首页 > 最新文献

Mental Lexicon最新文献

英文 中文
Effects of phonological and talker familiarity on second language lexical development 语音和说话人熟悉度对二语词汇发展的影响
IF 0.6 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2022-03-18 DOI: 10.1075/ml.20024.liu
Jiang Liu, Seth Wiener
Previous research has shown that second language (L2) learners of Mandarin learn new words more easily if the new word is homophonous with a word they already know (Liu and Wiener, 2020). That research involved word learning in which speech was produced by a single-talker with a specific pitch range. The present study examines whether the observed tonal homophone advantage is dependent on familiarity with the talker. Adult learners of Mandarin Chinese as an L2 were taught 20 new tonal words for three consecutive days. To manipulate phonological familiarity, 10 words had homophones already known to the learners and 10 words did not. To manipulate talker familiarity, participants were trained on a single talker but tested on 16 new talkers or trained and tested on 16 (multi)-talkers. Daily testing involved a 4-alternative-force-choice task. Both groups showed increased accuracy and faster response times on Day 2 compared to Day 1, but this learning was independent of homophone status or talker group. No other effects were found. These results suggest that the tonal homophone advantage in L2 word learning observed by Liu and Wiener (2020) may have been partially driven by an exceptionally high level of talker familiarity, since that study used a single speaker both for training and testing.
先前的研究表明,如果普通话的第二语言(L2)学习者与他们已经知道的单词同音,他们会更容易地学习新词(Liu和Wiener,2020)。这项研究涉及单词学习,其中语音是由一个特定音高范围的说话者产生的。本研究考察了观察到的音调同音优势是否取决于对说话者的熟悉程度。连续三天,对普通话二语成人学习者进行了20个声调新词的教学。为了操纵语音熟悉度,10个单词具有学习者已知的同音词,而10个单词没有。为了操纵说话者的熟悉度,参与者在单个说话者身上进行训练,但在16个新说话者身上测试,或在16个(多)说话者身上训练和测试。每天的测试包括一项4选一的任务。与第一天相比,两组在第二天都表现出更高的准确性和更快的反应时间,但这种学习与同音状态或谈话组无关。未发现其他影响。这些结果表明,Liu和Wiener(2020)观察到的第二语言单词学习中的音调同音优势可能部分是由说话者的高度熟悉所驱动的,因为该研究使用了一个说话者进行训练和测试。
{"title":"Effects of phonological and talker familiarity on second language lexical development","authors":"Jiang Liu, Seth Wiener","doi":"10.1075/ml.20024.liu","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1075/ml.20024.liu","url":null,"abstract":"\u0000 Previous research has shown that second language (L2) learners of Mandarin learn new words more easily if the new\u0000 word is homophonous with a word they already know (Liu and Wiener, 2020). That research\u0000 involved word learning in which speech was produced by a single-talker with a specific pitch range. The present study examines\u0000 whether the observed tonal homophone advantage is dependent on familiarity with the talker. Adult learners of Mandarin Chinese as\u0000 an L2 were taught 20 new tonal words for three consecutive days. To manipulate phonological familiarity, 10 words had homophones\u0000 already known to the learners and 10 words did not. To manipulate talker familiarity, participants were trained on a single talker\u0000 but tested on 16 new talkers or trained and tested on 16 (multi)-talkers. Daily testing involved a 4-alternative-force-choice\u0000 task. Both groups showed increased accuracy and faster response times on Day 2 compared to Day 1, but this learning was\u0000 independent of homophone status or talker group. No other effects were found. These results suggest that the tonal homophone\u0000 advantage in L2 word learning observed by Liu and Wiener (2020) may have been partially\u0000 driven by an exceptionally high level of talker familiarity, since that study used a single speaker both for training and\u0000 testing.","PeriodicalId":45215,"journal":{"name":"Mental Lexicon","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.6,"publicationDate":"2022-03-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"42165528","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
Is it you you’re looking for? 你在找你吗?
IF 0.6 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2022-03-18 DOI: 10.1075/ml.20031.wes
Chris Westbury, Lee H. Wurm
Previous evidence has implicated personal relevance as a predictive factor in lexical access. Westbury (2014) showed that personally relevant words were rated as having a higher subjective familiarity than words that were not personally relevant, suggesting that personally relevant words are processed more fluently than less personally relevant words. Here we extend this work by defining a measure of personal relevance that does not rely on human judgments but is rather derived from first-order co-occurrence of words with the first-person singular personal pronoun, I. We show that words estimated as most personally relevant are recognized more quickly, named faster, judged as more familiar, and used by infants earlier than words that are less personally relevant. Self-relevance is also a strong predictor of several measures that are usually measured only by human judgments or their computational estimates, such as subjective familiarity, age of acquisition, imageability, concreteness, and body-object interaction. We have made all self-relevance estimates (as well as the raw data and code from our experiments) available at https://osf.io/gdb6h/.
先前的证据表明,个人关联是词汇获取的预测因素。Westbury(2014)表明,与个人相关的词被评为比与个人无关的词具有更高的主观熟悉度,这表明与个人相关的词比不与个人相关的词被处理得更流畅。在这里,我们扩展了这项工作,定义了一种不依赖于人类判断的个人相关性度量,而是来自于单词与第一人称单数人称代词i的一阶共现。我们表明,与个人相关性较低的单词相比,被估计为最个人相关性的单词被婴儿更快地识别,更快地命名,被判断为更熟悉,并且更早地被婴儿使用。自我关联也是一些通常只能通过人类判断或计算估计来衡量的指标的有力预测指标,如主观熟悉度、习得年龄、可想象性、具体性和身体-对象交互。我们已经在https://osf.io/gdb6h/上提供了所有的自相关估计(以及来自我们实验的原始数据和代码)。
{"title":"Is it you you’re looking for?","authors":"Chris Westbury, Lee H. Wurm","doi":"10.1075/ml.20031.wes","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1075/ml.20031.wes","url":null,"abstract":"\u0000 Previous evidence has implicated personal relevance as a predictive factor in lexical access. Westbury (2014) showed that personally relevant words were rated as having a higher\u0000 subjective familiarity than words that were not personally relevant, suggesting that personally relevant words are processed more\u0000 fluently than less personally relevant words. Here we extend this work by defining a measure of personal relevance that does not\u0000 rely on human judgments but is rather derived from first-order co-occurrence of words with the first-person singular personal\u0000 pronoun, I. We show that words estimated as most personally relevant are recognized more quickly, named faster,\u0000 judged as more familiar, and used by infants earlier than words that are less personally relevant. Self-relevance is also a strong\u0000 predictor of several measures that are usually measured only by human judgments or their computational estimates, such as\u0000 subjective familiarity, age of acquisition, imageability, concreteness, and body-object interaction. We have made all\u0000 self-relevance estimates (as well as the raw data and code from our experiments) available at https://osf.io/gdb6h/.","PeriodicalId":45215,"journal":{"name":"Mental Lexicon","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.6,"publicationDate":"2022-03-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41856705","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 3
Morphological processing is gradient not discrete in L1 and L2 English masked priming 在母语和二语英语隐性启动中,形态学加工是梯度的,而不是离散的
IF 0.6 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2022-03-18 DOI: 10.1075/ml.21008.loo
Kaidi Lõo, Abigail Toth, Figen Karaca, Juhani Järvikivi
In recent years, evidence has emerged that readers may have access to the meaning of complex words even in the early stages of processing, suggesting that phenomena previously attributed to morphological decomposition may actually emerge from an interplay between formal and semantic effects. The present study adds to this line of work by deploying a forward masked priming experiment with both L1 (Experiment 1) and L2 (Experiment 2) speakers of English. Following recent research trends, we view morphological processing as a gradient process emerging over time. In order to model this, we used a large within-item stimulus design combined with advanced statistical methods such as generalised mixed models (GAMM) and quantile regression (QGAM). L1 GAMM analyses only showed priming for true morpho-semantic relations (the identity ‘bull’, inflected ‘bulls’ and derived conditions ‘bullish’), with no priming observed in the case of other relations (the pseudo-complex ‘bully’ or the stem-embedded ‘bullet’ conditions). Furthermore, with respect to the time-course of effects, we found significant differences between conditions were present from very early on as revealed by the QGAM analyses. In contrast, L2 speakers showed significant facilitation across all five conditions compared to the baseline condition, including the stem-embedded condition, suggesting early L2 processing is only dependant on the form.
近年来,有证据表明,读者甚至在处理复杂单词的早期阶段就可能获得其含义,这表明先前归因于形态分解的现象实际上可能是形式效应和语义效应相互作用的结果。本研究通过对第一语言(实验1)和第二语言(实验2)的英语使用者进行前向掩蔽启动实验,从而增加了这一工作。根据最近的研究趋势,我们认为形态学处理是一个随着时间的推移而出现的梯度过程。为了模拟这一点,我们使用了一个大型项目内刺激设计,并结合了先进的统计方法,如广义混合模型(GAMM)和分位数回归(QGAM)。L1 - GAMM分析仅显示了真正的形态语义关系(身份“公牛”,变形的“公牛”和衍生条件“看涨”)的启动,而在其他关系(伪复杂的“欺凌”或茎嵌入的“子弹”条件)的情况下没有观察到启动。此外,就影响的时间过程而言,我们发现,正如QGAM分析所揭示的那样,不同条件之间的显着差异从很早的时候就存在。相比之下,与基线条件相比,第二语言说话者在所有五种条件下都表现出显著的促进作用,包括茎嵌入条件,这表明早期的第二语言加工仅依赖于形式。
{"title":"Morphological processing is gradient not discrete in L1 and L2 English masked priming","authors":"Kaidi Lõo, Abigail Toth, Figen Karaca, Juhani Järvikivi","doi":"10.1075/ml.21008.loo","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1075/ml.21008.loo","url":null,"abstract":"\u0000 In recent years, evidence has emerged that readers may have access to the meaning of complex words even in the\u0000 early stages of processing, suggesting that phenomena previously attributed to morphological decomposition may actually emerge\u0000 from an interplay between formal and semantic effects. The present study adds to this line of work by deploying a forward masked\u0000 priming experiment with both L1 (Experiment 1) and L2 (Experiment 2) speakers of English. Following recent research trends, we\u0000 view morphological processing as a gradient process emerging over time. In order to model this, we used a large within-item\u0000 stimulus design combined with advanced statistical methods such as generalised mixed models (GAMM) and quantile regression (QGAM).\u0000 L1 GAMM analyses only showed priming for true morpho-semantic relations (the identity ‘bull’, inflected ‘bulls’ and derived\u0000 conditions ‘bullish’), with no priming observed in the case of other relations (the pseudo-complex ‘bully’ or the stem-embedded\u0000 ‘bullet’ conditions). Furthermore, with respect to the time-course of effects, we found significant differences between conditions\u0000 were present from very early on as revealed by the QGAM analyses. In contrast, L2 speakers showed significant facilitation across\u0000 all five conditions compared to the baseline condition, including the stem-embedded condition, suggesting early L2 processing is\u0000 only dependant on the form.","PeriodicalId":45215,"journal":{"name":"Mental Lexicon","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.6,"publicationDate":"2022-03-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"46771346","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
Learning beyond words 超越语言的学习
IF 0.6 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2021-12-31 DOI: 10.1075/ml.20030.oma
Niveen Omar, K. Banai, Bracha Nir
Learning multimorphemic words involves the simultaneous learning of two hierarchically organized categories. In such words, sub-lexical units usually encode superordinate categories, whereas whole words encode exemplars of these categories. Complex, non-linear word structure is common in Semitic languages and can be used to probe the learning of multiple form-meaning associations. The aim of this study was to investigate how well Hebrew-speaking adults learn the dual form-meaning relationships that reflect different categorical levels following a few exposures to novel Hebrew-like words. Twenty-four native Hebrew-speakers were exposed to novel words through an interactive video story. Following a few exposures to the words, the learning of the exemplars was tested in a three-alternative-forced-choice identification test. The learning of the sub-lexical morphemes and the categories they encode were tested in generalization tests. The results show that a few exposures to novel, morphologically and conceptually complex words are sufficient to allow unsupervised simultaneous learning of two hierarchical categories even though the superordinate was not explicitly represented in the input.
多同形词的学习涉及到同时学习两个等级组织的类别。在这些词中,亚词汇单位通常编码上级范畴,而整个词编码这些范畴的范例。复杂的非线性词结构在闪族语言中很常见,可以用来探索多种形式-意义关联的学习。这项研究的目的是调查讲希伯来语的成年人在接触一些新的类似希伯来语的单词后,如何很好地学习反映不同分类水平的双重形式-意义关系。24个以希伯来语为母语的人通过互动视频故事接触到新单词。在几次接触这些单词之后,在一个三选项强迫选择识别测试中测试了范例的学习情况。在泛化测试中测试了亚词汇语素及其编码类别的学习。结果表明,少量的新颖、形态和概念复杂的单词暴露足以允许无监督的同时学习两个层次类别,即使上级类别在输入中没有明确表示。
{"title":"Learning beyond words","authors":"Niveen Omar, K. Banai, Bracha Nir","doi":"10.1075/ml.20030.oma","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1075/ml.20030.oma","url":null,"abstract":"\u0000 Learning multimorphemic words involves the simultaneous learning of two hierarchically organized categories. In\u0000 such words, sub-lexical units usually encode superordinate categories, whereas whole words encode exemplars of these categories.\u0000 Complex, non-linear word structure is common in Semitic languages and can be used to probe the learning of multiple form-meaning\u0000 associations. The aim of this study was to investigate how well Hebrew-speaking adults learn the dual form-meaning relationships\u0000 that reflect different categorical levels following a few exposures to novel Hebrew-like words. Twenty-four native Hebrew-speakers\u0000 were exposed to novel words through an interactive video story. Following a few exposures to the words, the learning of the\u0000 exemplars was tested in a three-alternative-forced-choice identification test. The learning of the sub-lexical morphemes and the\u0000 categories they encode were tested in generalization tests. The results show that a few exposures to novel, morphologically and\u0000 conceptually complex words are sufficient to allow unsupervised simultaneous learning of two hierarchical categories even though\u0000 the superordinate was not explicitly represented in the input.","PeriodicalId":45215,"journal":{"name":"Mental Lexicon","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.6,"publicationDate":"2021-12-31","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"42527058","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Verbal reaction times based on tracking lip movement 基于跟踪嘴唇运动的言语反应时间
IF 0.6 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2021-12-31 DOI: 10.1075/ml.19018.liu
Qiang Liu, Bryan B. Holbrook, A. H. Kawamoto, Peter A. Krause
Valid and reliable measurements of response latency are crucial in testing empirical predictions across fields of psychology. In research utilizing verbal responses, acoustic latency is the typical measure of response latency, but its validity has been questioned. We describe a simple and affordable alternative – articulatory latency based on tracking lip position. Using this method, we measured the acoustic and articulatory latencies of syllables beginning with various simple and complex onsets and ending with “uh” using the speeded naming task, where participants were instructed to have their mouths either closed or open before articulating. The initial oral configuration, place of articulation, and voicing all had significant effects on this measure of articulatory latency across segments, factors that researchers must consider in designing experiments and selecting stimuli.
反应延迟的有效和可靠的测量在测试心理学领域的经验预测是至关重要的。在言语反应研究中,声潜伏期是典型的反应潜伏期测量方法,但其有效性一直受到质疑。我们描述了一个简单和负担得起的替代方案-基于跟踪嘴唇位置的发音延迟。使用这种方法,我们使用快速命名任务测量了以各种简单和复杂的开头和以“uh”结尾的音节的声学和发音潜伏期,在此任务中,参与者被指示在发音之前闭上或张开嘴巴。最初的口腔结构、发音位置和发声都对发音潜伏期的测量有显著影响,这些因素是研究人员在设计实验和选择刺激时必须考虑的因素。
{"title":"Verbal reaction times based on tracking lip movement","authors":"Qiang Liu, Bryan B. Holbrook, A. H. Kawamoto, Peter A. Krause","doi":"10.1075/ml.19018.liu","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1075/ml.19018.liu","url":null,"abstract":"\u0000 Valid and reliable measurements of response latency are crucial in testing empirical predictions across fields of\u0000 psychology. In research utilizing verbal responses, acoustic latency is the typical measure of response latency, but its validity\u0000 has been questioned. We describe a simple and affordable alternative – articulatory latency based on tracking lip position. Using\u0000 this method, we measured the acoustic and articulatory latencies of syllables beginning with various simple and complex onsets and\u0000 ending with “uh” using the speeded naming task, where participants were instructed to have their mouths either closed or open\u0000 before articulating. The initial oral configuration, place of articulation, and voicing all had significant effects on this\u0000 measure of articulatory latency across segments, factors that researchers must consider in designing experiments and selecting\u0000 stimuli.","PeriodicalId":45215,"journal":{"name":"Mental Lexicon","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.6,"publicationDate":"2021-12-31","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"47280969","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
Morphophonological activation in Chinese word recognition 汉语单词识别中的形态语音激活
IF 0.6 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2021-12-31 DOI: 10.1075/ml.21003.tsa
Yiu-Kei Tsang
The role of morphophonology during Chinese visual word recognition was investigated in three masked priming lexical decision experiments. The primes and targets were Cantonese Chinese bimorphemic words. In Chinese, most characters correspond to morphemes, but sometimes the mapping between character and morpheme is not one-to-one. Specifically, some characters are heteronymic, which had one visual form associated with multiple pronunciations and meanings (e.g., “長” is pronounced as /coeng4/ and /zoeng2/ in Cantonese, which means “long” and “senior”, respectively). In Experiment 1, facilitative priming was found when the primes and targets shared heteronymic characters of identical (e.g., “長遠-long term” /coeng4jyun5/ and “長短-length” /coeng4dyun2/), but not a different, pronunciation (e.g., “長官-senior official” /zoeng2gun1/). Sharing word-level phonology only (e.g., “場景-scene” /coeng4ging2/) had no effects. Experiment 2 replicated Experiment 1, and further indicated that the effects could not be attributed to sharing word-level meanings (e.g., “即時-immediate” /zik1si4/). Experiment 3 compared the priming effects produced by the two alternative pronunciations of the heteronymic characters. The results showed that the strength of priming was statistically comparable in the two pronunciation-congruent conditions. Together, this study provided evidence that morphophonology was activated to facilitate the ambiguity resolution of heteronymic characters. The lemma model was modified to accommodate the results.
通过三个掩蔽启动词汇决策实验,研究了词音在汉语视觉词识别中的作用。启动词和目标词均为广东话和汉语双同义词。在汉语中,大多数汉字都对应着语素,但有时汉字与语素之间的映射不是一一对应的。具体来说,有些汉字是异名异义的,它们有一种视觉形式,与多种发音和含义相关联(例如,“”在粤语中发音为/coeng4/和/ zeng2 /,分别表示“长”和“高级”)。在实验1中,使便利的启动时被发现质数和目标共享heteronymic字符相同的(例如,“長遠有序的术语“coeng4jyun5 /和“長短长度”/ coeng4dyun2 /),但不是一个不同的发音(例如,“長官高级官员”/ zoeng2gun1 /)。仅共享单词级别的音系(例如,“汉字-场景”/coeng4ging2/)没有效果。实验2重复了实验1,并进一步表明,这种效果不能归因于共享单词级别的含义(例如,“www.zik1si4 /”)。实验3比较了异音字的两种不同读音所产生的启动效应。结果表明,在两种发音一致的条件下,启动强度具有统计学上的可比性。综上所述,本研究提供的证据表明,语音学被激活以促进异名字的歧义解决。对引理模型进行了修改以适应结果。
{"title":"Morphophonological activation in Chinese word recognition","authors":"Yiu-Kei Tsang","doi":"10.1075/ml.21003.tsa","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1075/ml.21003.tsa","url":null,"abstract":"\u0000The role of morphophonology during Chinese visual word recognition was investigated in three masked priming lexical decision experiments. The primes and targets were Cantonese Chinese bimorphemic words. In Chinese, most characters correspond to morphemes, but sometimes the mapping between character and morpheme is not one-to-one. Specifically, some characters are heteronymic, which had one visual form associated with multiple pronunciations and meanings (e.g., “長” is pronounced as /coeng4/ and /zoeng2/ in Cantonese, which means “long” and “senior”, respectively). In Experiment 1, facilitative priming was found when the primes and targets shared heteronymic characters of identical (e.g., “長遠-long term” /coeng4jyun5/ and “長短-length” /coeng4dyun2/), but not a different, pronunciation (e.g., “長官-senior official” /zoeng2gun1/). Sharing word-level phonology only (e.g., “場景-scene” /coeng4ging2/) had no effects. Experiment 2 replicated Experiment 1, and further indicated that the effects could not be attributed to sharing word-level meanings (e.g., “即時-immediate” /zik1si4/). Experiment 3 compared the priming effects produced by the two alternative pronunciations of the heteronymic characters. The results showed that the strength of priming was statistically comparable in the two pronunciation-congruent conditions. Together, this study provided evidence that morphophonology was activated to facilitate the ambiguity resolution of heteronymic characters. The lemma model was modified to accommodate the results.","PeriodicalId":45215,"journal":{"name":"Mental Lexicon","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.6,"publicationDate":"2021-12-31","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"47410784","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Phonological similarity effects in cross-script word processing 跨文字处理中的语音相似效应
IF 0.6 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2021-12-31 DOI: 10.1075/ml.20001.lee
Sang-Im Lee-Kim, Xinran Ren, P. Mok
The present study explored the conditions under which phonological similarity effects arise without orthographic confounds by testing languages with true cognates but divergent scripts. We investigated the similarities and differences between within- and cross-script processing patterns by providing data from an understudied language pair, Korean and Cantonese, which have many cognates but bear no orthographic resemblance. In two word-naming and translation tasks, beginning and intermediate Cantonese-speaking learners of Korean (N = 112) were tested for the processing speed of Sino-Korean words. The results of the word-naming experiments showed that phonologically similar words were processed faster than dissimilar ones, regardless of L2 fluency, especially when the logographic L1 characters were used as primes. However, facilitation by shared phonology was not observed in the translation experiments in either direction. L1-to-L2 forward translation was much faster than L2-to-L1 backward translation, indicating conceptual memory being used as a primary processing pathway. The characteristics of cross-script processing patterns were discussed in terms of the structure of bilingual memory.
本研究通过测试具有真正同源词但不同脚本的语言,探讨了在没有拼写混淆的情况下产生语音相似效应的条件。我们通过提供一对研究不足的语言(朝鲜语和广东话)的数据,调查了内部和交叉脚本处理模式之间的相似性和差异,这两种语言有许多同源词,但没有拼写相似性。在两个单词的命名和翻译任务中,韩语(N = 112)进行了中韩文字处理速度的测试。单词命名实验的结果表明,无论第二语言的流利程度如何,语音相似的单词处理速度都比不相似的单词快,尤其是当使用拼音L1字符作为素数时。然而,在两个方向的翻译实验中都没有观察到共享音韵学的促进作用。L1到L2的正向翻译比L2到L1的反向翻译快得多,这表明概念记忆被用作主要的处理途径。从双语记忆结构的角度探讨了跨体加工模式的特点。
{"title":"Phonological similarity effects in cross-script word processing","authors":"Sang-Im Lee-Kim, Xinran Ren, P. Mok","doi":"10.1075/ml.20001.lee","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1075/ml.20001.lee","url":null,"abstract":"\u0000 The present study explored the conditions under which phonological similarity effects arise without orthographic\u0000 confounds by testing languages with true cognates but divergent scripts. We investigated the similarities and differences between\u0000 within- and cross-script processing patterns by providing data from an understudied language pair, Korean and Cantonese, which\u0000 have many cognates but bear no orthographic resemblance. In two word-naming and translation tasks, beginning and intermediate\u0000 Cantonese-speaking learners of Korean (N = 112) were tested for the processing speed of Sino-Korean words. The\u0000 results of the word-naming experiments showed that phonologically similar words were processed faster than dissimilar ones,\u0000 regardless of L2 fluency, especially when the logographic L1 characters were used as primes. However, facilitation by shared\u0000 phonology was not observed in the translation experiments in either direction. L1-to-L2 forward translation was much faster than\u0000 L2-to-L1 backward translation, indicating conceptual memory being used as a primary processing pathway. The characteristics of\u0000 cross-script processing patterns were discussed in terms of the structure of bilingual memory.","PeriodicalId":45215,"journal":{"name":"Mental Lexicon","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.6,"publicationDate":"2021-12-31","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"42946177","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Processing lexicality in healthy aging and Alzheimer’s disease 健康衰老和阿尔茨海默病的词性加工
IF 0.6 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2021-12-31 DOI: 10.1075/ml.20028.aze
N. Azevedo, R. Atchley, N. Vasavan Nair, E. Kehayia
To explore how processing lexicality may change with aging and in the presence of Alzheimer’s disease (AD), we conducted two experiments investigating lexicality judgements using an on-line behavioural psycholinguistic methodology and electrophysiological/event-related potential (ERP) methods; oddball lexical decision tasks. Results from these lexical decision tasks showed that while those with AD show similar rates of accuracy for their lexical decision as compared older adults (OA), they are particularly slowed when making judgements for pseudowords. Our results from the ERP tasks also showed that the two groups behaved differently with regard to elicitation of the P3 ERP response, which indicates differences in how these two groups form lexical categories. The pattern of ERP responses suggests that older adults are sensitive to the orthography/phonology of the stimuli during the course of lexical processing as compared to participants with AD who show less sensitivity to orthographic/phonological cues. Additionally, the ERP P3 amplitude results suggest further linguistically related differences between healthy older adults and those with AD, and highlight the importance and usefulness of combining behavioural psycholinguistic and ERP methodologies.
为了探索在阿尔茨海默病(AD)存在的情况下,词汇处理如何随着年龄的增长而变化,我们使用在线行为心理语言学方法和电生理/事件相关电位(ERP)方法进行了两项实验,研究词汇判断;奇怪的词汇决策任务。这些词汇决策任务的结果表明,尽管AD患者的词汇决策准确率与老年人(OA)相似,但他们在判断假名时尤其慢。我们的ERP任务结果还表明,两组在P3 ERP反应的激发方面表现不同,这表明这两组在形成词汇类别方面存在差异。ERP反应模式表明,与AD参与者相比,老年人在词汇处理过程中对刺激的正字法/音韵学敏感,AD参与者对正字法或音韵学线索的敏感性较低。此外,ERP P3振幅结果表明,健康老年人和AD患者之间存在进一步的语言相关差异,并强调了将行为心理语言学和ERP方法相结合的重要性和有用性。
{"title":"Processing lexicality in healthy aging and Alzheimer’s disease","authors":"N. Azevedo, R. Atchley, N. Vasavan Nair, E. Kehayia","doi":"10.1075/ml.20028.aze","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1075/ml.20028.aze","url":null,"abstract":"\u0000 To explore how processing lexicality may change with aging and in the presence of Alzheimer’s disease (AD), we\u0000 conducted two experiments investigating lexicality judgements using an on-line behavioural psycholinguistic methodology and\u0000 electrophysiological/event-related potential (ERP) methods; oddball lexical decision tasks. Results from these lexical decision\u0000 tasks showed that while those with AD show similar rates of accuracy for their lexical decision as compared older adults (OA),\u0000 they are particularly slowed when making judgements for pseudowords. Our results from the ERP tasks also showed that the two\u0000 groups behaved differently with regard to elicitation of the P3 ERP response, which indicates differences in how these two groups\u0000 form lexical categories. The pattern of ERP responses suggests that older adults are sensitive to the orthography/phonology of the\u0000 stimuli during the course of lexical processing as compared to participants with AD who show less sensitivity to\u0000 orthographic/phonological cues. Additionally, the ERP P3 amplitude results suggest further linguistically related differences\u0000 between healthy older adults and those with AD, and highlight the importance and usefulness of combining behavioural\u0000 psycholinguistic and ERP methodologies.","PeriodicalId":45215,"journal":{"name":"Mental Lexicon","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.6,"publicationDate":"2021-12-31","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"45156268","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Investigating bilinguals’ cognitive processing of affective words in minimal linguistic contexts 在最小语境下研究双语者对情感词的认知加工
IF 0.6 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2021-12-31 DOI: 10.1075/ml.20026.doq
Vahid Rahmani Doqaruni
The main aims of the present study are, first, to extend the current cognitive research on affective word processing in bilinguals by analyzing affective words in minimal linguistic contexts and, second, to explore the potential impact of the affective valence of prime nouns on the affective valence of target adjectives. To fulfill these aims, a semantic decision task was employed in which the Persian-English bilinguals saw a pair of words one after another, and were asked to decide whether or not the target word, which was an adjective loaded with positive or negative valence, was related in meaning to the preceding word, which was a noun. Mixed factorial repeated measure ANOVA was run on reaction times and error rates data. The results showed that bilinguals’ responses were slower and less accurate to negative target adjectives in comparison to positive target adjectives. The data further revealed that bilinguals were faster but less accurate when they were responding to related target adjectives compared to unrelated target adjectives. The results provide evidence for a dynamic interaction between cognitive and affective language processing in bilinguals.
本研究的主要目的是:首先,通过分析最小语言语境下的情意词,拓展当前双语者情意词处理的认知研究;其次,探索原初名词的情意价对目的形容词的情意价的潜在影响。为了实现这些目标,本研究采用语义决策任务,让波斯语-英语双语者依次看到一对词,并要求他们判断目标词(带有正负效价的形容词)与前一个词(名词)在意义上是否相关。对反应时间和错误率数据进行混合因子重复测量方差分析。结果表明,双语者对消极目标形容词的反应比对积极目标形容词的反应更慢、更不准确。数据进一步显示,双语者对相关目标形容词的反应速度比对不相关目标形容词的反应速度快,但准确性低。研究结果为双语者认知和情感语言加工之间的动态互动提供了证据。
{"title":"Investigating bilinguals’ cognitive processing of affective words in minimal linguistic contexts","authors":"Vahid Rahmani Doqaruni","doi":"10.1075/ml.20026.doq","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1075/ml.20026.doq","url":null,"abstract":"\u0000 The main aims of the present study are, first, to extend the current cognitive research on affective word\u0000 processing in bilinguals by analyzing affective words in minimal linguistic contexts and, second, to explore the potential impact\u0000 of the affective valence of prime nouns on the affective valence of target adjectives. To fulfill these aims, a semantic decision\u0000 task was employed in which the Persian-English bilinguals saw a pair of words one after another, and were asked to decide whether\u0000 or not the target word, which was an adjective loaded with positive or negative valence, was related in meaning to the preceding\u0000 word, which was a noun. Mixed factorial repeated measure ANOVA was run on reaction times and error rates data. The results showed\u0000 that bilinguals’ responses were slower and less accurate to negative target adjectives in comparison to positive target\u0000 adjectives. The data further revealed that bilinguals were faster but less accurate when they were responding to related target\u0000 adjectives compared to unrelated target adjectives. The results provide evidence for a dynamic interaction between cognitive and\u0000 affective language processing in bilinguals.","PeriodicalId":45215,"journal":{"name":"Mental Lexicon","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.6,"publicationDate":"2021-12-31","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"46982152","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Word order effect in collocation processing 搭配处理中的语序效应
IF 0.6 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2021-12-31 DOI: 10.1075/ml.20022.vil
Laura Vilkaitė‐Lozdienė, K. Conklin
Collocations are words associated because of their frequent co-occurrence, which makes them predictable and leads to facilitated processing. While there have been suggestions that collocations are stored as unanalysed chunks, other researchers disagree. One of the arguments against holistic storage is the fact that collocations are not fixed phrases, for example, their word order can vary. To explore whether reversed collocations retain the processing advantage that they have in their canonical form, we conducted two primed lexical decision experiments: Experiment 1 in English, and Experiment 2 in Lithuanian, an understudied language. We presented both forward and backward collocations and compared them to matched control phrases. We also explored which collocational measure (phrasal frequency, MI, t-score, or ΔP) worked as the best predictor of processing speed. We found a clear priming effect for both languages when collocations were presented in their forward form, which is in line with previous research. There was no priming for the backward condition in English, but a priming effect for it in Lithuanian, where the reversed word order is acceptable albeit marked. These results are not easily explained by holistic storage. As far as collocational measures are concerned, they all seem to perform reasonably well, with none of them being clearly better than the others.
搭配是指由于经常同时出现而产生关联的单词,这使得它们可以预测并促进处理。虽然有人认为搭配被存储为未分析的块,但其他研究人员不同意。反对整体存储的一个论点是,搭配不是固定的短语,例如,它们的词序可以变化。为了探讨反向搭配是否保留了它们在规范形式下的加工优势,我们进行了两个启动词汇决策实验:英语实验1和立陶宛语实验2,立陶宛语是一种未被充分研究的语言。我们提出了向前和向后搭配,并将它们与匹配的对照短语进行了比较。我们还探讨了哪种搭配测量(短语频率、MI、t-score或ΔP)是处理速度的最佳预测因子。我们发现,当搭配以前向形式呈现时,两种语言都有明显的启动效应,这与之前的研究一致。在英语中,对向后的条件没有启动效应,但在立陶宛语中有启动效应,在立陶宛语中,颠倒的词序是可以接受的,尽管有标记。这些结果不容易用整体存储来解释。就搭配措施而言,它们似乎都表现得相当好,没有一个比另一个明显更好。
{"title":"Word order effect in collocation processing","authors":"Laura Vilkaitė‐Lozdienė, K. Conklin","doi":"10.1075/ml.20022.vil","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1075/ml.20022.vil","url":null,"abstract":"\u0000 Collocations are words associated because of their frequent co-occurrence, which makes them predictable and leads\u0000 to facilitated processing. While there have been suggestions that collocations are stored as unanalysed chunks, other researchers\u0000 disagree. One of the arguments against holistic storage is the fact that collocations are not fixed phrases, for example, their\u0000 word order can vary. To explore whether reversed collocations retain the processing advantage that they have in their canonical\u0000 form, we conducted two primed lexical decision experiments: Experiment 1 in English, and Experiment 2 in Lithuanian, an\u0000 understudied language. We presented both forward and backward collocations and compared them to matched control phrases. We also\u0000 explored which collocational measure (phrasal frequency, MI, t-score, or ΔP) worked as the best predictor of\u0000 processing speed. We found a clear priming effect for both languages when collocations were presented in their forward form, which\u0000 is in line with previous research. There was no priming for the backward condition in English, but a priming effect for it in\u0000 Lithuanian, where the reversed word order is acceptable albeit marked. These results are not easily explained by holistic storage.\u0000 As far as collocational measures are concerned, they all seem to perform reasonably well, with none of them being clearly better\u0000 than the others.","PeriodicalId":45215,"journal":{"name":"Mental Lexicon","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.6,"publicationDate":"2021-12-31","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41730787","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
期刊
Mental Lexicon
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1