首页 > 最新文献

Psychologie Francaise最新文献

英文 中文
Le burn-out est-il une entité nosographique distincte ? 倦怠是一个独立的分类学实体吗?
IF 0.3 4区 心理学 Q4 Psychology Pub Date : 2021-09-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.psfr.2020.07.001
F. Schmid, T. Huyghebaert, A. Bertrand, M. Cartier, K. Deleau, A. Henry, N. Stefaniak

Although it has largely been studied since the 1970s, it remains controversial to accurately define what the concept of burnout encompasses. Indeed, burnout is not consensually defined and, although some consider that it is a work-related illness, there is no clear diagnostic criteria. This problem is intensified by the fact that some studies present methodological limitations, thus impeding an appropriate characterization of burnout. As a consequence of this lack of agreement, burnout is not acknowledged as a work-related illness, which minimizes the pain of those individuals who are suffering from it. The present paper will review findings that can help to disentangle whether burnout should be considered as a medical diagnosis. Cultural differences concerning the consideration of burnout as a medical condition will be pointed out and the distinctiveness of burnout compared to depression, adjustment disorder or other stress related symptoms will be highlighted. Moreover, we will explain why considering that burnout belongs to broader diagnostic categories implies that the weight of employees’ discomfort mainly relies on personal causes and therefore weakens the role of existing working conditions. This paper will also summarize clinical, psychological and biological evidences for and against the inclusion of burnout as a medical condition, before embedding them in a wider social, political and economic context. More specifically, we will explain why burnout is intimately related to the political and economic situation of a country: when society promotes cost-effectiveness, it increases stress and psychological pressure, which makes a burnout more likely to appear. Furthermore, if burnout is recognized as a work-related disease, the consequences for organizations will be that they will have to pay for additional health care charges. Finally, more effective prevention techniques could be developed, and more appropriate care could be provided to patients, but only if an agreement on diagnostic criteria was obtained and if reliable assessment tools were developed. As long as these issues are not addressed, treatments specifically targeting burnout symptoms will be further delayed although it is a serious clinical condition associated with physical and psychological symptoms impairing patients’ quality of life. In conclusion, we suggest that burnout should be considered as a medical condition despite the economic and political impediments. Nevertheless, up to now, several methodological limitations preclude to reliably establish a pathological cut-off and to delineate burnout from other mental illnesses.

尽管自20世纪70年代以来,人们对倦怠进行了大量研究,但准确定义倦怠的概念仍然存在争议。事实上,职业倦怠并不是双方同意的定义,尽管有些人认为这是一种与工作有关的疾病,但没有明确的诊断标准。由于一些研究存在方法上的局限性,从而妨碍了对职业倦怠的适当描述,这一事实使这个问题更加严重。由于缺乏共识,职业倦怠不被认为是一种与工作有关的疾病,这将那些遭受这种痛苦的人的痛苦降到最低。本论文将回顾研究结果,有助于理清倦怠是否应被视为一种医学诊断。将指出将倦怠视为一种医疗状况的文化差异,并强调倦怠与抑郁症、适应障碍或其他压力相关症状相比的独特性。此外,我们将解释为什么考虑到倦怠属于更广泛的诊断类别意味着员工不适的权重主要依赖于个人原因,因此削弱了现有工作条件的作用。本文还将总结临床、心理和生物学证据,支持和反对将职业倦怠作为一种医疗条件,然后将其纳入更广泛的社会、政治和经济背景。更具体地说,我们将解释为什么倦怠与一个国家的政治和经济状况密切相关:当社会促进成本效益时,它会增加压力和心理压力,这使得倦怠更容易出现。此外,如果倦怠被确认为一种与工作有关的疾病,对组织的后果将是,他们将不得不支付额外的医疗费用。最后,可以开发更有效的预防技术,并向病人提供更适当的护理,但前提是就诊断标准达成协议,并开发可靠的评估工具。只要不解决这些问题,专门针对倦怠症状的治疗将进一步延迟,尽管它是一种严重的临床状况,与身体和心理症状相关,损害患者的生活质量。总之,我们建议,尽管存在经济和政治障碍,但应将职业倦怠视为一种医疗状况。然而,到目前为止,一些方法上的限制妨碍了可靠地建立病理分界点和描述倦怠与其他精神疾病的关系。
{"title":"Le burn-out est-il une entité nosographique distincte ?","authors":"F. Schmid,&nbsp;T. Huyghebaert,&nbsp;A. Bertrand,&nbsp;M. Cartier,&nbsp;K. Deleau,&nbsp;A. Henry,&nbsp;N. Stefaniak","doi":"10.1016/j.psfr.2020.07.001","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.psfr.2020.07.001","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Although it has largely been studied since the 1970s, it remains controversial to accurately define what the concept of burnout encompasses. Indeed, burnout is not consensually defined and, although some consider that it is a work-related illness, there is no clear diagnostic criteria. This problem is intensified by the fact that some studies present methodological limitations, thus impeding an appropriate characterization of burnout. As a consequence of this lack of agreement, burnout is not acknowledged as a work-related illness, which minimizes the pain of those individuals who are suffering from it. The present paper will review findings that can help to disentangle whether burnout should be considered as a medical diagnosis. Cultural differences concerning the consideration of burnout as a medical condition will be pointed out and the distinctiveness of burnout compared to depression, adjustment disorder or other stress related symptoms will be highlighted. Moreover, we will explain why considering that burnout belongs to broader diagnostic categories implies that the weight of employees’ discomfort mainly relies on personal causes and therefore weakens the role of existing working conditions. This paper will also summarize clinical, psychological and biological evidences for and against the inclusion of burnout as a medical condition, before embedding them in a wider social, political and economic context. More specifically, we will explain why burnout is intimately related to the political and economic situation of a country: when society promotes cost-effectiveness, it increases stress and psychological pressure, which makes a burnout more likely to appear. Furthermore, if burnout is recognized as a work-related disease, the consequences for organizations will be that they will have to pay for additional health care charges. Finally, more effective prevention techniques could be developed, and more appropriate care could be provided to patients, but only if an agreement on diagnostic criteria was obtained and if reliable assessment tools were developed. As long as these issues are not addressed, treatments specifically targeting burnout symptoms will be further delayed although it is a serious clinical condition associated with physical and psychological symptoms impairing patients’ quality of life. In conclusion, we suggest that burnout should be considered as a medical condition despite the economic and political impediments. Nevertheless, up to now, several methodological limitations preclude to reliably establish a pathological cut-off and to delineate burnout from other mental illnesses.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":44717,"journal":{"name":"Psychologie Francaise","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.3,"publicationDate":"2021-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1016/j.psfr.2020.07.001","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"43838128","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Propriété égolytique des psychédéliques et intérêts dans le traitement de la dépression 迷幻药的利他性和治疗抑郁症的兴趣
IF 0.3 4区 心理学 Q4 Psychology Pub Date : 2021-09-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.psfr.2020.09.007
B. Fauvel , J. Mutlu , P. Piolino

Approximately a third of depressive patients doesn’t respond to classical treatments and never experiences remission, justifying the need for novel therapeutic approaches. Research on the effect of psychedelics on consciousness and their therapeutic use is currently re-exploding. New data confirm that the psychedelic experience with psychological support is well-tolerated and diminishes immediately and durably the symptoms associated with anxiety and depression. This article gathers neurophysiological and psychological arguments which posit that psychedelics’ effect on depressive symptoms is due to their egolytic properties. Ego roles are adaptive by sustaining a sense of personal unity, continuity over time, and by guiding behavior in a coherent manner. However, ego dysfunctions can lead to cognitive biases and schemas involved in several psychopathological conditions, such as depression. Therefore, ego is a cornerstone of psychotherapy. It is proposed that default mode network functions and organization are somewhat consistent with the description of the ego. In major depression, hyperconnectivity of the default mode network's brain areas causes ego-focused and stereotyped symptoms such as ruminations and excessive tendency to self-judgment. On the contrary, psychedelics disrupt the functional connectivity of brain areas belonging to the default mode network, causing ego dissolution and releasing consciousness from its constrains. As a result, a flow of supplementary bottom-up limbic information can reach consciousness and the subject experiences a mystical sense of connectedness with what is usually seen as external. Even if the psychedelic state is transitory, its benefits on depressive symptoms persist over time. The reason is that the psychedelic experience leads to phenomena of insights and long-term perspective shifts. Thus, psychedelic-assisted psychotherapies appear as a promising option in the treatment of treatment-resistant depressions. Research on the effects of psychedelics in human is an exciting and promising field, which might continue to yield interesting results regarding their psychotherapeutic potentials and the link between brain and consciousness.

大约有三分之一的抑郁症患者对传统治疗没有反应,也从未经历过缓解,这证明需要新的治疗方法。关于致幻剂对意识的影响及其治疗用途的研究目前正在重新爆发。新的数据证实,在心理支持下的迷幻体验具有良好的耐受性,并能立即持久地减轻与焦虑和抑郁相关的症状。这篇文章收集了神经生理学和心理学的论点,假设迷幻药对抑郁症状的影响是由于它们的溶血特性。自我角色是适应性的,通过维持个人的统一性,随着时间的推移的连续性,并通过以连贯的方式指导行为。然而,自我功能障碍会导致认知偏差和图式,涉及到一些精神病理状况,如抑郁症。因此,自我是心理治疗的基石。提出默认模式的网络功能和组织与自我的描述有一定的一致性。在重度抑郁症中,默认模式网络的大脑区域的超连通性导致自我关注和刻板印象症状,如沉思和过度的自我判断倾向。相反,迷幻药破坏了属于默认模式网络的大脑区域的功能连接,导致自我解体,并将意识从束缚中释放出来。因此,一股自下而上的补充边缘信息流可以到达意识,主体体验到一种与通常被视为外部的东西联系在一起的神秘感觉。即使迷幻状态是短暂的,它对抑郁症状的好处也会持续一段时间。原因是迷幻的体验会导致洞察力和长期视角的转变。因此,迷幻剂辅助心理疗法在治疗难治性抑郁症中似乎是一个很有前途的选择。迷幻药对人类的影响研究是一个令人兴奋和有前途的领域,它可能会继续产生有趣的结果,关于它们的心理治疗潜力和大脑与意识之间的联系。
{"title":"Propriété égolytique des psychédéliques et intérêts dans le traitement de la dépression","authors":"B. Fauvel ,&nbsp;J. Mutlu ,&nbsp;P. Piolino","doi":"10.1016/j.psfr.2020.09.007","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.psfr.2020.09.007","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Approximately a third of depressive patients doesn’t respond to classical treatments and never experiences remission, justifying the need for novel therapeutic approaches. Research on the effect of psychedelics on consciousness and their therapeutic use is currently re-exploding. New data confirm that the psychedelic experience with psychological support is well-tolerated and diminishes immediately and durably the symptoms associated with anxiety and depression. This article gathers neurophysiological and psychological arguments which posit that psychedelics’ effect on depressive symptoms is due to their egolytic properties. Ego roles are adaptive by sustaining a sense of personal unity, continuity over time, and by guiding behavior in a coherent manner. However, ego dysfunctions can lead to cognitive biases and schemas involved in several psychopathological conditions, such as depression. Therefore, ego is a cornerstone of psychotherapy. It is proposed that default mode network functions and organization are somewhat consistent with the description of the ego. In major depression, hyperconnectivity of the default mode network's brain areas causes ego-focused and stereotyped symptoms such as ruminations and excessive tendency to self-judgment. On the contrary, psychedelics disrupt the functional connectivity of brain areas belonging to the default mode network, causing ego dissolution and releasing consciousness from its constrains. As a result, a flow of supplementary bottom-up limbic information can reach consciousness and the subject experiences a mystical sense of connectedness with what is usually seen as external. Even if the psychedelic state is transitory, its benefits on depressive symptoms persist over time. The reason is that the psychedelic experience leads to phenomena of insights and long-term perspective shifts. Thus, psychedelic-assisted psychotherapies appear as a promising option in the treatment of treatment-resistant depressions. Research on the effects of psychedelics in human is an exciting and promising field, which might continue to yield interesting results regarding their psychotherapeutic potentials and the link between brain and consciousness.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":44717,"journal":{"name":"Psychologie Francaise","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.3,"publicationDate":"2021-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1016/j.psfr.2020.09.007","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"48134277","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Réguler ses émotions quand on apprend, oui mais pourquoi ? Motivations à réguler ses émotions, buts émotionnels et stratégies de régulation émotionnelle mises en œuvre par des étudiants universitaires primo-arrivants en situation de préparation d’examen 在学习的时候调节情绪,是的,但为什么?大学生在考试准备情境中调节情绪的动机、情绪目标和情绪调节策略
IF 0.3 4区 心理学 Q4 Psychology Pub Date : 2021-09-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.psfr.2020.10.001
Line Fischer , Pierre Philippot , Marc Romainville

Studying for the first exam period is a big challenge for freshmen students, especially because they must be able to regulate emotions emerging from this new learning situation. Indeed, it is now recognized that cognitions and emotions interact in learning and that emotion can hinder or support it. However, we argue that it is not only emotions per se but rather how students manage them in the targeted situation (i.e. their emotional regulation skills) that impacts students’ adaptation to this academic context. Using an online survey, this study explored motives in emotion regulation, emotion goals and concrete emotion regulation strategies implemented by students during the preparation of a significant course evaluation. It focuses both on “why” students engage in emotion regulation in the target situation and on “how” this regulation is implemented. A thematic content analysis, processing the data of the 235 respondents, indicates that different motivations in emotion regulation are present among students (hedonic and instrumental motivations to regulate emotions) and that these motivations can be plural among the same students. When instrumental and hedonic motivations are both reported, although the students’ discourse argues that hedonic motivation (feeling good/better) is at the service of instrumental motivation (studying the course), concrete SRE prioritize well-being, through distraction from the course, more than the study of the course (the SRE rarely supports learning). In addition, the most reported emotion regulation strategy is distraction from the course (taking a break and doing something to distract yourself from the course), even in the absence of motivational conflicts. As a result, the theoretical model of motivated regulation (Tamir, 2009; Tamir, 2015) applied to this learning situation offers an innovative reading of why and how university students attempt to manage their emotions in order to learn successfully. Although the current study approaches only the conscious side of emotion regulation, it provides an original perspective on this complex phenomenon without ignoring the context in which it emerges. Finally, this insight should help students, teachers and educational coaches to see emotion regulation as necessary for learning and to set up pedagogical and coaching practices that support the development of SRE, adapted to the learning situation and linked to the emotional states that students wish to experiment in order to optimize learning.

对于大一新生来说,准备第一次考试是一个很大的挑战,特别是因为他们必须能够调节这种新的学习环境中出现的情绪。事实上,现在已经认识到,认知和情感在学习中相互作用,情感可以阻碍或支持学习。然而,我们认为,影响学生适应这种学术环境的不仅仅是情绪本身,而是学生在目标情境中如何管理情绪(即他们的情绪调节技能)。本研究采用网络问卷调查的方式,探讨学生在重大课程评价准备过程中情绪调节动机、情绪目标和具体情绪调节策略。它既关注“为什么”学生在目标情境中进行情绪调节,也关注“如何”实施这种调节。通过对235名被调查者的数据进行主题内容分析,发现学生在情绪调节方面存在不同的动机(调节情绪的享乐动机和工具性动机),并且这些动机在同一学生中可以是多元的。当工具动机和享乐动机都被报告时,尽管学生的话语认为享乐动机(感觉良好/更好)是为工具动机(学习课程)服务的,但具体的SRE通过分散对课程的注意力来优先考虑幸福感,而不是对课程的学习(SRE很少支持学习)。此外,报道最多的情绪调节策略是分散注意力(休息一下,做一些事情来分散注意力),即使没有动机冲突。因此,动机性监管理论模型(Tamir, 2009;Tamir, 2015)对这种学习情况的应用提供了一种创新的解读,即大学生为什么以及如何试图管理自己的情绪以成功地学习。虽然目前的研究只涉及情绪调节的意识方面,但它在不忽视其产生的背景的情况下,为这一复杂现象提供了一个原始的视角。最后,这种见解应该有助于学生、教师和教育教练认识到情绪调节对学习是必要的,并建立支持SRE发展的教学和指导实践,适应学习情况,并与学生希望体验的情绪状态相联系,以优化学习。
{"title":"Réguler ses émotions quand on apprend, oui mais pourquoi ? Motivations à réguler ses émotions, buts émotionnels et stratégies de régulation émotionnelle mises en œuvre par des étudiants universitaires primo-arrivants en situation de préparation d’examen","authors":"Line Fischer ,&nbsp;Pierre Philippot ,&nbsp;Marc Romainville","doi":"10.1016/j.psfr.2020.10.001","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.psfr.2020.10.001","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Studying for the first exam period is a big challenge for freshmen students, especially because they must be able to regulate emotions emerging from this new learning situation. Indeed, it is now recognized that cognitions and emotions interact in learning and that emotion can hinder or support it. However, we argue that it is not only emotions per se but rather how students manage them in the targeted situation (i.e. their emotional regulation skills) that impacts students’ adaptation to this academic context. Using an online survey, this study explored motives in emotion regulation, emotion goals and concrete emotion regulation strategies implemented by students during the preparation of a significant course evaluation. It focuses both on “why” students engage in emotion regulation in the target situation and on “how” this regulation is implemented. A thematic content analysis, processing the data of the 235 respondents, indicates that different motivations in emotion regulation are present among students (hedonic and instrumental motivations to regulate emotions) and that these motivations can be plural among the same students. When instrumental and hedonic motivations are both reported, although the students’ discourse argues that hedonic motivation (feeling good/better) is at the service of instrumental motivation (studying the course), concrete SRE prioritize well-being, through distraction from the course, more than the study of the course (the SRE rarely supports learning). In addition, the most reported emotion regulation strategy is distraction from the course (taking a break and doing something to distract yourself from the course), even in the absence of motivational conflicts. As a result, the theoretical model of motivated regulation (Tamir, 2009; Tamir, 2015) applied to this learning situation offers an innovative reading of why and how university students attempt to manage their emotions in order to learn successfully. Although the current study approaches only the conscious side of emotion regulation, it provides an original perspective on this complex phenomenon without ignoring the context in which it emerges. Finally, this insight should help students, teachers and educational coaches to see emotion regulation as necessary for learning and to set up pedagogical and coaching practices that support the development of SRE, adapted to the learning situation and linked to the emotional states that students wish to experiment in order to optimize learning.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":44717,"journal":{"name":"Psychologie Francaise","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.3,"publicationDate":"2021-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1016/j.psfr.2020.10.001","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"48413209","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
Effet du niveau d’études sur les performances en mémoire épisodique chez des adultes âgés : rôle médiateur de la métamémoire 研究水平对老年人偶发性记忆表现的影响:元记忆的中介作用
IF 0.3 4区 心理学 Q4 Psychology Pub Date : 2021-06-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.psfr.2017.05.002
L. Guerrero-Sastoque , B. Bouazzaoui , L. Burger , L. Taconnat

Educational level is a factor of cognitive reserve and older adults with a higher level of formal education have a better memory performance than those having a lower educational level (Angel et al., 2010; Van Der Elst, Van Boxtel, Van Breukelen, & Jolles, 2005). Memory functioning can also be modulated by the beliefs and knowledge of a person about his/her own memory, that is, by his/her metamemory (Hultsch, Hertzog, & Dixon, 1987). The objective of this study was to examine the role of metamemory as a potential mediator of the effect of educational level on memory performance. Eighty-three older adults (60–80 years) participated in the experiment, they have been divided into two subgroups according to their educational level (high: 14.36 years and low level: 9.85 years). Episodic memory was evaluated with a cued recall task and metamemory by the Metamemory in Adulthood questionnaire (MIA). As shown by previous studies, results indicated that educational level had a significant effect on memory and metamemory, higher educational level was associated to better memory and metamemory capacities. At the MIA questionnaire, older adults with a high educational level affirmed using more internal and external strategies for learning, having higher motivation and perceiving less memory change with aging than older adults with a lower educational level. They also showed that the metamemory dimensions associated to the memory performance differed according to the educational level. For participants with a lower educational level, memory performance was correlated to the participants’ perception about their memory capacity and their knowledge about memory tasks, while for participants with a higher educational level, memory performance was correlated to the dimensions linked to memory control (strategies and motivation). Finally, the group effect was mediated by metamemory, specifically by the use of internal strategies. These results suggest that a prolonged educative experience would be associated to a better capacity to implement adapted strategies, which led individuals to maintain an optimal memory performance.

受教育程度是认知储备的一个因素,受教育程度较高的老年人的记忆表现优于受教育程度较低的老年人(Angel et al., 2010;范·德·埃尔斯特、范·博克斯特尔、范·布鲁克伦等人;Jolles, 2005)。记忆功能也可以通过一个人对自己记忆的信念和认识来调节,也就是说,通过他/她的元记忆来调节(Hultsch, Hertzog, &迪克森,1987)。本研究的目的是探讨元记忆作为教育水平对记忆表现影响的潜在中介的作用。83名老年人(60-80岁)参加了实验,根据他们的受教育程度分为两组(高14.36岁,低9.85岁)。情景记忆采用线索回忆任务和元记忆量表(MIA)进行评估。以往的研究结果表明,受教育程度对记忆和元记忆有显著影响,受教育程度越高,记忆和元记忆能力越强。在MIA问卷中,高教育水平的老年人比低教育水平的老年人使用更多的内部和外部学习策略,有更高的动机和更少的记忆随年龄变化。他们还表明,与记忆表现相关的元记忆维度因教育水平的不同而不同。对于受教育程度较低的被试,记忆表现与被试对自己记忆容量的认知和对记忆任务的了解相关;而对于受教育程度较高的被试,记忆表现与记忆控制相关的维度(策略和动机)相关。最后,群体效应是由元记忆介导的,特别是内部策略的使用。这些结果表明,长时间的教育经历与更好的实施适应策略的能力有关,这使得个人保持最佳的记忆表现。
{"title":"Effet du niveau d’études sur les performances en mémoire épisodique chez des adultes âgés : rôle médiateur de la métamémoire","authors":"L. Guerrero-Sastoque ,&nbsp;B. Bouazzaoui ,&nbsp;L. Burger ,&nbsp;L. Taconnat","doi":"10.1016/j.psfr.2017.05.002","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psfr.2017.05.002","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Educational level is a factor of cognitive reserve and older adults with a higher level of formal education have a better memory performance than those having a lower educational level (Angel et al., 2010; Van Der Elst, Van Boxtel, Van Breukelen, &amp; Jolles, 2005). Memory functioning can also be modulated by the beliefs and knowledge of a person about his/her own memory, that is, by his/her metamemory (Hultsch, Hertzog, &amp; Dixon, 1987). The objective of this study was to examine the role of metamemory as a potential mediator of the effect of educational level on memory performance. Eighty-three older adults (60–80 years) participated in the experiment, they have been divided into two subgroups according to their educational level (high: 14.36 years and low level: 9.85 years). Episodic memory was evaluated with a cued recall task and metamemory by the Metamemory in Adulthood questionnaire (MIA). As shown by previous studies, results indicated that educational level had a significant effect on memory and metamemory, higher educational level was associated to better memory and metamemory capacities. At the MIA questionnaire, older adults with a high educational level affirmed using more internal and external strategies for learning, having higher motivation and perceiving less memory change with aging than older adults with a lower educational level. They also showed that the metamemory dimensions associated to the memory performance differed according to the educational level. For participants with a lower educational level, memory performance was correlated to the participants’ perception about their memory capacity and their knowledge about memory tasks, while for participants with a higher educational level, memory performance was correlated to the dimensions linked to memory control (strategies and motivation). Finally, the group effect was mediated by metamemory, specifically by the use of internal strategies. These results suggest that a prolonged educative experience would be associated to a better capacity to implement adapted strategies, which led individuals to maintain an optimal memory performance.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":44717,"journal":{"name":"Psychologie Francaise","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.3,"publicationDate":"2021-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1016/j.psfr.2017.05.002","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"91709387","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 8
Appel à articles pour un numéro spécial sur la pandémie du Covid-19 为关于Covid-19大流行的特刊征集文章
IF 0.3 4区 心理学 Q4 Psychology Pub Date : 2021-06-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.psfr.2021.04.001
{"title":"Appel à articles pour un numéro spécial sur la pandémie du Covid-19","authors":"","doi":"10.1016/j.psfr.2021.04.001","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psfr.2021.04.001","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":44717,"journal":{"name":"Psychologie Francaise","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.3,"publicationDate":"2021-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1016/j.psfr.2021.04.001","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"91709376","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The influence of training task stimuli on transfer effects of working memory training in aging 训练任务刺激对老年人工作记忆训练迁移效应的影响
IF 0.3 4区 心理学 Q4 Psychology Pub Date : 2021-06-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.psfr.2017.04.005
A. Cantarella , E. Borella , B. Carretti , M. Kliegel , N. Mammarella , B. Fairfield , R. De Beni

Introduction

Working memory (WM) training is known to produce benefits in older adults’ WM. Transfer effects to untrained abilities, however, remain controversial and several aspects are thought to influence the generalization of benefits, including the kind of stimuli used in the training tasks, an aspect rarely addressed in older adults.

Objective

The present study had two aims: (1) to test the efficacy of a visuospatial WM training procedure in older adults, in terms of specific and transfer effects; (2) to examine in two experiments whether the type of stimuli used in the training task influences the training's effectiveness. Experiment 1 adopted images with a neutral valence while experiment 2 used emotionally positive images based on evidence that older adults tend to remember positive stimuli better. In both experiments, specific training-related gains in a visuospatial WM task (the criterion task) and transfer effects on measures of verbal WM, visuospatial short-term memory, processing speed and reasoning were examined. Maintenance of training benefits was also assessed at an 8-month follow-up.

Method

Seventy older adult (63–75 years old) volunteers (35 for experiment 1, and 35 for experiment 2) were randomly assigned to a training or active control group. The same visuospatial WM training procedure was used in both experiments, manipulating only the type of stimuli used (neutral in experiment 1 and emotionally positive in experiment 2).

Results

In both experiments, only trained participants showed specific benefits in the WM criterion task. These gains were also maintained at the follow-up, but no transfer effects were identified.

Conclusion

Overall, our findings using the present visuospatial WM training paradigm suggest that it is less effective, in terms of transfer effects, than the same paradigm administered verbally in a previous study, regardless of the type of stimuli used in WM training tasks (neutral or emotionally positive stimuli).

众所周知,工作记忆(WM)训练对老年人的WM有好处。然而,对未经训练的能力的转移效应仍然存在争议,有几个方面被认为影响益处的普遍化,包括在训练任务中使用的刺激类型,这一点很少在老年人中得到解决。目的本研究有两个目的:(1)检验视觉空间WM训练程序在老年人的特异性和转移效应方面的有效性;(2)在两个实验中检验训练任务中使用的刺激类型是否影响训练的有效性。实验1采用中性效价的图像,实验2采用情绪积极的图像,这是基于有证据表明老年人倾向于更好地记住积极的刺激。在这两个实验中,特定训练相关的增益在视觉空间WM任务(标准任务)和转移效应对言语WM、视觉空间短期记忆、处理速度和推理的测量进行了检验。在8个月的随访中也评估了培训收益的维持情况。方法将70名年龄在63 ~ 75岁之间的老年志愿者(实验1 35人,实验2 35人)随机分为训练组和活动对照组。两个实验都使用了相同的视觉空间WM训练程序,只操纵所使用的刺激类型(实验1为中性刺激,实验2为情绪积极刺激)。结果在两个实验中,只有经过训练的参与者在WM标准任务中表现出特定的优势。这些收益在随访中也保持不变,但没有发现转移效应。总的来说,我们的研究结果表明,无论在WM训练任务中使用何种类型的刺激(中性或情绪积极刺激),当前的视觉空间WM训练范式在迁移效应方面都不如先前研究中使用的相同语言范式有效。
{"title":"The influence of training task stimuli on transfer effects of working memory training in aging","authors":"A. Cantarella ,&nbsp;E. Borella ,&nbsp;B. Carretti ,&nbsp;M. Kliegel ,&nbsp;N. Mammarella ,&nbsp;B. Fairfield ,&nbsp;R. De Beni","doi":"10.1016/j.psfr.2017.04.005","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.psfr.2017.04.005","url":null,"abstract":"<div><h3>Introduction</h3><p>Working memory (WM) training is known to produce benefits in older adults’ WM. Transfer effects to untrained abilities, however, remain controversial and several aspects are thought to influence the generalization of benefits, including the kind of stimuli used in the training tasks, an aspect rarely addressed in older adults.</p></div><div><h3>Objective</h3><p>The present study had two aims: (1) to test the efficacy of a visuospatial WM training procedure in older adults, in terms of specific and transfer effects; (2) to examine in two experiments whether the type of stimuli used in the training task influences the training's effectiveness. Experiment 1 adopted images with a neutral valence while experiment 2 used emotionally positive images based on evidence that older adults tend to remember positive stimuli better. In both experiments, specific training-related gains in a visuospatial WM task (the criterion task) and transfer effects on measures of verbal WM, visuospatial short-term memory, processing speed and reasoning were examined. Maintenance of training benefits was also assessed at an 8-month follow-up.</p></div><div><h3>Method</h3><p>Seventy older adult (63–75 years old) volunteers (35 for experiment 1, and 35 for experiment 2) were randomly assigned to a training or active control group. The same visuospatial WM training procedure was used in both experiments, manipulating only the type of stimuli used (neutral in experiment 1 and emotionally positive in experiment 2).</p></div><div><h3>Results</h3><p>In both experiments, only trained participants showed specific benefits in the WM criterion task. These gains were also maintained at the follow-up, but no transfer effects were identified.</p></div><div><h3>Conclusion</h3><p>Overall, our findings using the present visuospatial WM training paradigm suggest that it is less effective, in terms of transfer effects, than the same paradigm administered verbally in a previous study, regardless of the type of stimuli used in WM training tasks (neutral or emotionally positive stimuli).</p></div>","PeriodicalId":44717,"journal":{"name":"Psychologie Francaise","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.3,"publicationDate":"2021-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1016/j.psfr.2017.04.005","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"44002693","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 3
Stimulation cognitive chez les personnes âgées : effets d’une méthode de stimulation cognitive par les jeux sur les fonctions cognitives et l’estime de soi 老年人的认知刺激:游戏认知刺激方法对认知功能和自尊的影响
IF 0.3 4区 心理学 Q4 Psychology Pub Date : 2021-06-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.psfr.2019.11.002
E. Grimaud, D. Clarys, S. Vanneste, L. Taconnat

The purpose of this study was to examine the effects of a cognitive stimulation program with games on scores to cognitive tests (processing speed, mental flexibility, working memory, inhibition) and how this program can benefit to a psycho-affective measure, self-esteem in older adults. Forty-eight participants over 60 years old took part in the experiment. They were divided into two groups: 1 group followed a program of cognitive stimulation using leisure activities with games and 1 control group in which people gathered every week. There were 8 sessions of cognitive stimulation using leisure activities like games, one-hour session a week. Measures have focused on speed of processing and executive functions (shifting, updating and inhibition). They have been evaluated before and after the training program. Results show that the cognitive stimulation program using leisure activities with games is effective on speed of processing, memory span, inhibition and self-esteem but shows no benefits on shifting and updating. These results indicate that it seems to be possible to enhance cognitive resources, inhibition and self-esteem using leisure activities with games as a tool for cognitive stimulation.

本研究的目的是检验带有游戏的认知刺激程序对认知测试分数(处理速度、心理灵活性、工作记忆、抑制)的影响,以及该程序如何有益于老年人的心理情感测量——自尊。48名60岁以上的参与者参加了这项实验。他们被分为两组:一组通过休闲活动和游戏进行认知刺激,另一组每周聚会。有8次认知刺激,使用休闲活动,比如游戏,每周一小时。措施侧重于处理速度和执行功能(转移、更新和抑制)。他们在培训项目前后都接受了评估。结果表明,休闲活动与游戏相结合的认知刺激方案在加工速度、记忆广度、抑制和自尊方面有显著效果,但在转移和更新方面无显著效果。这些结果表明,利用休闲活动和游戏作为认知刺激的工具,似乎有可能增强认知资源、抑制和自尊。
{"title":"Stimulation cognitive chez les personnes âgées : effets d’une méthode de stimulation cognitive par les jeux sur les fonctions cognitives et l’estime de soi","authors":"E. Grimaud,&nbsp;D. Clarys,&nbsp;S. Vanneste,&nbsp;L. Taconnat","doi":"10.1016/j.psfr.2019.11.002","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psfr.2019.11.002","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>The purpose of this study was to examine the effects of a cognitive stimulation program with games on scores to cognitive tests (processing speed, mental flexibility, working memory, inhibition) and how this program can benefit to a psycho-affective measure, self-esteem in older adults. Forty-eight participants over 60 years old took part in the experiment. They were divided into two groups: 1 group followed a program of cognitive stimulation using leisure activities with games and 1 control group in which people gathered every week. There were 8 sessions of cognitive stimulation using leisure activities like games, one-hour session a week. Measures have focused on speed of processing and executive functions (shifting, updating and inhibition). They have been evaluated before and after the training program. Results show that the cognitive stimulation program using leisure activities with games is effective on speed of processing, memory span, inhibition and self-esteem but shows no benefits on shifting and updating. These results indicate that it seems to be possible to enhance cognitive resources, inhibition and self-esteem using leisure activities with games as a tool for cognitive stimulation.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":44717,"journal":{"name":"Psychologie Francaise","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.3,"publicationDate":"2021-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1016/j.psfr.2019.11.002","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"91746952","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Diversité des facteurs de réserve 储备因素的多样性
IF 0.3 4区 心理学 Q4 Psychology Pub Date : 2021-06-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.psfr.2021.03.003
L. Taconnat, B. Bouazzaoui
{"title":"Diversité des facteurs de réserve","authors":"L. Taconnat,&nbsp;B. Bouazzaoui","doi":"10.1016/j.psfr.2021.03.003","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.psfr.2021.03.003","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":44717,"journal":{"name":"Psychologie Francaise","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.3,"publicationDate":"2021-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1016/j.psfr.2021.03.003","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"44486260","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Connaissances et contrôle exécutif : deux facteurs cognitifs de protection contre le vieillissement de la mémoire épisodique ? 知识和执行控制:防止情景记忆老化的两个认知因素?
IF 0.3 4区 心理学 Q4 Psychology Pub Date : 2021-06-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.psfr.2017.03.001
S. Gombart , S. Fay , M. Isingrini

In this study we refer to Craik and Bialystok's model distinguishing the “knowledge” (cultural learning experience that forms the basis for knowledge of the world) and the “executive control” (set of operations that control and regulate cognitive performance) as two main factors susceptible accounting for the age-related negative effects on the cognitive functioning, and for a possible age-related protective effect. Thus, the aim of the present study was to examine the possible protective effect of these two factors on the age-related decline in a cued-recall task. Four age groups of participants (20–39 years, 40–59 years, 60–74 years and 75–90 years) were administered with a word-stem cued-recall test to assess episodic memory, a “vocabulary” sub-test of the WAIS-R to assess knowledge and a reading span test to assess control. Results showed an age-related effect on the episodic memory, the control performance and the knowledge measure, indicating that performance decreases with age. A GLM analysis revealed a positive effect of the knowledge and the control factors on the cued-recall performance, and interaction between age and the knowledge factor resulting from a positive effect of the knowledge level only for the youngest group of participants (20–39 years). Furthermore, results revealed an interaction between age and the control factor, indicating a positive effect of the control level for the two oldest groups of participants (60–74 years and 75–90 years). These results suggest that, contrary to the knowledge, the control factor could be an effective protection against the age-related negative effects on the episodic memory.

在本研究中,我们参考了Craik和Bialystok的模型,将“知识”(构成世界知识基础的文化学习经验)和“执行控制”(控制和调节认知表现的一套操作)作为两个主要因素,可以解释与年龄相关的认知功能负面影响,并可能产生与年龄相关的保护作用。因此,本研究的目的是检验这两个因素对提示回忆任务中与年龄相关的衰退的可能保护作用。4个年龄组的参与者(20-39岁、40-59岁、60-74岁和75-90岁)接受了单词干线索回忆测试来评估情景记忆,WAIS-R的“词汇”子测试来评估知识,阅读广度测试来评估控制。结果显示,情景记忆、控制表现和知识测量均存在年龄相关的影响,表明表现随年龄增长而下降。GLM分析显示,知识和控制因素对线索回忆的表现有正向影响,而年龄和知识因素之间的交互作用是由知识水平的正向影响引起的,仅在最年轻的参与者组(20-39岁)。此外,结果还揭示了年龄与控制因素之间的相互作用,表明控制水平对两组年龄最大的参与者(60-74岁和75-90岁)有积极影响。这些结果表明,与以往的认识相反,控制因素可以有效地防止年龄对情景记忆的负面影响。
{"title":"Connaissances et contrôle exécutif : deux facteurs cognitifs de protection contre le vieillissement de la mémoire épisodique ?","authors":"S. Gombart ,&nbsp;S. Fay ,&nbsp;M. Isingrini","doi":"10.1016/j.psfr.2017.03.001","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.psfr.2017.03.001","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>In this study we refer to Craik and Bialystok's model distinguishing the “knowledge” (cultural learning experience that forms the basis for knowledge of the world) and the “executive control” (set of operations that control and regulate cognitive performance) as two main factors susceptible accounting for the age-related negative effects on the cognitive functioning, and for a possible age-related protective effect. Thus, the aim of the present study was to examine the possible protective effect of these two factors on the age-related decline in a cued-recall task. Four age groups of participants (20–39 years, 40–59 years, 60–74 years and 75–90 years) were administered with a word-stem cued-recall test to assess episodic memory, a “vocabulary” sub-test of the WAIS-R to assess knowledge and a reading span test to assess control. Results showed an age-related effect on the episodic memory, the control performance and the knowledge measure, indicating that performance decreases with age. A GLM analysis revealed a positive effect of the knowledge and the control factors on the cued-recall performance, and interaction between age and the knowledge factor resulting from a positive effect of the knowledge level only for the youngest group of participants (20–39 years). Furthermore, results revealed an interaction between age and the control factor, indicating a positive effect of the control level for the two oldest groups of participants (60–74 years and 75–90 years). These results suggest that, contrary to the knowledge, the control factor could be an effective protection against the age-related negative effects on the episodic memory.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":44717,"journal":{"name":"Psychologie Francaise","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.3,"publicationDate":"2021-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1016/j.psfr.2017.03.001","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"47632723","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
La pratique répétée du Trail Making Test réduit les différences liées à l’âge sur la performance à ce test 重复的越轨测试减少了与年龄相关的测试表现差异
IF 0.3 4区 心理学 Q4 Psychology Pub Date : 2021-06-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.psfr.2017.03.002
L. Burger , L. Taconnat , L. Angel , P. Plusquellec , S. Fay

Age-related impairment in executive functioning has been found to explain partially the decrease of cognitive performance with aging. However, the practice of an executive test can improve performance to this test. In the present study, we investigated first how the practice of the Trail Making Test (TMT, flexibility test) may influence the age-related deficit to the performance in this test and then how the performance tended to improve, and at which pace, through the practice of this executive test. Two age groups of participants (young and older adults) practiced the TMT and were compared to two control groups (i.e., no practice between pre- and post-test). The practice groups’ scores were obtained at the end of each session. Globally, the results showed an improvement of performance (1) greater in the practice groups than in the control groups and (2) in practice groups, greater in older adults than in younger ones. Both younger and older adults progressed during the early practice sessions but the younger ones reached their optimal level earlier than the older adults who continued to improve over the sessions. These results could have a major impact on adapting cognitive stimulation programs to individual's characteristics such as age.

与年龄相关的执行功能障碍已被发现可以部分解释随着年龄增长认知能力下降的原因。然而,执行测试的实践可以提高该测试的性能。在本研究中,我们首先考察了柔韧性测验(TMT, Trail Making Test)的练习如何影响年龄相关的测试成绩缺陷,然后通过这项执行测试的练习,研究了年龄相关的测试成绩如何倾向于提高,以及以何种速度提高。两个年龄组的参与者(年轻人和老年人)练习TMT,并与两个对照组(即在测试前和测试后之间没有练习)进行比较。练习组的分数在每次练习结束时获得。在全球范围内,结果显示:(1)练习组的表现比对照组更好,(2)练习组的老年人比年轻人更好。年轻人和老年人在早期的练习中都取得了进步,但年轻人比老年人更早达到最佳水平,老年人在练习过程中继续提高。这些结果可能会对使认知刺激方案适应个人特征(如年龄)产生重大影响。
{"title":"La pratique répétée du Trail Making Test réduit les différences liées à l’âge sur la performance à ce test","authors":"L. Burger ,&nbsp;L. Taconnat ,&nbsp;L. Angel ,&nbsp;P. Plusquellec ,&nbsp;S. Fay","doi":"10.1016/j.psfr.2017.03.002","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.psfr.2017.03.002","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Age-related impairment in executive functioning has been found to explain partially the decrease of cognitive performance with aging. However, the practice of an executive test can improve performance to this test. In the present study, we investigated first how the practice of the Trail Making Test (TMT, flexibility test) may influence the age-related deficit to the performance in this test and then how the performance tended to improve, and at which pace, through the practice of this executive test. Two age groups of participants (young and older adults) practiced the TMT and were compared to two control groups (i.e., no practice between pre- and post-test). The practice groups’ scores were obtained at the end of each session. Globally, the results showed an improvement of performance (1) greater in the practice groups than in the control groups and (2) in practice groups, greater in older adults than in younger ones. Both younger and older adults progressed during the early practice sessions but the younger ones reached their optimal level earlier than the older adults who continued to improve over the sessions. These results could have a major impact on adapting cognitive stimulation programs to individual's characteristics such as age.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":44717,"journal":{"name":"Psychologie Francaise","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.3,"publicationDate":"2021-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1016/j.psfr.2017.03.002","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"46336497","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
期刊
Psychologie Francaise
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1