首页 > 最新文献

Glossa-A Journal of General Linguistics最新文献

英文 中文
Frozen by context: Pragmatic factors of syntactic freezing 语境冻结:句法冻结的语用因素
IF 1 2区 文学 0 LANGUAGE & LINGUISTICS Pub Date : 2021-10-07 DOI: 10.16995/glossa.5870
Gouming Martens
Syntactic freezing has mainly been approached from a structural point of view, recently though, more cognitive approaches in terms of processing costs have been proposed. One such processing account is the additive account. According to this approach, the freezing effect is best explained as an additive effect of two syntactic processes coming together, rather than being a phenomenon on its own. Another processing account argues that the freezing effect is the result of a prosodic garden path according to which extraction can only take place from a prosodically focused constituent. The current study provides empirical evidence for a less discussed factor contributing to the freezing effect, namely a pragmatic one. The pragmatic account requires frozen sentences to have contextually given referents. If no such referent is present, the sentence becomes less acceptable. The need for such a referent comes from the non- default word order associated with frozen sentences, which often highlights/focuses a certain constituent. Several experiments were run to test the pragmatic account. Based on the results it was concluded that pragmatic factors play a significant role in explaining the apparent freezing effects. Other factors however, seem to contribute to this effect as well since this effect cannot be fully explained in terms of pragmatic factors solely.
句法冻结主要是从结构的角度来研究的,但最近从加工成本的角度提出了更多的认知方法。其中一个这样的处理帐户是添加帐户。根据这种方法,冻结效应最好被解释为两个句法过程结合在一起的叠加效应,而不是一个单独的现象。另一种加工解释认为,冻结效应是韵律花园路径的结果,根据该路径,提取只能从韵律集中的成分中进行。目前的研究提供了经验证据较少讨论的因素,有助于冻结效应,即一个实用的。语用解释要求冻结句具有上下文给定的指称物。如果没有这样的指称物存在,句子就不太容易被接受。对这样一个指称的需求来自于与冻结句相关的非默认词序,它经常突出/关注某个组成部分。为了验证实用主义的说法,进行了几个实验。结果表明,实用因素在解释明显的冻结效应中起着重要作用。然而,其他因素似乎也有助于这种影响,因为这种影响不能完全用实用主义因素来解释。
{"title":"Frozen by context: Pragmatic factors of syntactic freezing","authors":"Gouming Martens","doi":"10.16995/glossa.5870","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.16995/glossa.5870","url":null,"abstract":"Syntactic freezing has mainly been approached from a structural point of view, recently though, more cognitive approaches in terms of processing costs have been proposed. One such processing account is the additive account. According to this approach, the freezing effect is best explained as an additive effect of two syntactic processes coming together, rather than being a phenomenon on its own. Another processing account argues that the freezing effect is the result of a prosodic garden path according to which extraction can only take place from a prosodically focused constituent. The current study provides empirical evidence for a less discussed factor contributing to the freezing effect, namely a pragmatic one. The pragmatic account requires frozen sentences to have contextually given referents. If no such referent is present, the sentence becomes less acceptable. The need for such a referent comes from the non- default word order associated with frozen sentences, which often highlights/focuses a certain constituent. Several experiments were run to test the pragmatic account. Based on the results it was concluded that pragmatic factors play a significant role in explaining the apparent freezing effects. Other factors however, seem to contribute to this effect as well since this effect cannot be fully explained in terms of pragmatic factors solely.","PeriodicalId":46319,"journal":{"name":"Glossa-A Journal of General Linguistics","volume":"32 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.0,"publicationDate":"2021-10-07","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"81388272","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Resolving ambiguous polarity stripping ellipsis structures in Persian 解决波斯语歧义极性剥离省略结构
IF 1 2区 文学 0 LANGUAGE & LINGUISTICS Pub Date : 2021-10-07 DOI: 10.16995/glossa.5881
Vahideh Rasekhi, Jesse A. Harris
Previous studies have shown that English speakers use a range of factors including locality, information structure, and semantic parallelism to interpret clausal ellipsis structures. Yet, the relative importance of each factor is currently underexplored. As cues to information structure and semantic parallelism are often implicit in English, we turned to Persian which marks information structure overtly via word order scrambling and uses the -rā morpheme to indicate definiteness/specificity on direct objects. To determine what strategies Persian speakers use to disambiguate clausal ellipsis, we conducted a naturalness rating study and sentence completion task on polarity stripping structures. Our results show that information structure and parallelism strongly influence correlate resolution in both tasks, but that a weaker preference for a local correlate emerges in scrambling in the sentence completion task. As these results diverge from those obtained in English studies, we speculate that the morphosyntactic properties of Persian constrain the strategies the processer uses in selecting a contrastive correlate and resolving ambiguity in stripping ellipsis.
以往的研究表明,英语使用者使用一系列因素,包括局部性、信息结构和语义平行性来解释小句省略结构。然而,每个因素的相对重要性目前尚未得到充分探讨。由于信息结构和语义平行性的线索在英语中往往是隐含的,我们转向波斯语,它通过词序混乱来明显地标记信息结构,并使用-rā语素来指示直接宾语的确定性/特异性。为了确定波斯语使用者使用什么策略来消除小句省略的歧义,我们对极性剥离结构进行了自然度评级研究和句子完成任务。我们的研究结果表明,信息结构和并行性在两种任务中都强烈地影响关联解析,但在句子补全任务中,对局部关联的偏好较弱。由于这些结果与英语研究中获得的结果不同,我们推测波斯语的形态句法特性限制了处理器在选择对比关联和消除省略中歧义时使用的策略。
{"title":"Resolving ambiguous polarity stripping ellipsis structures in Persian","authors":"Vahideh Rasekhi, Jesse A. Harris","doi":"10.16995/glossa.5881","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.16995/glossa.5881","url":null,"abstract":"Previous studies have shown that English speakers use a range of factors including locality, information structure, and semantic parallelism to interpret clausal ellipsis structures. Yet, the relative importance of each factor is currently underexplored. As cues to information structure and semantic parallelism are often implicit in English, we turned to Persian which marks information structure overtly via word order scrambling and uses the -rā morpheme to indicate definiteness/specificity on direct objects. To determine what strategies Persian speakers use to disambiguate clausal ellipsis, we conducted a naturalness rating study and sentence completion task on polarity stripping structures. Our results show that information structure and parallelism strongly influence correlate resolution in both tasks, but that a weaker preference for a local correlate emerges in scrambling in the sentence completion task. As these results diverge from those obtained in English studies, we speculate that the morphosyntactic properties of Persian constrain the strategies the processer uses in selecting a contrastive correlate and resolving ambiguity in stripping ellipsis.","PeriodicalId":46319,"journal":{"name":"Glossa-A Journal of General Linguistics","volume":"27 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.0,"publicationDate":"2021-10-07","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"75804945","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Revisiting the configurationality issue in Old Icelandic 重新审视古冰岛语的配置问题
IF 1 2区 文学 0 LANGUAGE & LINGUISTICS Pub Date : 2021-10-05 DOI: 10.16995/glossa.5804
Hannah Booth
The status of Old Icelandic with respect to (argument) configurationality was subject to debate in the early 1990s (e.g. Faarlund 1990; Rögnvaldsson 1995) and remains unresolved. Since this work, further research on a wide range of languages has enhanced our understanding of configurationality, in particular within Lexical Functional Grammar (e.g. Austin & Bresnan 1996; Nordlinger 1998) and syntactically annotated Old Icelandic data are now available (Wallenberg et al. 2011). It is thus fitting to revisit the matter. In this paper, I show that allowing for argument configurationality as a gradient property, and also taking into account discourse configurationality (Kiss 1995) as a further gradient property, can neatly account for word order patterns in this early stage of Icelandic. Specifically, I show that corpus data supports part of the original claim in Faarlund (1990), that Old Icelandic lacks a VP-constituent, thus being somewhat less argument-configurational than the modern language. Furthermore, the observed word order patterns indicate a designated topic position in the postfinite domain, thus reflecting some degree of discourse configurationality at this early stage of the language.
1990年代初,古冰岛语在(论点)构型方面的地位受到争论(例如Faarlund 1990;Rögnvaldsson 1995),问题仍未解决。自这项工作以来,对广泛语言的进一步研究增强了我们对配置性的理解,特别是在词汇功能语法中(例如Austin & Bresnan 1996;Nordlinger 1998)和有语法注释的旧冰岛数据现在是可用的(Wallenberg et al. 2011)。因此,重新讨论这个问题是合适的。在本文中,我表明,允许论点配置性作为一种梯度属性,并考虑到话语配置性(Kiss 1995)作为一种进一步的梯度属性,可以很好地解释冰岛语早期阶段的词序模式。具体来说,我表明语料库数据支持Faarlund(1990)的部分原始主张,即古冰岛语缺乏vp成分,因此与现代语言相比,其论点配置较少。此外,观察到的词序模式表明了后限定域中指定的主题位置,从而反映了语言早期阶段的某种程度的话语配置性。
{"title":"Revisiting the configurationality issue in Old Icelandic","authors":"Hannah Booth","doi":"10.16995/glossa.5804","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.16995/glossa.5804","url":null,"abstract":"The status of Old Icelandic with respect to (argument) configurationality was subject to debate in the early 1990s (e.g. Faarlund 1990; Rögnvaldsson 1995) and remains unresolved. Since this work, further research on a wide range of languages has enhanced our understanding of configurationality, in particular within Lexical Functional Grammar (e.g. Austin & Bresnan 1996; Nordlinger 1998) and syntactically annotated Old Icelandic data are now available (Wallenberg et al. 2011). It is thus fitting to revisit the matter. In this paper, I show that allowing for argument configurationality as a gradient property, and also taking into account discourse configurationality (Kiss 1995) as a further gradient property, can neatly account for word order patterns in this early stage of Icelandic. Specifically, I show that corpus data supports part of the original claim in Faarlund (1990), that Old Icelandic lacks a VP-constituent, thus being somewhat less argument-configurational than the modern language. Furthermore, the observed word order patterns indicate a designated topic position in the postfinite domain, thus reflecting some degree of discourse configurationality at this early stage of the language.","PeriodicalId":46319,"journal":{"name":"Glossa-A Journal of General Linguistics","volume":"39 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.0,"publicationDate":"2021-10-05","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"77545362","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
The prosody of focus projection: word stress and F0 patterns in Georgian 焦点投射的韵律:格鲁吉亚语单词重音和F0模式
IF 1 2区 文学 0 LANGUAGE & LINGUISTICS Pub Date : 2021-10-02 DOI: 10.16995/glossa.5733
Lena Borise
Based on experimental evidence, this paper shows that focus projection/percolation – the phenomenon by way of which prosodic prominence on a sub-constituent signals focus on the whole constituent – has a consistent prosodic realization in Georgian. The novelty of these findings lies in two properties of Georgian that have not been explored from the perspective of focus projection: it is a language with a dedicated focus position (linearly immediately preverbal) and one that does not rely on pitch accents in the expression of phrasal prosody (Skopeteas & Féry 2010; 2016). According to focus projection accounts (Selkirk 1984; Cinque 1993; Ladd 1996; Zubizarreta 1998, a. o.), utterances with narrow focus on the direct object are realized in the same way as broad focus utterances, since in all three cases prosodic prominence is realized on the direct object. In contrast, in utterances with narrow focus on the subject, the subject is the most prosodically prominent element, which means that the whole utterance has a different prosodic realization from that of broad focus contexts. This paper shows that the distribution of prosodic prominence in object- and subject-focus contexts in Georgian fits with this generalization. Specifically, the realization of utterances with narrowly focused objects does not differ from broad focus contexts in their F0 patterns and prominence of the stressed syllable, while narrowly focused subjects differ from subjects in broad focus utterances in both of these parameters.
基于实验证据,本文表明焦点投射/渗透(focus projection/percolation)在格鲁吉亚语中具有一致性的韵律实现。这些发现的新颖之处在于格鲁吉亚语的两个特性,这两个特性没有从焦点投射的角度进行探索:它是一种具有专门焦点位置的语言(线性直接言语前),并且在表达短语韵律时不依赖音高重音(Skopeteas & fsamry 2010;2016)。根据焦点投射帐目(Selkirk 1984;五1993;Ladd 1996;Zubizarreta 1998, a. o.),窄焦点直接宾语的话语与宽焦点话语的实现方式相同,因为在所有三种情况下,韵律突出都是在直接宾语上实现的。相比之下,在窄焦点语篇中,主词是韵律上最突出的元素,这意味着整个话语的韵律实现与宽焦点语篇不同。本文表明,格鲁吉亚语在客体和主体关注语境中的韵律突出分布符合这一概括。具体而言,窄聚焦对象的话语实现在F0模式和重音音节的突出程度上与宽聚焦上下文没有区别,而窄聚焦主体在这两个参数上与宽聚焦话语中的主体不同。
{"title":"The prosody of focus projection: word stress and F0 patterns in Georgian","authors":"Lena Borise","doi":"10.16995/glossa.5733","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.16995/glossa.5733","url":null,"abstract":"Based on experimental evidence, this paper shows that focus projection/percolation – the phenomenon by way of which prosodic prominence on a sub-constituent signals focus on the whole constituent – has a consistent prosodic realization in Georgian. The novelty of these findings lies in two properties of Georgian that have not been explored from the perspective of focus projection: it is a language with a dedicated focus position (linearly immediately preverbal) and one that does not rely on pitch accents in the expression of phrasal prosody (Skopeteas & Féry 2010; 2016). According to focus projection accounts (Selkirk 1984; Cinque 1993; Ladd 1996; Zubizarreta 1998, a. o.), utterances with narrow focus on the direct object are realized in the same way as broad focus utterances, since in all three cases prosodic prominence is realized on the direct object. In contrast, in utterances with narrow focus on the subject, the subject is the most prosodically prominent element, which means that the whole utterance has a different prosodic realization from that of broad focus contexts. This paper shows that the distribution of prosodic prominence in object- and subject-focus contexts in Georgian fits with this generalization. Specifically, the realization of utterances with narrowly focused objects does not differ from broad focus contexts in their F0 patterns and prominence of the stressed syllable, while narrowly focused subjects differ from subjects in broad focus utterances in both of these parameters.","PeriodicalId":46319,"journal":{"name":"Glossa-A Journal of General Linguistics","volume":"2 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.0,"publicationDate":"2021-10-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"84281101","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Case-sensitive plural suppletion in Barguzin Buryat: On case containment, suppletion typology, and competition in morphology 巴尔古津布里亚特的区分大小写的复数补语:关于大小写遏制、补语类型和形态学上的竞争
IF 1 2区 文学 0 LANGUAGE & LINGUISTICS Pub Date : 2021-09-30 DOI: 10.16995/glossa.5885
Colin Davis, Colin Davis
This paper examines plural suppletion in the Barguzin dialect of Buryat (Mongolic, Russia), which occurs only in accusative and genitive noun phrases. The restricted distribution of this process, specifically its absence in oblique cases, is significant for recent research on the typology of suppletion and the feature structure of case. For much work in this vein, this plural suppletion would qualify as having an ‘ABA’ pattern, which is predicted to be unattested. I argue that the suppletive plural morpheme in question is a portmanteau whose morphological requirements cause it to conflict, for independent reasons, with the realization of oblique noun phrases. Consequently, I argue that its distribution does not falsify the theories that normally ban ABA patterns, but rather instantiates a principled exception to them which sharpens our understanding of them.
本文研究了俄罗斯蒙古布里亚特巴尔古津方言中复数补语,这种补语只出现在宾格和属格名词短语中。这一过程的有限分布,特别是其在倾斜病例中的缺失,对最近关于补充类型和病例特征结构的研究具有重要意义。对于这方面的许多工作,这种复数补充将有资格具有“ABA”模式,预计这是未经证实的。我认为所讨论的补充复数语素是一个混合词,其形态要求由于独立的原因导致它与斜名词短语的实现相冲突。因此,我认为它的分布并没有证伪通常禁止ABA模式的理论,而是实例化了一个原则性的例外,这加深了我们对ABA模式的理解。
{"title":"Case-sensitive plural suppletion in Barguzin Buryat: On case containment, suppletion typology, and competition in morphology","authors":"Colin Davis, Colin Davis","doi":"10.16995/glossa.5885","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.16995/glossa.5885","url":null,"abstract":"This paper examines plural suppletion in the Barguzin dialect of Buryat (Mongolic, Russia), which occurs only in accusative and genitive noun phrases. The restricted distribution of this process, specifically its absence in oblique cases, is significant for recent research on the typology of suppletion and the feature structure of case. For much work in this vein, this plural suppletion would qualify as having an ‘ABA’ pattern, which is predicted to be unattested. I argue that the suppletive plural morpheme in question is a portmanteau whose morphological requirements cause it to conflict, for independent reasons, with the realization of oblique noun phrases. Consequently, I argue that its distribution does not falsify the theories that normally ban ABA patterns, but rather instantiates a principled exception to them which sharpens our understanding of them.","PeriodicalId":46319,"journal":{"name":"Glossa-A Journal of General Linguistics","volume":"50 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.0,"publicationDate":"2021-09-30","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"88435182","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 3
Mood variation with belief predicates: Modal comparison in semantics and the common ground 信念谓词的情绪变化:语义学上的情态比较及其共同点
IF 1 2区 文学 0 LANGUAGE & LINGUISTICS Pub Date : 2021-09-25 DOI: 10.16995/glossa.5726
A. Mari, P. Portner
This paper proposes that subjunctive in the complement of belief sen- tences in Italian expresses a relation between the attitude holder’s beliefs and the common ground. In contrast to most other Romance languages, ‘believe’ commonly and prescriptively takes subjunctive in Italian, though indicative is found as well, and as has been observed in the literature, the choice of indicative or subjunctive has semantic effects. We show that the indicative with ‘believe’ is used when the belief statement describes the personal mental state of the holder of the attitude, an interpretation that follows from the traditional Hintikkean semantics. In contrast, we show that subjunctive with ‘believe’ is used to mark a relation between the content of belief and the discourse context. To analyze these facts, we propose that the modal quantification present in attitude reports comes not from the attitude verb, but instead from the embedded verbal mood. What differentiates Italian from related languages where ‘believe’ only takes indicative, is that Italian allows the subjunctive to access the com- mon ground as a modal base, utilizing the verb’s doxastic background as an ordering source. The fact that subjunctive relates the common ground to the subject’s beliefs explains the discourse oriented meaning of this combination. We extend our analysis to several other predicates that show mood variation in Italian.
本文认为,意大利语信念句补语中的虚拟语气表达了态度持有者的信念与共同立场之间的关系。与大多数其他罗曼语不同的是,在意大利语中,“believe”一词通常使用虚拟语气,尽管也有指示性的用法,而且正如在文献中观察到的那样,指示性或虚拟语气的选择具有语义效果。我们表明,当信念陈述描述态度持有者的个人心理状态时,使用带有“believe”的指示语,这是一种遵循传统欣提克语义学的解释。与此相反,我们发现使用带有“believe”的虚拟语气来标记信念内容与话语语境之间的关系。为了分析这些事实,我们提出态度报告中出现的情态量化不是来自态度动词,而是来自嵌入的言语语气。意大利语与其他相关语言的不同之处在于,意大利语允许虚拟语气作为情态基础,利用动词的对比背景作为排序源。虚拟语气将共同点与主体的信念联系起来,这一事实解释了这种组合的话语导向意义。我们将分析扩展到意大利语中显示情绪变化的其他几个谓词。
{"title":"Mood variation with belief predicates: Modal comparison in semantics and the common ground","authors":"A. Mari, P. Portner","doi":"10.16995/glossa.5726","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.16995/glossa.5726","url":null,"abstract":"This paper proposes that subjunctive in the complement of belief sen- tences in Italian expresses a relation between the attitude holder’s beliefs and the common ground. In contrast to most other Romance languages, ‘believe’ commonly and prescriptively takes subjunctive in Italian, though indicative is found as well, and as has been observed in the literature, the choice of indicative or subjunctive has semantic effects. We show that the indicative with ‘believe’ is used when the belief statement describes the personal mental state of the holder of the attitude, an interpretation that follows from the traditional Hintikkean semantics. In contrast, we show that subjunctive with ‘believe’ is used to mark a relation between the content of belief and the discourse context. To analyze these facts, we propose that the modal quantification present in attitude reports comes not from the attitude verb, but instead from the embedded verbal mood. What differentiates Italian from related languages where ‘believe’ only takes indicative, is that Italian allows the subjunctive to access the com- mon ground as a modal base, utilizing the verb’s doxastic background as an ordering source. The fact that subjunctive relates the common ground to the subject’s beliefs explains the discourse oriented meaning of this combination. We extend our analysis to several other predicates that show mood variation in Italian.","PeriodicalId":46319,"journal":{"name":"Glossa-A Journal of General Linguistics","volume":"227 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.0,"publicationDate":"2021-09-25","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"80178505","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 9
Differential object marking in Modern Hebrew: from definiteness to partitivity 现代希伯来语中不同的客体标记:从确定性到可分性
IF 1 2区 文学 0 LANGUAGE & LINGUISTICS Pub Date : 2021-09-24 DOI: 10.16995/glossa.5729
Aviya Hacohen, O. Kagan, Dana Plaut
This paper investigates the change in differential object marking (DOM) currently exhibited in Modern Hebrew. To date, the consensus in the theoretical literature on Hebrew has been that the object marker 'et' is only licensed in the context of definite DPs. We observe, however, that in Modern Hebrew partitive indefinite DPs may also be preceded by 'et'. Here, we experimentally investigate this change in the distribution of 'et'. Using a judgment task, we asked 41 native Hebrew-speaking adults to rate sentences with 'et'-marked indefinite object DPs on a 5-point acceptability scale. Our results reveal that partitive items received a considerably high acceptance score, with an overall average of 3.6/5. In addition, we found a main effect for object-position and quantifier-type. In particular, acceptability of 'et'-marked partitives increased significantly for topicalized DPs and for DPs that contained proportional quantifiers (as opposed to cardinals). These data support the analysis of 'et' as a DOM marker. We propose that Modern Hebrew is currently undergoing a process of change, whereby the distribution of its object marker is shifting in the direction of the Turkish DOM pattern, along the Definiteness Scale proposed by Aissen (2003). Further, the Hebrew facts provide novel evidence for the relevance of this scale not only in a synchronic, but also in a diachronic investigation of DOM. Moreover, they point to a special status of partitivity among specific DPs, suggesting that it should be distinguished as a separate category on the Definiteness Scale.
本文研究了现代希伯来语中差异对象标记(DOM)的变化。迄今为止,关于希伯来语的理论文献中的共识是,对象标记“et”仅在明确的dp上下文中被许可。然而,我们注意到,在现代希伯来语中,分词不定的DPs也可能前面有“et”。在这里,我们通过实验研究了“et”分布的这种变化。通过一项判断任务,我们要求41名母语为希伯来语的成年人对带有“et”标记的不确定宾语DPs的句子进行5分制的可接受性评分。我们的结果显示,部分项目获得了相当高的接受分数,总体平均为3.6/5。此外,我们发现对象位置和量词类型是主要的影响因素。特别是,对于主题化的DPs和包含比例量词(与基数相反)的DPs,“et”标记的分割的可接受性显着提高。这些数据支持“et”作为DOM标记的分析。我们认为现代希伯来语目前正在经历一个变化的过程,其对象标记的分布沿着Aissen(2003)提出的确定性尺度向土耳其DOM模式的方向转移。此外,希伯来语的事实提供了新的证据,不仅在共时的相关性,而且在历时调查DOM。此外,他们指出,在特定dp中,参与性具有特殊地位,建议应将其作为确定性量表上的一个单独类别加以区分。
{"title":"Differential object marking in Modern Hebrew: from definiteness to partitivity","authors":"Aviya Hacohen, O. Kagan, Dana Plaut","doi":"10.16995/glossa.5729","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.16995/glossa.5729","url":null,"abstract":"This paper investigates the change in differential object marking (DOM) currently exhibited in Modern Hebrew. To date, the consensus in the theoretical literature on Hebrew has been that the object marker 'et' is only licensed in the context of definite DPs. We observe, however, that in Modern Hebrew partitive indefinite DPs may also be preceded by 'et'. Here, we experimentally investigate this change in the distribution of 'et'. Using a judgment task, we asked 41 native Hebrew-speaking adults to rate sentences with 'et'-marked indefinite object DPs on a 5-point acceptability scale. \u0000Our results reveal that partitive items received a considerably high acceptance score, with an overall average of 3.6/5. In addition, we found a main effect for object-position and quantifier-type. In particular, acceptability of 'et'-marked partitives increased significantly for topicalized DPs and for DPs that contained proportional quantifiers (as opposed to cardinals). \u0000These data support the analysis of 'et' as a DOM marker. We propose that Modern Hebrew is currently undergoing a process of change, whereby the distribution of its object marker is shifting in the direction of the Turkish DOM pattern, along the Definiteness Scale proposed by Aissen (2003). Further, the Hebrew facts provide novel evidence for the relevance of this scale not only in a synchronic, but also in a diachronic investigation of DOM. Moreover, they point to a special status of partitivity among specific DPs, suggesting that it should be distinguished as a separate category on the Definiteness Scale.","PeriodicalId":46319,"journal":{"name":"Glossa-A Journal of General Linguistics","volume":"1 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.0,"publicationDate":"2021-09-24","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"79575860","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
On quantified DPs in Baule 关于《Baule》中的量化DPs
IF 1 2区 文学 0 LANGUAGE & LINGUISTICS Pub Date : 2021-09-23 DOI: 10.16995/glossa.5707
Yan Konan, G. Puskás
Baule, a language of the Kwa family, has a rich set of quantifiers, which occur post-nominally within the DP. Moreover, Baule DPs can also combine more than one quantifier, and the order of occurrence of the quantifiers yields different interpretations of the quantified DPs. We focus on three quantifiers, ngba ‘all’, klwaa ‘every’ and jwe ‘some’. Given the distribution and the semantic contribution these quantifiers, we propose a structure of the Baule DP in which quantifiers merge as the head of different functional projections. We show that Baule DPs are strictly head-initial, and argue that the word orders observed within the DP is the result of systematic leftward movement of different portions of the nominal structure.
鲍勒语是瓜语家族的一种语言,它有一套丰富的量词,这些量词出现在DP的名义后。此外,Baule DPs还可以组合多个量词,量词的出现顺序会产生对量化DPs的不同解释。我们关注三个量词,ngba是“all”,klwaa是“every”,jwe是“some”。考虑到这些量词的分布和语义贡献,我们提出了一种量词合并为不同功能投影头部的Baule DP结构。我们证明了鲍勒语的词序是严格的首字母,并认为在鲍勒语中观察到的词序是名义结构的不同部分有系统地向左移动的结果。
{"title":"On quantified DPs in Baule","authors":"Yan Konan, G. Puskás","doi":"10.16995/glossa.5707","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.16995/glossa.5707","url":null,"abstract":"Baule, a language of the Kwa family, has a rich set of quantifiers, which occur post-nominally within the DP. Moreover, Baule DPs can also combine more than one quantifier, and the order of occurrence of the quantifiers yields different interpretations of the quantified DPs. We focus on three quantifiers, ngba ‘all’, klwaa ‘every’ and jwe ‘some’. Given the distribution and the semantic contribution these quantifiers, we propose a structure of the Baule DP in which quantifiers merge as the head of different functional projections. We show that Baule DPs are strictly head-initial, and argue that the word orders observed within the DP is the result of systematic leftward movement of different portions of the nominal structure.","PeriodicalId":46319,"journal":{"name":"Glossa-A Journal of General Linguistics","volume":"47 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.0,"publicationDate":"2021-09-23","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"85104219","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Asymmetry and Contrast: Coordination in Sign Language of the Netherlands 不对称与对比:荷兰手语中的协调
IF 1 2区 文学 0 LANGUAGE & LINGUISTICS Pub Date : 2021-09-20 DOI: 10.16995/glossa.5872
Katharina Hartmann, R. Pfau, Iris Legeland
This paper investigates coordination in Sign Language of the Netherlands (NGT). We offer an account for a typologically unusual coordination pattern found in this language. We show that the conjuncts of a coordinated structure in NGT may violate a constraint governing coordinated structures in spoken languages, which we refer to as the ‘Parallel Structure Constraint’. The violation consists in asymmetric fronting in the second conjunct of a coordinated structure. We argue that a violation of the Parallel Structure Constraint is acceptable in NGT in order to express a contrast across the conjuncts. Hence asymmetric reordering in the second conjunct is a strategy that allows signers to obtain the desired strength of marking when in situ marking is insufficient.
本文对荷兰语手语中的协调现象进行了研究。我们为这种语言中发现的一种类型学上不寻常的协调模式提供了一个解释。我们的研究表明,NGT中协调结构的连词可能违反了口语中协调结构的约束,我们称之为“平行结构约束”。这种违背存在于配位结构的第二连接部分的不对称正面。我们认为,为了表达连词之间的对比,违反平行结构约束在NGT中是可以接受的。因此,在第二连接中的不对称重排是一种策略,允许签字人在原位标记不足时获得所需的标记强度。
{"title":"Asymmetry and Contrast: Coordination in Sign Language of the Netherlands","authors":"Katharina Hartmann, R. Pfau, Iris Legeland","doi":"10.16995/glossa.5872","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.16995/glossa.5872","url":null,"abstract":"This paper investigates coordination in Sign Language of the Netherlands (NGT). We offer an account for a typologically unusual coordination pattern found in this language. We show that the conjuncts of a coordinated structure in NGT may violate a constraint governing coordinated structures in spoken languages, which we refer to as the ‘Parallel Structure Constraint’. The violation consists in asymmetric fronting in the second conjunct of a coordinated structure. We argue that a violation of the Parallel Structure Constraint is acceptable in NGT in order to express a contrast across the conjuncts. Hence asymmetric reordering in the second conjunct is a strategy that allows signers to obtain the desired strength of marking when in situ marking is insufficient.","PeriodicalId":46319,"journal":{"name":"Glossa-A Journal of General Linguistics","volume":"27 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.0,"publicationDate":"2021-09-20","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"86558933","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Locality in the acquisition of object A’-dependencies: insights from French 对象A '依赖性习得中的局部性:来自法语的见解
IF 1 2区 文学 0 LANGUAGE & LINGUISTICS Pub Date : 2021-09-20 DOI: 10.16995/glossa.5876
Stéphanie Durrleman, Anamaria Bentea
Children’s difficulties with dependencies involving movement of an object to the left periphery of the clause (object relative clauses/RCs and wh-questions), have been explained in terms of intervention effects arising when the moved object and the intervening subject share a lexical N feature (Friedmann, Belletti & Rizzi 2009). Such an account raises various questions: (1) Do these effects hold in the absence of a lexical N feature when the object and the intervener share other relevant features? (2) Do phi-features with a semantic role modulate such effects? (3) Does the degree of feature overlap determine a gradience in performance? We addressed these in three sentence-picture matching studies with French-speaking children (4;8 to 6;3), by assessing comprehension of (1) subject and object RCs headed by the demonstrative pronouns celui/celle and matching or mismatching in number; (2) object RCs headed by a lexical N and matching or mismatching in animacy; (3) object who- and which-questions. Our results show that mismatches in number, not in animacy, enhance comprehension of object RCs, even in the absence of a lexical N feature, and confirm previous findings that object who-questions yield better comprehension than object which-questions. Comparing across studies, the following gradation emerges with respect to performance accuracy: disjunction > intersection > inclusion. The global interpretation of these findings is that fine-grained phi-features determining movement are both sufficient and necessary for locality, and the degree of overlap of these features can capture the pattern of performance observed in children, namely higher accuracy as featural differences increase.
儿童在依赖关系方面的困难涉及到一个物体移动到子句的左边缘(对象关系子句/ rc和wh-问句),这已经被解释为当移动的物体和干预的主体共享一个词汇N特征时产生的干预效应(Friedmann, Belletti & Rizzi 2009)。这样的解释提出了各种问题:(1)当对象和干预者共享其他相关特征时,在没有词汇N特征的情况下,这些效应是否成立?(2)具有语义作用的phil -features是否调节了这种效应?(3)特征重叠的程度是否决定了性能的梯度?我们通过对法语儿童(4、8至6、3)的三个句子-图片匹配研究来解决这些问题,通过评估(1)以指示代词celui/cell为首的主语和宾语rc的理解以及数量的匹配或不匹配;(2)以词汇N开头的对象匹配和不匹配;(3)反对who- and - which疑问句。我们的研究结果表明,即使在没有词汇N特征的情况下,数量上的不匹配,而不是动画性上的不匹配,也能增强对对象rc的理解,并证实了之前的研究结果,即对象who问题比对象which问题更容易理解。比较各研究,在性能准确性方面出现以下等级:分离>交叉>包含。对这些发现的整体解释是,细粒度的phi特征决定了运动的局域性,这些特征的重叠程度可以捕捉到在儿童中观察到的表现模式,即随着特征差异的增加,准确性更高。
{"title":"Locality in the acquisition of object A’-dependencies: insights from French","authors":"Stéphanie Durrleman, Anamaria Bentea","doi":"10.16995/glossa.5876","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.16995/glossa.5876","url":null,"abstract":"Children’s difficulties with dependencies involving movement of an object to the left periphery of the clause (object relative clauses/RCs and wh-questions), have been explained in terms of intervention effects arising when the moved object and the intervening subject share a lexical N feature (Friedmann, Belletti & Rizzi 2009). Such an account raises various questions: (1) Do these effects hold in the absence of a lexical N feature when the object and the intervener share other relevant features? (2) Do phi-features with a semantic role modulate such effects? (3) Does the degree of feature overlap determine a gradience in performance? We addressed these in three sentence-picture matching studies with French-speaking children (4;8 to 6;3), by assessing comprehension of (1) subject and object RCs headed by the demonstrative pronouns celui/celle and matching or mismatching in number; (2) object RCs headed by a lexical N and matching or mismatching in animacy; (3) object who- and which-questions. Our results show that mismatches in number, not in animacy, enhance comprehension of object RCs, even in the absence of a lexical N feature, and confirm previous findings that object who-questions yield better comprehension than object which-questions. Comparing across studies, the following gradation emerges with respect to performance accuracy: disjunction > intersection > inclusion. The global interpretation of these findings is that fine-grained phi-features determining movement are both sufficient and necessary for locality, and the degree of overlap of these features can capture the pattern of performance observed in children, namely higher accuracy as featural differences increase.","PeriodicalId":46319,"journal":{"name":"Glossa-A Journal of General Linguistics","volume":"1 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.0,"publicationDate":"2021-09-20","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"88872617","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 3
期刊
Glossa-A Journal of General Linguistics
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1