首页 > 最新文献

Mental Lexicon最新文献

英文 中文
Explorations of morphological structure in distributional space 分布空间形态结构探索
IF 0.6 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2023-11-06 DOI: 10.1075/ml.00021.baa
Harald Baayen, Dunstan Brown, Yu-Ying Chuang
{"title":"Explorations of morphological structure in distributional space","authors":"Harald Baayen, Dunstan Brown, Yu-Ying Chuang","doi":"10.1075/ml.00021.baa","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1075/ml.00021.baa","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":45215,"journal":{"name":"Mental Lexicon","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.6,"publicationDate":"2023-11-06","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"47836551","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Long-lag repetition priming in natural text reading 自然文本阅读中的长滞后重复启动
Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2023-10-03 DOI: 10.1075/ml.21014.cos
Melda Coskun, Victor Kuperman, Jay Rueckl
Abstract Most of the empirical evidence that lays the ground for research on recognition of printed morphologically complex words comes from experimental paradigms employing morphological priming, e.g., exposure to morphologically related forms. Furthermore, most of these paradigms rely on context-less presentation of isolated words. We examined whether well-established morphological priming effects (i.e., faster recognition of a word preceded by a morphologically related word) are observable under more natural conditions of fluent text reading. Using the GECO database of eye-movements recorded during the reading of a novel, we examined the long-lag morphological and identity priming in one’s first language (L1, English and Dutch) or second language (L2, English). While the effects of identity priming were ubiquitous, no evidence of morphological priming was observed in the L1 or L2 eye-movement record. We discuss implications of these findings for ecological validity and generalizability of select current theories of morphological processing.
为印刷形态复杂单词识别研究奠定基础的大多数经验证据来自于使用形态启动的实验范式,例如,暴露于与形态相关的形式。此外,这些范式大多依赖于孤立词的无上下文表示。我们研究了在流畅文本阅读的更自然的条件下,是否可以观察到成熟的形态启动效应(即,更快地识别前面与形态相关的单词)。利用GECO在阅读小说时记录的眼球运动数据库,我们研究了第一语言(L1,英语和荷兰语)或第二语言(L2,英语)的长滞后形态和身份启动。虽然身份启动的影响是普遍存在的,但在L1或L2眼动记录中没有观察到形态启动的证据。我们讨论了这些发现对生态有效性的影响和选择当前形态学处理理论的普遍性。
{"title":"Long-lag repetition priming in natural text reading","authors":"Melda Coskun, Victor Kuperman, Jay Rueckl","doi":"10.1075/ml.21014.cos","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1075/ml.21014.cos","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract Most of the empirical evidence that lays the ground for research on recognition of printed morphologically complex words comes from experimental paradigms employing morphological priming, e.g., exposure to morphologically related forms. Furthermore, most of these paradigms rely on context-less presentation of isolated words. We examined whether well-established morphological priming effects (i.e., faster recognition of a word preceded by a morphologically related word) are observable under more natural conditions of fluent text reading. Using the GECO database of eye-movements recorded during the reading of a novel, we examined the long-lag morphological and identity priming in one’s first language (L1, English and Dutch) or second language (L2, English). While the effects of identity priming were ubiquitous, no evidence of morphological priming was observed in the L1 or L2 eye-movement record. We discuss implications of these findings for ecological validity and generalizability of select current theories of morphological processing.","PeriodicalId":45215,"journal":{"name":"Mental Lexicon","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-10-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"135738986","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Native and foreign language orthotactic probability and neighborhood density in word learning 单词学习中的母语和外语正凑概率和邻域密度
IF 0.6 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2023-08-17 DOI: 10.1075/ml.22006.rin
Josh Ring, Frank T. M. Leoné, T. Dijkstra
Laboratory studies on word learning in a foreign language (L2) have identified several variables involved in the learning process, key amongst them the orthotactic probability and neighborhood density of new words relative to learners’ native (L1) lexicons. More recently, learners’ sensitivity to orthotactic probability and neighborhood density relative to their developing L2 lexicons has come into focus. Past studies on word learning have largely focused on early stages of learning, in controlled studies spanning hours or days. Few studies have considered large corpora of ‘real-life’ learning data, spanning several weeks. In this study, we validate past findings outside of controlled laboratory conditions, by analyzing a dataset collected from Duolingo (Settles et al., 2018), a popular language learning app. Effects of orthotactic probability and neighborhood density observed in controlled studies persist under uncontrolled, big-data conditions for learners of Spanish, but not French. As learning progresses, we observe a previously unreported reversal of the effects of L1 orthotactic probability and neighborhood density, challenging theoretical models of word learning. Finally, we confirm the importance of orthotactic probability and neighborhood density relative to learners’ developing L2 Spanish lexicons, lending support to theories which posit that the same processes underly both L1 and L2 acquisition.
关于外语词汇学习的实验室研究已经确定了学习过程中涉及的几个变量,其中关键的是新词相对于学习者母语词汇的正交概率和邻域密度。近年来,学习者对二语词汇发展过程中对正凑概率和邻域密度的敏感性逐渐受到关注。过去关于单词学习的研究主要集中在学习的早期阶段,在几个小时或几天的对照研究中。很少有研究考虑到跨越数周的“现实生活”学习数据的大型语料库。在本研究中,我们通过分析流行的语言学习应用程序Duolingo (Settles et al., 2018)收集的数据集,验证了在受控实验室条件之外的过去发现。在对照研究中观察到的正拼概率和邻居密度的影响在不受控制的大数据条件下对西班牙语学习者持续存在,但对法语学习者没有影响。随着学习的进展,我们观察到先前未报道的L1正交概率和邻居密度效应的逆转,这对单词学习的理论模型提出了挑战。最后,我们确认了正序概率和邻域密度对学习者发展二语西班牙语词汇的重要性,为假设母语和二语习得背后有相同过程的理论提供了支持。
{"title":"Native and foreign language orthotactic probability and neighborhood density in word learning","authors":"Josh Ring, Frank T. M. Leoné, T. Dijkstra","doi":"10.1075/ml.22006.rin","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1075/ml.22006.rin","url":null,"abstract":"\u0000Laboratory studies on word learning in a foreign language (L2) have identified several variables involved in the learning process, key amongst them the orthotactic probability and neighborhood density of new words relative to learners’ native (L1) lexicons. More recently, learners’ sensitivity to orthotactic probability and neighborhood density relative to their developing L2 lexicons has come into focus. Past studies on word learning have largely focused on early stages of learning, in controlled studies spanning hours or days. Few studies have considered large corpora of ‘real-life’ learning data, spanning several weeks. In this study, we validate past findings outside of controlled laboratory conditions, by analyzing a dataset collected from Duolingo (Settles et al., 2018), a popular language learning app. Effects of orthotactic probability and neighborhood density observed in controlled studies persist under uncontrolled, big-data conditions for learners of Spanish, but not French. As learning progresses, we observe a previously unreported reversal of the effects of L1 orthotactic probability and neighborhood density, challenging theoretical models of word learning. Finally, we confirm the importance of orthotactic probability and neighborhood density relative to learners’ developing L2 Spanish lexicons, lending support to theories which posit that the same processes underly both L1 and L2 acquisition.","PeriodicalId":45215,"journal":{"name":"Mental Lexicon","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.6,"publicationDate":"2023-08-17","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"42940165","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The cognate continuum 同源连续体
IF 0.6 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2023-07-24 DOI: 10.1075/ml.22018.str
I. M. Strangmann, Katarina Antolović, Pernille Hansen, H. G. Simonsen
Cognates, words that are similar in form and meaning across two languages, form compelling test cases for bilingual access and representation. Overwhelmingly, cognate pairs are subjectively selected in a categorical either- or manner, often with criteria and modality unspecified. Yet the few studies that take a more nuanced approach, selecting cognate pairs along a continuum of overlap, show interesting, albeit somewhat divergent results. This study compares three measures that quantify cognateness continuously to obtain modality-specific cognate scores for the same set of Norwegian-English word-translation pairs: (1) Researcher Intuitions – bilingual researchers rate the degree of overlap between the paired words, (2) Levenshtein Distance – an algorithm that computes overlap between word pairs, and (3) Translation Elicitation – English-speaking monolinguals guess what Norwegian words mean. Results demonstrate that cognateness can be ranked on a continuum and reveal measure and modality-specific effects. Orthographic presentation yields higher cognateness status than auditory presentation overall. Though all three measures intercorrelated moderately to highly, Researcher Intuitions demonstrated a bimodal distribution, yielding scores on the high and low end of the spectrum, consistent with the common categorical approach in the field. Levenshtein Distance would be preferred for fine-grained distinctions along the continuum of form overlap.
同源词,即在两种语言中形式和意义相似的单词,为双语访问和表示提供了令人信服的测试用例。绝大多数情况下,同源词对都是以非此即彼的方式主观选择的,通常没有明确的标准和情态。然而,少数研究采取了更细致的方法,沿着重叠的连续体选择同源对,显示出有趣的结果,尽管有些不同。这项研究比较了三种连续量化同源性的方法,以获得同一组挪威语-英语单词翻译对的情态特定同源分数:(1)研究者直觉-双语研究人员评估成对单词之间的重叠程度,(2)Levenshtein距离-计算单词对之间重叠的算法,以及(3)翻译Elicitation -英语单语者猜测挪威语单词的意思。结果表明,认知性可以在一个连续体上进行排名,并揭示了测量和模式的特定效应。整体而言,正字法呈现比听觉呈现产生更高的认知状态。虽然这三种测量方法之间的相关性从中等到高度,但研究者直觉显示出双峰分布,在光谱的高端和低端产生分数,与该领域常见的分类方法一致。对于沿着形式重叠连续体的细粒度区分,首选Levenshtein距离。
{"title":"The cognate continuum","authors":"I. M. Strangmann, Katarina Antolović, Pernille Hansen, H. G. Simonsen","doi":"10.1075/ml.22018.str","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1075/ml.22018.str","url":null,"abstract":"\u0000 Cognates, words that are similar in form and meaning across two languages, form compelling test cases for\u0000 bilingual access and representation. Overwhelmingly, cognate pairs are subjectively selected in a categorical either- or manner,\u0000 often with criteria and modality unspecified. Yet the few studies that take a more nuanced approach, selecting cognate pairs along\u0000 a continuum of overlap, show interesting, albeit somewhat divergent results. This study compares three measures that quantify\u0000 cognateness continuously to obtain modality-specific cognate scores for the same set of Norwegian-English word-translation pairs:\u0000 (1) Researcher Intuitions – bilingual researchers rate the degree of overlap between the paired words, (2) Levenshtein Distance –\u0000 an algorithm that computes overlap between word pairs, and (3) Translation Elicitation – English-speaking monolinguals guess what\u0000 Norwegian words mean. Results demonstrate that cognateness can be ranked on a continuum and reveal measure and modality-specific\u0000 effects. Orthographic presentation yields higher cognateness status than auditory presentation overall. Though all three measures\u0000 intercorrelated moderately to highly, Researcher Intuitions demonstrated a bimodal distribution, yielding scores on the high and\u0000 low end of the spectrum, consistent with the common categorical approach in the field. Levenshtein Distance would be preferred for\u0000 fine-grained distinctions along the continuum of form overlap.","PeriodicalId":45215,"journal":{"name":"Mental Lexicon","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.6,"publicationDate":"2023-07-24","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"43094440","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
CONVERSATIONAL MAXIM VIOLATION BY THE MAIN CHARACTER IN THE SHANG-CHI AND THE LEGEND OF THE TEN RINGS MOVIE 《上气》与《十戒传》电影中主角对白格言的违背
IF 0.6 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2023-05-07 DOI: 10.22146/lexicon.v10i1.77798
Putu Ameylia Maheswari Dewi, Ni Nyoman Deni Ariyaningsih
This study aims to identify the types of conversational maxims violation that the violated by the main character found in the Shang-Chi and the Legend of the Ten Rings Movie. Conversational maxim is a set of rule proposed by Grice (1975) that categorizes into four types, which are maxim of quantity, quality, relation, and manner. These rules are important to make an effective and clear conversation. By violating maxims, the participants of the conversation broke the rules of cooperative principle. The participants of the conversation seem to hide the real intended meanings and certain purposes which are conveyed by the speaker behind the utterance. This research uses theory by Grice’s Cooperative Principle and also support with theory by Cutting to analyze four types of conversational maxims violation. The method used to conduct this research is descriptive qualitative method. The amount of maxim violation of quantity is 6 (40%), that appears most frequently throughout the film. The violation of maxim of quality becomes the second violation that frequently found, that brings the total 4 violations (26,6%), also the third violation that occurs is violations of maxim of relation with 3 violations in total (20%). The main character violates the maxim of manners 2 times in totals (13,4%) more than any other violation, which is the least common infraction in the film.
本研究旨在找出《上气》和《十戒传》电影中主人公违反会话格言的类型。会话格言是Grice(1975)提出的一套规则,它分为四种类型,即数量格言、质量格言、关系格言和方式格言。这些规则对于进行有效而清晰的对话非常重要。通过违反格言,对话的参与者打破了合作原则的规则。谈话的参与者似乎把说话者所传达的真正的意图和某些目的隐藏在话语背后。本研究运用格赖斯的合作原则理论,并以卡特的理论为支撑,分析了四种类型的会话准则违反。本研究采用描述性定性方法。最大违章次数为6次(40%),是全片出现频率最高的违章次数。违反质量准则成为第二大违规行为,共发生4起违规行为(26.6%),第三大违规行为是违反关系准则,共发生3起违规行为(20%)。主角违反礼仪准则的次数比其他任何违规行为都多2次(13.4%),这是全片中最不常见的违规行为。
{"title":"CONVERSATIONAL MAXIM VIOLATION BY THE MAIN CHARACTER IN THE SHANG-CHI AND THE LEGEND OF THE TEN RINGS MOVIE","authors":"Putu Ameylia Maheswari Dewi, Ni Nyoman Deni Ariyaningsih","doi":"10.22146/lexicon.v10i1.77798","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.22146/lexicon.v10i1.77798","url":null,"abstract":"This study aims to identify the types of conversational maxims violation that the violated by the main character found in the Shang-Chi and the Legend of the Ten Rings Movie. Conversational maxim is a set of rule proposed by Grice (1975) that categorizes into four types, which are maxim of quantity, quality, relation, and manner. These rules are important to make an effective and clear conversation. By violating maxims, the participants of the conversation broke the rules of cooperative principle. The participants of the conversation seem to hide the real intended meanings and certain purposes which are conveyed by the speaker behind the utterance. This research uses theory by Grice’s Cooperative Principle and also support with theory by Cutting to analyze four types of conversational maxims violation. The method used to conduct this research is descriptive qualitative method. The amount of maxim violation of quantity is 6 (40%), that appears most frequently throughout the film. The violation of maxim of quality becomes the second violation that frequently found, that brings the total 4 violations (26,6%), also the third violation that occurs is violations of maxim of relation with 3 violations in total (20%). The main character violates the maxim of manners 2 times in totals (13,4%) more than any other violation, which is the least common infraction in the film.","PeriodicalId":45215,"journal":{"name":"Mental Lexicon","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.6,"publicationDate":"2023-05-07","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"83175670","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The Impact of Colonialism in Hugh Lofting’s The Story of Doctor Dolittle 殖民主义对休·洛夫廷《杜立德医生的故事》的影响
IF 0.6 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2023-05-07 DOI: 10.22146/lexicon.v10i1.72968
Raden Alexander Carlson Medicio, Nur Saktiningrum
The Story of Doctor Dolittle is the first entry in the Doctor Dolittle series, a children’s book series by English author Hugh Lofting. Despite being primarily aimed at children, the series contains mature themes that help it attract the attention of adults. In particular, The Story of Doctor Dolittle contains themes of colonialism that are drawn from the author’s own experiences. This research aims to discover how colonialism in The Story of Doctor Dolittle has impacted the numerous characters found in it. The original 1920 publication of the book was used as the primary data source, in which it was analyzed using a thorough closed reading of its contents, which is then compared with real-life colonial events. Jurgen Osterhammel’s theory of colonialism is used as the primary theory for this research, which details the processes and results of colonialism on both the colonizers and the colonized, as well as the different types of colonies created to serve different purposes for the colonizers. The results have shown that there are multiple instances of colonialism in The Story of Doctor Dolittle, both positive and negative, that have impacted its characters to varying degrees. Characters such as Doctor Dolittle and the monkeys of the Land of the Monkeys benefited from colonialism due to the former’s altruistic motives, while others like the Jolliginki tribe suffered due to reckless exploitation by the unnamed White Man.
《杜立德医生的故事》是英国作家休·洛夫廷创作的杜立德医生系列儿童读物的第一部。尽管主要针对儿童,但该系列包含成熟的主题,有助于吸引成年人的注意。特别是,《杜立德医生的故事》包含了殖民主义的主题,这些主题来自作者自己的经历。本研究旨在发现《杜立德医生的故事》中的殖民主义是如何影响其中众多人物的。这本书最初于1920年出版,作为主要数据来源,通过对其内容进行彻底的闭门阅读来分析,然后将其与真实的殖民事件进行比较。Jurgen Osterhammel的殖民主义理论被用作本研究的主要理论,该理论详细介绍了殖民主义对殖民者和被殖民者的过程和结果,以及为殖民者服务于不同目的而创建的不同类型的殖民地。研究结果表明,在《杜立德医生的故事》中有很多殖民主义的例子,无论是积极的还是消极的,都在不同程度上影响了它的角色。杜立德医生和猴子之国的猴子等人物因为前者的利他主义动机而受益于殖民主义,而乔利金基部落等人则因为不知名的白人的肆意剥削而遭受苦难。
{"title":"The Impact of Colonialism in Hugh Lofting’s The Story of Doctor Dolittle","authors":"Raden Alexander Carlson Medicio, Nur Saktiningrum","doi":"10.22146/lexicon.v10i1.72968","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.22146/lexicon.v10i1.72968","url":null,"abstract":"The Story of Doctor Dolittle is the first entry in the Doctor Dolittle series, a children’s book series by English author Hugh Lofting. Despite being primarily aimed at children, the series contains mature themes that help it attract the attention of adults. In particular, The Story of Doctor Dolittle contains themes of colonialism that are drawn from the author’s own experiences. This research aims to discover how colonialism in The Story of Doctor Dolittle has impacted the numerous characters found in it. The original 1920 publication of the book was used as the primary data source, in which it was analyzed using a thorough closed reading of its contents, which is then compared with real-life colonial events. Jurgen Osterhammel’s theory of colonialism is used as the primary theory for this research, which details the processes and results of colonialism on both the colonizers and the colonized, as well as the different types of colonies created to serve different purposes for the colonizers. The results have shown that there are multiple instances of colonialism in The Story of Doctor Dolittle, both positive and negative, that have impacted its characters to varying degrees. Characters such as Doctor Dolittle and the monkeys of the Land of the Monkeys benefited from colonialism due to the former’s altruistic motives, while others like the Jolliginki tribe suffered due to reckless exploitation by the unnamed White Man.","PeriodicalId":45215,"journal":{"name":"Mental Lexicon","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.6,"publicationDate":"2023-05-07","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"75863592","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Power Relation Between Katniss Everdeen and President Snow in The Hunger Games Trilogy 《饥饿游戏》三部曲中凯特尼斯·伊夫狄恩和雪诺总统的权力关系
IF 0.6 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2023-05-07 DOI: 10.22146/lexicon.v10i1.76432
Shintya Princesa, Achmad Munjid
This paper aims to find out about the dynamic of the power relation between Katniss Everdeen and President Snow from The Hunger Games trilogy, and how the power relation reflects domination and resistance. For the analysis, this paper applies Foucault's theory of power relations. The discussion concludes that the power relation between Katniss and President Snow is formed through the distribution of power that makes them subjects who can exercise power, and in the process, leads them to create a power relation where they complement each other’s power. A productive network is then formed as they work out the power relation with objectives and rationalization to exercise their power throughout the trilogy. Furthermore, the domination and resistance are reflected in the power relation through the use of those two effects of power as strategies by President Snow and Katniss respectively. Resulting in interchanging responses of domination and resistance that affect a series of events in the story.
本文旨在了解《饥饿游戏》三部曲中Katniss Everdeen和Snow总统之间的权力关系的动态,以及这种权力关系是如何体现统治与反抗的。本文运用福柯的权力关系理论进行分析。讨论的结论是,Katniss和Snow总统之间的权力关系是通过权力的分配形成的,使他们成为可以行使权力的主体,并在此过程中导致他们形成了一种权力互补的权力关系。在整个三部曲中,他们通过目标和合理化来解决权力关系,从而形成了一个生产网络。此外,通过Snow总统和Katniss总统分别将这两种权力效应作为策略体现在权力关系中。这导致了统治和抵抗的交替反应,影响了故事中的一系列事件。
{"title":"Power Relation Between Katniss Everdeen and President Snow in The Hunger Games Trilogy","authors":"Shintya Princesa, Achmad Munjid","doi":"10.22146/lexicon.v10i1.76432","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.22146/lexicon.v10i1.76432","url":null,"abstract":"This paper aims to find out about the dynamic of the power relation between Katniss Everdeen and President Snow from The Hunger Games trilogy, and how the power relation reflects domination and resistance. For the analysis, this paper applies Foucault's theory of power relations. The discussion concludes that the power relation between Katniss and President Snow is formed through the distribution of power that makes them subjects who can exercise power, and in the process, leads them to create a power relation where they complement each other’s power. A productive network is then formed as they work out the power relation with objectives and rationalization to exercise their power throughout the trilogy. Furthermore, the domination and resistance are reflected in the power relation through the use of those two effects of power as strategies by President Snow and Katniss respectively. Resulting in interchanging responses of domination and resistance that affect a series of events in the story.","PeriodicalId":45215,"journal":{"name":"Mental Lexicon","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.6,"publicationDate":"2023-05-07","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"86433611","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Mimetic Reading of J.K. Rowling’s The Casual Vacancy: A Political Critique of the United Kingdom’s 2010 General Election Through the Analysis of Children Characters’ Sufferings j·k·罗琳《偶然空缺》的仿读:从儿童人物的苦难分析对2010年英国大选的政治批判
IF 0.6 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2023-05-07 DOI: 10.22146/lexicon.v10i1.72946
Levina Elsa Prastiwi Sugiyanto, Moh. Arif Rohman
In 2010, the United Kingdom called on a General Election after the Queen agreed to grant an approval to Gordon Brown’s proposal to dissolve the parliament. The Queen’s approval resulted on prorogation marking the end of Gordon Brown’s parliamentary period. The General Election was used as a stage for the United Kingdom’s three biggest parties to assert their political dominance. Publishing her first novel for adult, The Casual Vacancy, J.K. Rowling attempted to deliberate the change of political atmosphere during the major event.The recent paper employs Paul Ricoeur’s Threefold Mimesis to analyse the symbolisms presented in The Casual Vacancy in order to demonstrate how it is used as a means of constructive criticism of the three biggest parties participating in the UK’s 2010 General Election. The criticism mentioned in this paper are based on writer’s interpretation. Those are the Conservative needs to evaluate its rigid, too right-winged, outdated policies and to expunge its tendency of establishing class-exclusivity, Labour Party needs to evaluate its members and to pay more attention to its supporters’ concerns; Liberal-Democrats needs to resolve its scandals and to be more selective in postulating its candidates. Details are discussed.
2010年,在女王同意批准戈登·布朗解散议会的提议后,英国呼吁举行大选。女王的批准标志着戈登·布朗议会任期的结束。大选被用作英国三大政党维护其政治主导地位的舞台。J.K.罗琳出版了她的第一部成人小说《临时空缺》,试图在重大事件期间思考政治氛围的变化。最近的一篇论文采用Paul Ricoeur的《三重模仿》来分析《偶发空缺》中所呈现的象征主义,以展示它是如何被用作对参与英国2010年大选的三个最大政党进行建设性批评的一种手段。本文所提到的批评都是基于作者自己的解读。保守党需要评估其僵化、过于右倾、过时的政策,并消除其建立阶级排他性的倾向;工党需要评估其成员,并更多地关注其支持者的担忧;自由民主党需要解决其丑闻,并在推举候选人时更有选择性。讨论了细节。
{"title":"Mimetic Reading of J.K. Rowling’s The Casual Vacancy: A Political Critique of the United Kingdom’s 2010 General Election Through the Analysis of Children Characters’ Sufferings","authors":"Levina Elsa Prastiwi Sugiyanto, Moh. Arif Rohman","doi":"10.22146/lexicon.v10i1.72946","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.22146/lexicon.v10i1.72946","url":null,"abstract":"In 2010, the United Kingdom called on a General Election after the Queen agreed to grant an approval to Gordon Brown’s proposal to dissolve the parliament. The Queen’s approval resulted on prorogation marking the end of Gordon Brown’s parliamentary period. The General Election was used as a stage for the United Kingdom’s three biggest parties to assert their political dominance. Publishing her first novel for adult, The Casual Vacancy, J.K. Rowling attempted to deliberate the change of political atmosphere during the major event.The recent paper employs Paul Ricoeur’s Threefold Mimesis to analyse the symbolisms presented in The Casual Vacancy in order to demonstrate how it is used as a means of constructive criticism of the three biggest parties participating in the UK’s 2010 General Election. The criticism mentioned in this paper are based on writer’s interpretation. Those are the Conservative needs to evaluate its rigid, too right-winged, outdated policies and to expunge its tendency of establishing class-exclusivity, Labour Party needs to evaluate its members and to pay more attention to its supporters’ concerns; Liberal-Democrats needs to resolve its scandals and to be more selective in postulating its candidates. Details are discussed.","PeriodicalId":45215,"journal":{"name":"Mental Lexicon","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.6,"publicationDate":"2023-05-07","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"74460167","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
"What the F---": Taboo Words in the TV Series Sex Education “什么鬼”:电视剧《性教育》中的禁忌语
IF 0.6 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2023-04-01 DOI: 10.22146/lexicon.v10i1.81484
Brilliannisa Syahri Syahidna, Ni Gusti Ayu Roselani
This study investigates the functions of taboo words used by male, female, young and adult characters in the first season of Sex Education. Azzaro (2005) and Allan & Burridge's (2009) functions of taboo words were furthermore employed to identify and classify the use of taboo words in the TV series. There were 114 utterances of taboo words in the TV series. The most common function is swearing or expletive expression (53.51%), followed by insults or abusive expression (29.82%), and the least used functions are expression of social solidarity (11.40%) and stylistic choice (5.26 %). In regards to gender, both male and female use taboo words at nearly the same frequency. In fact, 58 out of 114 taboo words and expressions were used by female characters and 56 by male characters. Moreover, with respect to age group, young characters or teenagers in Sex Education appear to use taboo words more frequently than the adult characters. Overall, 83 out of 114 taboo words and expressions were used by teenage characters and 31 by adult characters. In addition, there are frequency differences in the use of taboo words in four specific gender and age groups. Those groups are young male, adult male, young female, and adult female.Keywords: taboo words, functions, gender, age, and TV series
本研究考察了《性教育》第一季中男性、女性、青少年和成人角色禁忌语的功能。进一步利用Azzaro(2005)和Allan & Burridge(2009)的禁忌语功能对电视剧中禁忌语的使用进行识别和分类。电视剧中出现了114处禁忌语。最常见的功能是咒骂或咒骂表达(53.51%),其次是侮辱或辱骂表达(29.82%),使用最少的功能是表达社会团结(11.40%)和风格选择(5.26%)。在性别方面,男性和女性使用禁忌语的频率几乎相同。事实上,114个禁忌语和短语中,女性角色使用了58个,男性角色使用了56个。此外,从年龄层来看,性教育中的青少年角色比成人角色更频繁地使用禁忌语。总的来说,在114个禁忌语和表达中,青少年角色使用了83个,成人角色使用了31个。此外,在四个特定的性别和年龄群体中,禁忌语的使用频率也存在差异。这些群体是年轻男性、成年男性、年轻女性和成年女性。关键词:禁忌语,功能,性别,年龄,电视剧
{"title":"\"What the F---\": Taboo Words in the TV Series Sex Education","authors":"Brilliannisa Syahri Syahidna, Ni Gusti Ayu Roselani","doi":"10.22146/lexicon.v10i1.81484","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.22146/lexicon.v10i1.81484","url":null,"abstract":"This study investigates the functions of taboo words used by male, female, young and adult characters in the first season of Sex Education. Azzaro (2005) and Allan & Burridge's (2009) functions of taboo words were furthermore employed to identify and classify the use of taboo words in the TV series. There were 114 utterances of taboo words in the TV series. The most common function is swearing or expletive expression (53.51%), followed by insults or abusive expression (29.82%), and the least used functions are expression of social solidarity (11.40%) and stylistic choice (5.26 %). In regards to gender, both male and female use taboo words at nearly the same frequency. In fact, 58 out of 114 taboo words and expressions were used by female characters and 56 by male characters. Moreover, with respect to age group, young characters or teenagers in Sex Education appear to use taboo words more frequently than the adult characters. Overall, 83 out of 114 taboo words and expressions were used by teenage characters and 31 by adult characters. In addition, there are frequency differences in the use of taboo words in four specific gender and age groups. Those groups are young male, adult male, young female, and adult female.Keywords: taboo words, functions, gender, age, and TV series","PeriodicalId":45215,"journal":{"name":"Mental Lexicon","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.6,"publicationDate":"2023-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"83190522","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Regular polysemy and novel word-sense identification 规则的一词多义和新颖的词义辨析
IF 0.6 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2023-03-30 DOI: 10.1075/ml.21002.lom
Alizée Lombard, R. Huyghe, L. Barque, D. Gras
This study examines speakers’ intuitions about novel word senses created through regular polysemy patterns. We investigate the effect of scalar regularity and lexical figure (metaphor vs. metonymy) on the identification of novel word senses, based on a detection experiment. It is shown that the more regular a polysemy pattern is, the less salient are the novel senses it produces, and that metaphorical patterns derive more salient novel senses than metonymic patterns. These results provide insights into the processing of novel word senses and support a non-homogeneous mental representation of regular polysemous words.
本研究考察了说话者通过规则的一词多义模式创造新词的直觉。通过一项检测实验,研究了标量规则和词汇形象(隐喻与转喻)对新词义识别的影响。结果表明,一词多义模式越规则,其产生的新义就越不突出,隐喻模式比转喻模式产生的新义更突出。这些结果为新词义的加工提供了见解,并支持了规则多义词的非同质心理表征。
{"title":"Regular polysemy and novel word-sense identification","authors":"Alizée Lombard, R. Huyghe, L. Barque, D. Gras","doi":"10.1075/ml.21002.lom","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1075/ml.21002.lom","url":null,"abstract":"\u0000 This study examines speakers’ intuitions about novel word senses created through regular polysemy patterns. We\u0000 investigate the effect of scalar regularity and lexical figure (metaphor vs. metonymy) on the identification of novel word senses,\u0000 based on a detection experiment. It is shown that the more regular a polysemy pattern is, the less salient\u0000 are the novel senses it produces, and that metaphorical patterns derive more salient novel senses than metonymic patterns. These\u0000 results provide insights into the processing of novel word senses and support a non-homogeneous mental representation of regular\u0000 polysemous words.","PeriodicalId":45215,"journal":{"name":"Mental Lexicon","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.6,"publicationDate":"2023-03-30","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"44035353","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Mental Lexicon
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1