a more diffuse picture with the late starters seemingly halting in development and the early starters advancing. The results also showed gender differences with boys achieving a higher level of proficiency and exhibiting a faster learning rate than girls. The pedagogical implications of the results are discussed.
本文介绍了一项半纵向研究的结果,该研究调查了年龄在英语作为外语学习中的作用。我们比较了两组丹麦儿童(N = 276),他们在2014年教育改革后开始接受英语教育的年龄不同。一组从一年级开始(7-8岁),另一组从三年级开始(9-10岁)。他们使用了三种接受测试,一种是词汇测试,一种是语法测试,另一种是语音识别测试。这些测试每年进行一次,以比较两组儿童在一年后和两年后的学习速度和学习程度。这项研究的结果表明,开始较晚的孩子比开始较早的孩子做得更好。学习速度,守护在稍后开始的测试表明,孩子方面有优势的测试语法而测试我的能力歧视fonetica表现出最模糊的结果与paron稍后开始为孩子发展和进步的孩子比开始起得更早。最后,研究表明,与女孩相比,儿童达到了更高水平的语言掌握和更快的学习速度。本文报告了一项半纵向研究的结果,该研究调查了开始年龄在早期外语(英语)学习中的作用。我们比较了两组丹麦学童(N = 276),他们在2014年教育改革后,同一年开始上第一节英语课,但年龄不同。一组(早起者)在一年级(7-8岁)介绍英语,另一组(晚起者)在三年级(9-10岁)介绍英语。对儿童的接受性词汇、接受性语法和语音识别技能进行了三年的跟踪,以便进行跨群体比较,并跟踪学习速度和较短的熟练程度(经过一年和两年的指导)。= =地理= =根据美国人口普查,这个县的总面积为,其中土地和(1.2%)水。在学习速度方面,该测试还显示,晚开始者在接受性语法测试中具有优势,而语音辨别测试显示的情况更为广泛,晚开始者在发展方面似乎停滞不前,而早开始者在进步方面则有所进步。结果也显示出性别差异,男孩的熟练程度更高,学习速度比女孩快。讨论了这些结果的教学意义。
{"title":"Does younger mean better? Age of onset, learning rate and shortterm L2 proficiency in young Danish learners of English","authors":"Teresa Cadierno, Mikkel Hansen, J. Lauridsen, S. Eskildsen, K. Fenyvesi, S. Jensen, Maria Vanessa aus der Wieschen","doi":"10.35869/vial.v0i17.1465","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.35869/vial.v0i17.1465","url":null,"abstract":"espanolEste trabajo presenta los resultados de un estudio semi-longitudinal que investigo el papel de la edad en el aprendizaje del ingles como lengua extranjera. Se compararon dos grupos de ninos daneses (N = 276) que, siguiendo una reforma educativa del ano 2014, diferian con respecto a la edad en la que comenzaron a recibir ensenanza en ingles. Un grupo comenzo en el primer grado (a la edad de 7-8 anos) mientras que el otro grupo empezo en el tercer grado (a la edad de 9-10 anos). Se utilizaron tres tests receptivos, uno de vocabulario, otro de gramatica y otro que media habilidades de discriminacion fonetica. Estos tests se administraron anualmente permitiendo comparar la rapidez de aprendizaje y el grado de aprendizaje de los dos grupos de ninos despues de uno y dos anos de instruccion. Los resultados del estudio mostraron que los ninos que empezaron mas tarde realizaron la mayor parte de los tests mejor que los ninos que empezaron mas temprano. En relacion a la velocidad de aprendizaje, los tests mostraron que los ninos que empezaron mas tarde tenian una ventaja con respecto al test gramatical mientras que el test de habilidad de discriminacion fonetica mostro un resultado mas difuso con un paron en el desarrollo para los ninos que empezaron mas tarde y un mayor avance de los ninos que empezaron mas temprano. Finalmente, el estudio mostro que los ninos alcanzaron un mayor nivel de dominio linguistico y una mayor velocidad de aprendizaje que las ninas. EnglishThis paper reports the results of a semi-longitudinal study investigating the role of age of onset in early foreign language (English) learning. We compared two groups of Danish school children (N = 276) who, following an educational reform in 2014, started their first English classes the same year but at different ages. One group (the early starters) was introduced to English in the 1st grade (age 7-8) and the other group (the late starters) in the 3rd grade (age 9-10). Children’s receptive vocabulary, receptive grammar, and phonetic discrimination skills were followed for three years, allowing comparisons across groups and time and tracking learning rate and shortterm proficiency (after one and two years of instruction). Results showed that the late starters outperformed the early starters in most tests. With respect to learning rate, the tests also revealed that the late starters had an advantage in the receptive grammar test, whereas the phonetic discrimination test showed a more diffuse picture with the late starters seemingly halting in development and the early starters advancing. The results also showed gender differences with boys achieving a higher level of proficiency and exhibiting a faster learning rate than girls. The pedagogical implications of the results are discussed.","PeriodicalId":42598,"journal":{"name":"Vial-Vigo International Journal of Applied Linguistics","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.9,"publicationDate":"2020-01-20","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"76444025","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 10
The Persistence of L1 Patterns in SLA: the Boundary Crossing Constraint and Incidental Learning 二语习得中L1模式的持久性:边界跨越约束与附带学习
IF 0.9 4区 文学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2019-05-03 DOI: 10.35869/VIAL.V0I16.94
Amani S. Alghamdi, M. Daller, James Milton
Based on the theoretical framework of Talmy (1985 et passim) and Slobin (1987 et passim) the present study analyses the influence of L1 patterns on the description of motion events with boundary crossings. Arab speakers avoid the use of manner of motion verbs in the description of these events and use simple path verbs (e.g. enter, go etc.), whereas speakers of English mostly use manner verbs (run, crawl etc.). These deeply engrained differences between L1 and L2 are a learning challenge in SLA. We analyse the use of manner verbs by an intermediate and an advanced group of Arab EFL learners, who live in the UK. Most learners either avoid the description or use path verbs as in Arabic. As the learners do not produce ungrammatical sentences, they will not receive negative feedback (e.g. from a teacher) and rely entirely on incidental learning from the input. However, despite the high frequency of these manner verbs in the daily input of the learners, they do not acquire the patterns of the target language even at a high proficiency level. Implicit learning in this context is hardly possible and explicit teaching and learning is needed to overcome the influence of the first language.
基于Talmy(1985 et passim)和Slobin(1987 et passim。讲阿拉伯语言的人在描述这些事件时避免使用动作方式动词,而是使用简单的路径动词(如enter、go等),而讲英语的人大多使用方式动词(run、crawl等)。L1和L2之间这些根深蒂固的差异是SLA的学习挑战。我们分析了居住在英国的一组中级和高级阿拉伯英语学习者对方式动词的使用。大多数学习者要么避免描述,要么像阿拉伯语一样使用路径动词。由于学习者不会产生不符合语法的句子,他们不会收到负面反馈(例如来自老师),并且完全依赖于从输入中附带学习。然而,尽管这些方式动词在学习者的日常输入中频率很高,但即使在高水平的熟练程度下,他们也无法获得目标语言的模式。在这种情况下,内隐学习是不可能的,需要外显教学来克服第一语言的影响。
{"title":"The Persistence of L1 Patterns in SLA: the Boundary Crossing Constraint and Incidental Learning","authors":"Amani S. Alghamdi, M. Daller, James Milton","doi":"10.35869/VIAL.V0I16.94","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.35869/VIAL.V0I16.94","url":null,"abstract":"Based on the theoretical framework of Talmy (1985 et passim) and Slobin (1987 et passim) the present study analyses the influence of L1 patterns on the description of motion events with boundary crossings. Arab speakers avoid the use of manner of motion verbs in the description of these events and use simple path verbs (e.g. enter, go etc.), whereas speakers of English mostly use manner verbs (run, crawl etc.). These deeply engrained differences between L1 and L2 are a learning challenge in SLA. We analyse the use of manner verbs by an intermediate and an advanced group of Arab EFL learners, who live in the UK. Most learners either avoid the description or use path verbs as in Arabic. As the learners do not produce ungrammatical sentences, they will not receive negative feedback (e.g. from a teacher) and rely entirely on incidental learning from the input. However, despite the high frequency of these manner verbs in the daily input of the learners, they do not acquire the patterns of the target language even at a high proficiency level. Implicit learning in this context is hardly possible and explicit teaching and learning is needed to overcome the influence of the first language.","PeriodicalId":42598,"journal":{"name":"Vial-Vigo International Journal of Applied Linguistics","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.9,"publicationDate":"2019-05-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"44252241","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 4
Dubbing attitudes through tonal patterns: when tones speak louder than words 通过音调模式给态度配音:当音调比语言更响亮时
IF 0.9 4区 文学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2019-05-03 DOI: 10.35869/VIAL.V0I16.96
Sofía Sánchez Mompeán
This article explores the conveyance of attitudinal content through intonation in dubbed dialogue and presents the findings from an empirical corpus-based analysis. Research-wise, intonation is hitherto an understudied topic in Audiovisual Translation and has generally taken a back seat in dubbing literature. However, its communicative value and attitudinal function in oral discourse cannot be overlooked when interpreting and producing dubbed speech. The possibility of associating a particular tonal pattern with specific attitudes has enabled the comparison between a number of English original and Spanish dubbed intonation phrases via a speech analysis software. The results obtained provide empirical data on the dubbing of the attitudinal content under analysis and account for the main trends that could negatively affect both the quality of the final outcome and the way the dubbed text is received by the target audience.
本文探讨了配音对话中语调对态度内容的传递,并提出了基于经验语料库的分析结果。从研究角度来看,语调在视听翻译中一直是一个研究不足的话题,在配音文学中一直处于次要地位。然而,在翻译和制作配音语时,它在口语话语中的交际价值和态度功能是不可忽视的。通过语音分析软件,可以将特定的语调模式与特定的态度联系起来,从而可以对一些英语原版和西班牙语配音语调短语进行比较。所获得的结果为所分析的态度内容的配音提供了经验数据,并解释了可能对最终结果的质量和配音文本被目标受众接受的方式产生负面影响的主要趋势。
{"title":"Dubbing attitudes through tonal patterns: when tones speak louder than words","authors":"Sofía Sánchez Mompeán","doi":"10.35869/VIAL.V0I16.96","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.35869/VIAL.V0I16.96","url":null,"abstract":"This article explores the conveyance of attitudinal content through intonation in dubbed dialogue and presents the findings from an empirical corpus-based analysis. Research-wise, intonation is hitherto an understudied topic in Audiovisual Translation and has generally taken a back seat in dubbing literature. However, its communicative value and attitudinal function in oral discourse cannot be overlooked when interpreting and producing dubbed speech. The possibility of associating a particular tonal pattern with specific attitudes has enabled the comparison between a number of English original and Spanish dubbed intonation phrases via a speech analysis software. The results obtained provide empirical data on the dubbing of the attitudinal content under analysis and account for the main trends that could negatively affect both the quality of the final outcome and the way the dubbed text is received by the target audience.","PeriodicalId":42598,"journal":{"name":"Vial-Vigo International Journal of Applied Linguistics","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.9,"publicationDate":"2019-05-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"42860951","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 4
An n-gram based approach to the automatic classification of schoolchildren’s writing 基于n-gram的小学生写作自动分类方法
IF 0.9 4区 文学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2019-05-03 DOI: 10.35869/VIAL.V0I16.93
Jordi Cicres, Sheila Queralt
This article focuses on the analysis of schoolchildren’s writing (throughout the whole primary school period) using sets of morphological labels (n-grams). We analyzed the sets of bigrams and trigrams from a group of literary texts written by Catalan schoolchildren in order to identify which bigrams and trigrams can help discriminate between texts from the three cycles into which the Spanish primary education system is divided: lower cycle (6- and 7-year-olds), middle cycle (8- and 9-year- olds) and upper cycle (10- and 11-year-olds). The results obtained are close to 70% of correct classifications (77.5% bigrams and 68.6% trigrams), making this technique useful for automatic document classification by age.
本文使用形态标签集(n-grams)对小学生的写作(整个小学时期)进行分析。我们分析了加泰罗尼亚学童撰写的一组文学文本中的八卦和八卦,以确定哪些八卦和八卦有助于区分西班牙小学教育系统分为三个周期的文本:下周期(6岁和7岁)、中周期(8岁和9岁)和上周期(10岁和11岁)。所获得的结果接近70%的正确分类(77.5%的二元图和68.6%的三元图),这使得该技术可用于按年龄自动分类文档。
{"title":"An n-gram based approach to the automatic classification of schoolchildren’s writing","authors":"Jordi Cicres, Sheila Queralt","doi":"10.35869/VIAL.V0I16.93","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.35869/VIAL.V0I16.93","url":null,"abstract":"This article focuses on the analysis of schoolchildren’s writing (throughout the whole primary school period) using sets of morphological labels (n-grams). We analyzed the sets of bigrams and trigrams from a group of literary texts written by Catalan schoolchildren in order to identify which bigrams and trigrams can help discriminate between texts from the three cycles into which the Spanish primary education system is divided: lower cycle (6- and 7-year-olds), middle cycle (8- and 9-year- olds) and upper cycle (10- and 11-year-olds). The results obtained are close to 70% of correct classifications (77.5% bigrams and 68.6% trigrams), making this technique useful for automatic document classification by age.","PeriodicalId":42598,"journal":{"name":"Vial-Vigo International Journal of Applied Linguistics","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.9,"publicationDate":"2019-05-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"43139761","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 3
Readability indices for the assessment of textbooks: a feasibility study in the context of EFL 教材可读性评价指标的可行性研究
IF 0.9 4区 文学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2019-05-03 DOI: 10.35869/VIAL.V0I16.92
Pascual Cantos Gómez, Ángela Almela Sánchez Lafuente
Readability indices have been widely used in order to measure textual difficulty. They can be useful for the automatic classification of texts, especially in language teaching. Among other applications, they allow for the previous determination of the difficulty level of texts without the need of reading them through. The aim of this research is twofold: first, to examine the degree of accuracy of the six most commonly used readability indices, and second, to present a new optimized measure. The main problem is that these readability indices may offer disparity, and this is precisely what has motivated our attempt to unite their potential. A discriminant analysis of all the variables under examination has enabled the creation of a much more precise model, improving the previous best results by 15%. Furthermore, errors and disparities in the difficulty level of the analyzed texts have been detected.
可读性指标被广泛用于衡量文本的难易程度。它们可以用于文本的自动分类,特别是在语言教学中。在其他应用程序中,它们允许预先确定文本的难度级别,而无需通读它们。本研究的目的是双重的:第一,检查六个最常用的可读性指标的准确性程度,第二,提出一个新的优化措施。主要的问题是,这些可读性指数可能会带来差异,而这正是促使我们尝试统一它们的潜力的原因。对所有被检查的变量进行判别分析,可以创建一个更精确的模型,将之前的最佳结果提高15%。此外,在分析文本的难度水平上发现了错误和差异。
{"title":"Readability indices for the assessment of textbooks: a feasibility study in the context of EFL","authors":"Pascual Cantos Gómez, Ángela Almela Sánchez Lafuente","doi":"10.35869/VIAL.V0I16.92","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.35869/VIAL.V0I16.92","url":null,"abstract":"Readability indices have been widely used in order to measure textual difficulty. They can be useful for the automatic classification of texts, especially in language teaching. Among other applications, they allow for the previous determination of the difficulty level of texts without the need of reading them through. The aim of this research is twofold: first, to examine the degree of accuracy of the six most commonly used readability indices, and second, to present a new optimized measure. The main problem is that these readability indices may offer disparity, and this is precisely what has motivated our attempt to unite their potential. A discriminant analysis of all the variables under examination has enabled the creation of a much more precise model, improving the previous best results by 15%. Furthermore, errors and disparities in the difficulty level of the analyzed texts have been detected.","PeriodicalId":42598,"journal":{"name":"Vial-Vigo International Journal of Applied Linguistics","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.9,"publicationDate":"2019-05-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"45746537","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 5
期刊
Vial-Vigo International Journal of Applied Linguistics
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1