首页 > 最新文献

Glossa-A Journal of General Linguistics最新文献

英文 中文
Syllable weight, vowel length and focus in Lebanese Arabic 黎巴嫩阿拉伯语的音节重量,元音长度和焦点
IF 1 2区 文学 0 LANGUAGE & LINGUISTICS Pub Date : 2021-11-06 DOI: 10.16995/glossa.5727
Niamh E. Kelly
Research on a variety of languages has shown that vowel duration is influenced by phonological vowel length as well as syllable structure (e.g., Maddieson, 1997). Further, the phonological concept of a mora has been shown to relate to phonetic measurements of duration (Cohn, 2003; Hubbard, 1993; Port, Dalby, & O'Dell, 1987). In Levantine Arabic, non-final closed syllables that contain a long vowel have been described as partaking in mora-sharing (Broselow, Chen, & Huffman, 1997; Khattab & Al-Tamimi, 2014). The current investigation examines the effect of vowel length and syllable structure on vowel duration, as well as how this interacts with durational effects of prosodic focus. Disyllabic words with initial, stressed syllables that were either open or closed and contained either a long or a short vowel wereexamined when non-focused and in contrastive focus. Contrastive focus was associated with longer words and syllables but not vowels. Short vowels were shorter when in a syllable closed by a singleton but not by a geminate consonant, while long vowels were not shortened before coda singletons. An analysis is proposed whereby long vowels followed by an intervocalic consonant cluster are parsed as open syllables, with the first consonant forming a semisyllable (Kiparsky, 2003), while long vowels followed by geminate consonants partake in mora-sharing (Broselow, Huffman, Chen, & Hsieh, 1995). The results also indicate compensatory shortening for short vowels followed by a singleton coda.
对多种语言的研究表明,元音持续时间不仅受到音节结构的影响,还受到语音元音长度的影响(如maddison, 1997)。此外,mora的音系概念已被证明与持续时间的语音测量有关(Cohn, 2003;哈伯德,1993;波特,达尔比和奥戴尔,1987)。在地中海阿拉伯语中,包含长元音的非词尾闭合音节被描述为参与mora-sharing (Broselow, Chen, & Huffman, 1997;Khattab & Al-Tamimi, 2014)。目前的研究考察了元音长度和音节结构对元音持续时间的影响,以及它们如何与韵律焦点的持续时间效应相互作用。双音节单词的开头重读音节要么是开的,要么是闭的,包含一个长元音或短元音,在非聚焦和对比聚焦时进行检查。对比焦点与较长的单词和音节有关,但与元音无关。短元音在以单音节结尾但不以双辅音结尾的音节中会变短,而长元音在尾单音节结尾之前不会缩短。提出了一种分析,即长元音后面跟着一个中间辅音簇被解析为开放音节,第一个辅音形成一个半音节(Kiparsky, 2003),而长元音后面跟着双韵母辅音则参与了语素共享(Broselow, Huffman, Chen, & Hsieh, 1995)。结果还表明,补偿性缩短短元音之后,一个单一的尾。
{"title":"Syllable weight, vowel length and focus in Lebanese Arabic","authors":"Niamh E. Kelly","doi":"10.16995/glossa.5727","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.16995/glossa.5727","url":null,"abstract":"Research on a variety of languages has shown that vowel duration is influenced by phonological vowel length as well as syllable structure (e.g., Maddieson, 1997). Further, the phonological concept of a mora has been shown to relate to phonetic measurements of duration (Cohn, 2003; Hubbard, 1993; Port, Dalby, & O'Dell, 1987). In Levantine Arabic, non-final closed syllables that contain a long vowel have been described as partaking in mora-sharing (Broselow, Chen, & Huffman, 1997; Khattab & Al-Tamimi, 2014). The current investigation examines the effect of vowel length and syllable structure on vowel duration, as well as how this interacts with durational effects of prosodic focus. Disyllabic words with initial, stressed syllables that were either open or closed and contained either a long or a short vowel wereexamined when non-focused and in contrastive focus. Contrastive focus was associated with longer words and syllables but not vowels. Short vowels were shorter when in a syllable closed by a singleton but not by a geminate consonant, while long vowels were not shortened before coda singletons. An analysis is proposed whereby long vowels followed by an intervocalic consonant cluster are parsed as open syllables, with the first consonant forming a semisyllable (Kiparsky, 2003), while long vowels followed by geminate consonants partake in mora-sharing (Broselow, Huffman, Chen, & Hsieh, 1995). The results also indicate compensatory shortening for short vowels followed by a singleton coda.","PeriodicalId":46319,"journal":{"name":"Glossa-A Journal of General Linguistics","volume":"20 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.0,"publicationDate":"2021-11-06","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"83326576","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Immobile wh-phrases in Tagalog 他加禄语中不动的h-词组
IF 1 2区 文学 0 LANGUAGE & LINGUISTICS Pub Date : 2021-11-04 DOI: 10.16995/glossa.5762
N. Richards
I argue that overt movement cannot always be triggered by a need to create a specifier for the head bearing the Probe, as commonly assumed. In Tagalog, I claim, the best description of the behavior of wh-phrases is that they must become linearly adjacent to C; I show that Tagalog wh-phrases in fact occupy several different structural positions which are consistent with this linear requirement.
我认为,明显的运动并不总是由需要为承载探针的头部创建一个说明符来触发,就像通常认为的那样。我认为,在他加禄语中,对h-词组的行为最好的描述是它们必须线性地与C相邻;我展示了他加禄语的h-短语实际上占据了几个不同的结构位置,这与线性要求是一致的。
{"title":"Immobile wh-phrases in Tagalog","authors":"N. Richards","doi":"10.16995/glossa.5762","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.16995/glossa.5762","url":null,"abstract":"I argue that overt movement cannot always be triggered by a need to create a specifier for the head bearing the Probe, as commonly assumed. In Tagalog, I claim, the best description of the behavior of wh-phrases is that they must become linearly adjacent to C; I show that Tagalog wh-phrases in fact occupy several different structural positions which are consistent with this linear requirement.","PeriodicalId":46319,"journal":{"name":"Glossa-A Journal of General Linguistics","volume":"56 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.0,"publicationDate":"2021-11-04","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"80429102","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Intervention effects in clefts: a study in quantitative computational syntax 裂隙中的干预效应:定量计算语法的研究
IF 1 2区 文学 0 LANGUAGE & LINGUISTICS Pub Date : 2021-10-31 DOI: 10.16995/glossa.5742
Giuseppe Samo, Paola Merlo
Clefts structures show an important asymmetry in interpretation: subject clefts can provide bothcorrective or new information foci, while non-subjects (objects, adjuncts) are only corrective.According to Belletti (2015), such an asymmetry arises from the fact that movement derivingsubject clefts can target two focus positions, but non-subjects can target only one. In bothcases a long-distance dependency is created, triggering locality effects. In this paper, weshow that intervention effects causing ungrammaticality in certain configurations give rise tolower-than-expected frequencies in corresponding grammatical configurations. Based on sets offeatures that play a role in the syntactic computation of locality, we compare the theoreticallyexpected and the actually observed counts of features in a corpus of thirteen syntacticallyannotated treebanks for three languages (English, French, Italian). We find the quantitativeeffects predicted by the theory of intervention locality. First, subject clefts, where no interventionis at play, are more frequent than object clefts, where intervention is at play. Secondly, objectclefts are less frequent than expected in intervention configuration, while subject clefts areroughly as frequent as expected. Finally, we also find that the differential and direction ofdifference between expected and observed counts is directly proportional to the number offeatures that establish the intervention, the strength of the intervention. These results providea three-fold contribution. First, they extend the empirical evidence in favour of the interventiontheory of locality. Second, they provide theory-driven quantitative evidence, thus extending in anovel way the sources of evidence used to adjudicate theories. Finally, the paper provides ablueprint for future theory-driven quantitative investigations.
裂隙结构在解释中表现出重要的不对称性:主体裂隙既可以提供纠正性的信息焦点,也可以提供新的信息焦点,而非主体(客体、附着物)只能起到纠正性的作用。根据Belletti(2015)的说法,这种不对称源于这样一个事实,即运动产生的主体裂缝可以瞄准两个焦点位置,而非主体只能瞄准一个焦点位置。在这两种情况下,都会创建一个远程依赖,从而触发局部性效应。在本文中,我们发现导致某些语法结构不符合语法的干预效应会导致相应语法结构出现低于预期的频率。基于在局部性句法计算中发挥作用的特征集,我们比较了三种语言(英语、法语、意大利语)的13个语法标注树库的语料库中理论期望和实际观察到的特征计数。我们发现了干预局部性理论预测的定量效应。首先,主体唇裂,在没有干预的情况下,比客体唇裂更频繁,在干预的情况下。其次,在干预配置中,客体断裂的频率低于预期,而主体断裂的频率与预期大致相同。最后,我们还发现,期望计数和观察计数之间的差值和方向与建立干预的数量特征,干预的强度成正比。这些结果提供了三倍的贡献。首先,他们扩展了支持地方干预理论的经验证据。其次,它们提供了理论驱动的定量证据,从而以一种新的方式扩展了用于评判理论的证据来源。最后,本文为未来理论驱动的定量研究提供了蓝图。
{"title":"Intervention effects in clefts: a study in quantitative computational syntax","authors":"Giuseppe Samo, Paola Merlo","doi":"10.16995/glossa.5742","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.16995/glossa.5742","url":null,"abstract":"Clefts structures show an important asymmetry in interpretation: subject clefts can provide both\u0000corrective or new information foci, while non-subjects (objects, adjuncts) are only corrective.\u0000According to Belletti (2015), such an asymmetry arises from the fact that movement deriving\u0000subject clefts can target two focus positions, but non-subjects can target only one. In both\u0000cases a long-distance dependency is created, triggering locality effects. In this paper, we\u0000show that intervention effects causing ungrammaticality in certain configurations give rise to\u0000lower-than-expected frequencies in corresponding grammatical configurations. Based on sets of\u0000features that play a role in the syntactic computation of locality, we compare the theoretically\u0000expected and the actually observed counts of features in a corpus of thirteen syntactically\u0000annotated treebanks for three languages (English, French, Italian). We find the quantitative\u0000effects predicted by the theory of intervention locality. First, subject clefts, where no intervention\u0000is at play, are more frequent than object clefts, where intervention is at play. Secondly, object\u0000clefts are less frequent than expected in intervention configuration, while subject clefts are\u0000roughly as frequent as expected. Finally, we also find that the differential and direction of\u0000difference between expected and observed counts is directly proportional to the number of\u0000features that establish the intervention, the strength of the intervention. These results provide\u0000a three-fold contribution. First, they extend the empirical evidence in favour of the intervention\u0000theory of locality. Second, they provide theory-driven quantitative evidence, thus extending in a\u0000novel way the sources of evidence used to adjudicate theories. Finally, the paper provides a\u0000blueprint for future theory-driven quantitative investigations.","PeriodicalId":46319,"journal":{"name":"Glossa-A Journal of General Linguistics","volume":"128 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.0,"publicationDate":"2021-10-31","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"88088321","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Comparing MaxEnt and Noisy Harmonic Grammar MaxEnt和Noisy Harmonic语法的比较
IF 1 2区 文学 0 LANGUAGE & LINGUISTICS Pub Date : 2021-10-30 DOI: 10.16995/glossa.5775
Edward Flemming
MaxEnt grammar is a probabilistic version of Harmonic Grammar in which the harmony scores of candidates are mapped onto probabilities. It has become the tool of choice for analyzing phonological phenomena involving probabilistic variation or gradient acceptability, but there is a competing proposal for making Harmonic Grammar probabilistic, Noisy Harmonic Grammar, in which variation is derived by adding random ‘noise’ to constraint weights. In this paper these grammar frameworks, and variants of them, are analyzed by reformulating them all in a format where noise is added to candidate harmonies, and the differences between frameworks lie in the distribution of this noise. This analysis reveals a basic difference between the models: in MaxEnt the relative probabilities of two candidates depend only on the difference in their harmony scores, whereas in Noisy Harmonic Grammar it also depends on the differences in the constraint violations incurred by the two candidates. This difference leads to testable predictions which are evaluated against data on variable realization of schwa in French (Smith & Pater 2020). The results support MaxEnt over Noisy Harmonic Grammar.
MaxEnt语法是谐波语法的概率版本,其中候选人的和谐分数被映射到概率上。它已成为分析涉及概率变化或梯度可接受性的语音现象的首选工具,但也有一种竞争性的建议,即使谐波语法具有概率性,即噪声谐波语法,其中通过在约束权重中添加随机“噪声”来导出变化。本文对这些语法框架及其变体进行了分析,将它们重新表述为一种格式,在候选和声中添加噪声,框架之间的差异在于噪声的分布。这一分析揭示了模型之间的一个基本区别:在MaxEnt中,两个候选词的相对概率仅取决于它们的和谐分数的差异,而在Noisy Harmonic Grammar中,它还取决于两个候选词违反约束的差异。这种差异导致了可测试的预测,这些预测是根据法语弱读音的可变实现数据进行评估的(Smith & Pater 2020)。结果支持MaxEnt胜过嘈杂的谐波语法。
{"title":"Comparing MaxEnt and Noisy Harmonic Grammar","authors":"Edward Flemming","doi":"10.16995/glossa.5775","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.16995/glossa.5775","url":null,"abstract":"MaxEnt grammar is a probabilistic version of Harmonic Grammar in which the harmony scores of candidates are mapped onto probabilities. It has become the tool of choice for analyzing phonological phenomena involving probabilistic variation or gradient acceptability, but there is a competing proposal for making Harmonic Grammar probabilistic, Noisy Harmonic Grammar, in which variation is derived by adding random ‘noise’ to constraint weights. In this paper these grammar frameworks, and variants of them, are analyzed by reformulating them all in a format where noise is added to candidate harmonies, and the differences between frameworks lie in the distribution of this noise. This analysis reveals a basic difference between the models: in MaxEnt the relative probabilities of two candidates depend only on the difference in their harmony scores, whereas in Noisy Harmonic Grammar it also depends on the differences in the constraint violations incurred by the two candidates. This difference leads to testable predictions which are evaluated against data on variable realization of schwa in French (Smith & Pater 2020). The results support MaxEnt over Noisy Harmonic Grammar.","PeriodicalId":46319,"journal":{"name":"Glossa-A Journal of General Linguistics","volume":"36 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.0,"publicationDate":"2021-10-30","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"91257774","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 4
Growing trees: The acquisition of the left periphery 树木生长:左侧外围的获得
IF 1 2区 文学 0 LANGUAGE & LINGUISTICS Pub Date : 2021-10-26 DOI: 10.16995/glossa.5877
N. Friedmann, A. Belletti, L. Rizzi
We suggest here a Growing Trees approach for the description of the acquisition of various syntactic structures in Hebrew, based on the main results reported in Friedmann and Reznick (this volume) and on our own research on a corpus of natural productions. The heart of our account is that stages of acquisition follow the geometry of the syntactic tree, along the lines of the cartographic analysis of the clause, with early stages of acquisition corresponding to small portions of the adult syntactic tree, which keeps growing with the growth of the child. The lower parts of the tree are acquired first, and higher parts are acquired later. We propose three stages of acquisition connected to the development of functional layers of the syntactic tree. In the first stage, the IP is acquired, including the lexical and inflectional layers. This allows for the appearance of A-movement structures, including SV/VS alternations with unaccusative verbs, alongside SV sentences with unergative/transitive verbs. The second stage involves the acquisition of the lower part of the left periphery, up to QP, which allows for the acquisition of subject and object Wh questions, some adjunct questions, yes/no questions, and sentence-initial adverbs. In the third stage, the rich structure of the left periphery is completely acquired, including the higher CP field. This is the stage in which sentential embedding (of finite declarative and interrogative clauses), subject and object relative clauses, why questions, and topicalization appear. A further, different type of stage, which occurs on the already-grown tree and which is independent of structure building, is the acquisition of intervention configurations, allowing for the mastery of structures involving movement of a lexically-restricted object across an intervening lexically-restricted subject. The paper illustrates the fruitful dialogue between the science of syntax acquisition and the cartography of syntactic structures.
基于弗里德曼和雷兹尼克(本卷)报告的主要结果,以及我们自己对自然产物语料库的研究,我们在这里建议用一种生长的树木方法来描述希伯来语中各种句法结构的习得。我们的研究的核心是,习得阶段遵循句法树的几何形状,沿着小句的地图分析的线条,早期的习得阶段对应于成人句法树的一小部分,随着儿童的成长而不断增长。首先获取树的较低部分,然后获取较高部分。我们提出了与语法树的功能层发展相关的三个习得阶段。在第一阶段,获取IP,包括词法层和屈折层。这允许出现a运动结构,包括与非宾格动词的SV/VS交替,以及与非否定动词/及物动词的SV句子。第二阶段包括左外周下半部分的习得,一直到QP,它允许习得主语和宾语Wh问题、一些附加问题、是/否问题和句子开头副词。在第三阶段,左周丰富的结构完全获得,包括高CP区。在这个阶段,句子嵌入(有限陈述句和疑问从句)、主语和宾语关系从句、为什么问题和话题化出现。另一种不同类型的阶段发生在已经生长的树上,它独立于结构构建,是干预配置的获得,允许掌握涉及词汇受限对象在干预词汇受限主体之间移动的结构。本文阐述了句法习得学与句法结构制图学之间富有成效的对话。
{"title":"Growing trees: The acquisition of the left periphery","authors":"N. Friedmann, A. Belletti, L. Rizzi","doi":"10.16995/glossa.5877","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.16995/glossa.5877","url":null,"abstract":"We suggest here a Growing Trees approach for the description of the acquisition of various syntactic structures in Hebrew, based on the main results reported in Friedmann and Reznick (this volume) and on our own research on a corpus of natural productions. The heart of our account is that stages of acquisition follow the geometry of the syntactic tree, along the lines of the cartographic analysis of the clause, with early stages of acquisition corresponding to small portions of the adult syntactic tree, which keeps growing with the growth of the child. The lower parts of the tree are acquired first, and higher parts are acquired later. We propose three stages of acquisition connected to the development of functional layers of the syntactic tree. In the first stage, the IP is acquired, including the lexical and inflectional layers. This allows for the appearance of A-movement structures, including SV/VS alternations with unaccusative verbs, alongside SV sentences with unergative/transitive verbs. The second stage involves the acquisition of the lower part of the left periphery, up to QP, which allows for the acquisition of subject and object Wh questions, some adjunct questions, yes/no questions, and sentence-initial adverbs. In the third stage, the rich structure of the left periphery is completely acquired, including the higher CP field. This is the stage in which sentential embedding (of finite declarative and interrogative clauses), subject and object relative clauses, why questions, and topicalization appear. A further, different type of stage, which occurs on the already-grown tree and which is independent of structure building, is the acquisition of intervention configurations, allowing for the mastery of structures involving movement of a lexically-restricted object across an intervening lexically-restricted subject. The paper illustrates the fruitful dialogue between the science of syntax acquisition and the cartography of syntactic structures.","PeriodicalId":46319,"journal":{"name":"Glossa-A Journal of General Linguistics","volume":"94 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.0,"publicationDate":"2021-10-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"72523036","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 28
Towards a complete Logical Phonology model of intrasegmental changes 迈向一个完整的音段内变化的逻辑音系模型
IF 1 2区 文学 0 LANGUAGE & LINGUISTICS Pub Date : 2021-10-26 DOI: 10.16995/glossa.5886
C. Reiss
All changes to the internal structure of phonological segments arise from combinations of rules based on two set-theoretic operations: feature deletion by set subtraction and feature insertion by unification. Apparent cases of rules targeting underspecified segments reflect two kinds of vacuous rule application, one due to unification failure and the other due to vacuous unification. Despite this reduction of all segment-internal changes to two basic mechanisms we can account for a wide variety of patterns, including the reciprocal neutralization and apparent exceptional behavior seen in Hungarian voicing assimilation.
音韵段内部结构的所有变化都源于基于两种集合理论操作的规则组合:通过集合减法删除特征和通过统一来插入特征。针对未指定段的规则的明显案例反映了两种真空规则的应用,一种是由于统一失败,另一种是由于真空统一。尽管将所有片段内部变化减少到两种基本机制,但我们可以解释各种各样的模式,包括在匈牙利语音同化中看到的相互中和和明显的异常行为。
{"title":"Towards a complete Logical Phonology model of intrasegmental changes","authors":"C. Reiss","doi":"10.16995/glossa.5886","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.16995/glossa.5886","url":null,"abstract":"All changes to the internal structure of phonological segments arise from combinations of rules based on two set-theoretic operations: feature deletion by set subtraction and feature insertion by unification. Apparent cases of rules targeting underspecified segments reflect two kinds of vacuous rule application, one due to unification failure and the other due to vacuous unification. Despite this reduction of all segment-internal changes to two basic mechanisms we can account for a wide variety of patterns, including the reciprocal neutralization and apparent exceptional behavior seen in Hungarian voicing assimilation.","PeriodicalId":46319,"journal":{"name":"Glossa-A Journal of General Linguistics","volume":"26 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.0,"publicationDate":"2021-10-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"83442718","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Unpacking the blocking effect: Syntactic prominence and perspective-taking in antecedent retrieval in Mandarin Chinese 解封挡效应:汉语先行词检索中的句法突出与换位思考
IF 1 2区 文学 0 LANGUAGE & LINGUISTICS Pub Date : 2021-10-23 DOI: 10.16995/glossa.5781
Jun-Hyun Lyu, E. Kaiser
In the linguistics literature, it is generally accepted that the non-local use of the bare reflexive ziji in Mandarin Chinese is sensitive to perspective centers. The introduction of a local first-person pronoun encoding the comprehender’s perspective is assumed to block non-local binding (i.e. make it unavailable), a phenomenon called the blocking effect. However, it is not yet clear whether the blocking effect is absolute, nor whether the syntactic prominence of the blocking pronoun influences the strength of blocking. In this study, we report two sets of offline and online experiments to examine the blocking effect associated with ziji. By comparing the forced choice judgment results in Experiments 1 and 2, we find that syntactically prominent subject blockers lead to stronger blocking compared to object blockers, and that the strength of the blocking effect can be modulated by verb semantics. Furthermore, only subject blockers caused blocking during incremental real-time processing while object blockers did not. The results of these experiments have implications for both the linguistic formulation of the blocking effect and for sentence processing models.
在语言学文献中,人们普遍认为普通话中裸反身词“子己”的非局部使用对透视中心很敏感。假定引入编码理解者视角的局部第一人称代词会阻止非局部绑定(即使其不可用),这种现象称为阻塞效应。然而,目前尚不清楚阻断效应是否是绝对的,也不清楚阻断代词的句法重要性是否会影响阻断的强度。在本研究中,我们报告了两组离线和在线实验来检验与子鸡相关的阻断效应。通过对比实验1和实验2的强迫选择判断结果,我们发现句法突出的主语阻断者比客体阻断者产生更强的阻断效应,并且这种阻断效应的强度可以通过动词语义来调节。此外,在增量实时处理过程中,只有主题阻塞器引起阻塞,而对象阻塞器则没有。这些实验结果对阻塞效应的语言表述和句子加工模型都有启示意义。
{"title":"Unpacking the blocking effect: Syntactic prominence and perspective-taking in antecedent retrieval in Mandarin Chinese","authors":"Jun-Hyun Lyu, E. Kaiser","doi":"10.16995/glossa.5781","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.16995/glossa.5781","url":null,"abstract":"In the linguistics literature, it is generally accepted that the non-local use of the bare reflexive ziji in Mandarin Chinese is sensitive to perspective centers. The introduction of a local first-person pronoun encoding the comprehender’s perspective is assumed to block non-local binding (i.e. make it unavailable), a phenomenon called the blocking effect. However, it is not yet clear whether the blocking effect is absolute, nor whether the syntactic prominence of the blocking pronoun influences the strength of blocking. In this study, we report two sets of offline and online experiments to examine the blocking effect associated with ziji. By comparing the forced choice judgment results in Experiments 1 and 2, we find that syntactically prominent subject blockers lead to stronger blocking compared to object blockers, and that the strength of the blocking effect can be modulated by verb semantics. Furthermore, only subject blockers caused blocking during incremental real-time processing while object blockers did not. The results of these experiments have implications for both the linguistic formulation of the blocking effect and for sentence processing models.","PeriodicalId":46319,"journal":{"name":"Glossa-A Journal of General Linguistics","volume":"68 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.0,"publicationDate":"2021-10-23","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"87180571","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 4
Perspectival factors and pro-drop: A corpus study of speaker/addressee pronouns with creer ‘think/ believe’ and saber ‘know’ in spoken Spanish 视角因素与亲降:西班牙语口语中带有“认为/相信”和“知道”的说话/收件人代词的语料库研究
IF 1 2区 文学 0 LANGUAGE & LINGUISTICS Pub Date : 2021-10-14 DOI: 10.16995/glossa.5873
Peter Herbeck
This paper examines overt and covert speaker/addressee pronouns with the cognitive verbs creer ‘think/believe’ and saber ‘know’ in a corpus of spoken peninsular Spanish – the Madrid and Alcalá samples of PRESEEA (2014– ) – with a focus on 1st person singular (yo) creo que ‘(I) think that’. Departing from the observation made in the literature that overt pronouns are highly frequent with creer and that topic shift cannot account for all of them, it will be argued that perspectival factors related to evidentiality/epistemicity and subjectivity influence overt pronoun realization. A corpus study was conducted to investigate whether (i) [person] and [polarity] and (ii) the type of complement affect overt pronoun realization with the cognitive verbs creer and saber. The results indicate that the type of belief expressed in the embedded clause should be taken into account, as well as person and polarity. The ultimate trigger for phonetic realization of speaker/addressee pronouns will be argued to be the notion of contrast: cognitive verbs whose embedded complement encodes evaluations and non-visual, abstract information have high frequencies of overt pronoun realization because these contexts favor the evoking of alternative perspective holders. Overt pronouns will be analyzed as the result of a [+contrast] feature which is assigned to the specifier of a functional category encoding perspective in the split IP.
本文用认知动词creer ' think/believe '和saber ' know '在半岛西班牙语口语语料库(PRESEEA(2014 -)的马德里和alcal样本)中考察了显性和隐性说话者/受信者代词,重点关注第一人称单数(yo) creo que ' (I) think that '。从文献中观察到的显性代词频繁出现,并且话题转移不能解释所有这些,我们将认为与证据性/认识性和主观性相关的视角因素影响着显性代词的实现。通过语料库研究,探讨了(i)[人]和[极性]以及(ii)补语类型是否影响认知动词creer和saber的显性代词实现。结果表明,嵌入子句中所表达的信念类型,以及人与极性都应考虑在内。说话人/收件人代词语音实现的最终触发因素将被认为是对比的概念:认知动词,其嵌入的补语编码评价和非视觉、抽象信息具有高频率的显性代词实现,因为这些上下文有利于唤起替代视角持有者。显性代词将作为[+对比]特征的结果进行分析,该特征被分配给拆分IP中功能类别编码透视图的说明符。
{"title":"Perspectival factors and pro-drop: A corpus study of speaker/addressee pronouns with creer ‘think/ believe’ and saber ‘know’ in spoken Spanish","authors":"Peter Herbeck","doi":"10.16995/glossa.5873","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.16995/glossa.5873","url":null,"abstract":"This paper examines overt and covert speaker/addressee pronouns with the cognitive verbs creer ‘think/believe’ and saber ‘know’ in a corpus of spoken peninsular Spanish – the Madrid and Alcalá samples of PRESEEA (2014– ) – with a focus on 1st person singular (yo) creo que ‘(I) think that’. Departing from the observation made in the literature that overt pronouns are highly frequent with creer and that topic shift cannot account for all of them, it will be argued that perspectival factors related to evidentiality/epistemicity and subjectivity influence overt pronoun realization. A corpus study was conducted to investigate whether (i) [person] and [polarity] and (ii) the type of complement affect overt pronoun realization with the cognitive verbs creer and saber. The results indicate that the type of belief expressed in the embedded clause should be taken into account, as well as person and polarity. The ultimate trigger for phonetic realization of speaker/addressee pronouns will be argued to be the notion of contrast: cognitive verbs whose embedded complement encodes evaluations and non-visual, abstract information have high frequencies of overt pronoun realization because these contexts favor the evoking of alternative perspective holders. Overt pronouns will be analyzed as the result of a [+contrast] feature which is assigned to the specifier of a functional category encoding perspective in the split IP.","PeriodicalId":46319,"journal":{"name":"Glossa-A Journal of General Linguistics","volume":"2 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.0,"publicationDate":"2021-10-14","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"79085017","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
Timelines and Temporal Pointing in Chinese Sign Language 中国手语的时间线和时间指向
IF 1 2区 文学 0 LANGUAGE & LINGUISTICS Pub Date : 2021-10-07 DOI: 10.16995/glossa.5836
Hao Lin, J. Kuhn, Huan Sheng, Philippe Schlenker
We argue that Chinese Sign Language (CSL) provides new insights into temporal anaphora, as well as new puzzles. Partee 1973 showed that temporal talk in English involves abstract anaphoric mechanisms akin to pronouns, although with a very different form. Schlenker 2013 argued that in American Sign Language (ASL), one and the same overt pronominal form, the pointing sign, can have individual and temporal uses, but his data involved the same loci across domains: no formal property distinguished temporal from individual anaphora. We replicate ASL temporal anaphora data in CSL, but we also display a new finding: CSL allows for locus establishment and anaphoric pointing on two specifically temporal timelines, a sagittal one (past is backwards) and a vertical one (past is up). Not only can temporal anaphora be overt in CSL; it can also be morphologically distinguished from nominal anaphora (various interesting restrictions on the timelines are also described).
我们认为,中国手语(CSL)提供了时间回指的新见解,以及新的难题。parte 1973表明,英语中的时间会话涉及类似于代词的抽象回指机制,尽管形式非常不同。Schlenker 2013认为,在美国手语(ASL)中,一个相同的代词形式,即指向符号,可以有个人和时间的用途,但他的数据涉及跨域的相同位点:没有形式属性区分时间和个人回指。我们在CSL中复制了ASL的时间回指数据,但我们也显示了一个新的发现:CSL允许在两个特定的时间线上建立位点和回指,一个矢状线(过去是向后的)和一个垂直线(过去是向上的)。在CSL中,时间回指不仅是显性的;它也可以在形态上区别于名义回指(也描述了时间线上的各种有趣限制)。
{"title":"Timelines and Temporal Pointing in Chinese Sign Language","authors":"Hao Lin, J. Kuhn, Huan Sheng, Philippe Schlenker","doi":"10.16995/glossa.5836","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.16995/glossa.5836","url":null,"abstract":"We argue that Chinese Sign Language (CSL) provides new insights into temporal anaphora, as well as new puzzles. Partee 1973 showed that temporal talk in English involves abstract anaphoric mechanisms akin to pronouns, although with a very different form. Schlenker 2013 argued that in American Sign Language (ASL), one and the same overt pronominal form, the pointing sign, can have individual and temporal uses, but his data involved the same loci across domains: no formal property distinguished temporal from individual anaphora. We replicate ASL temporal anaphora data in CSL, but we also display a new finding: CSL allows for locus establishment and anaphoric pointing on two specifically temporal timelines, a sagittal one (past is backwards) and a vertical one (past is up). Not only can temporal anaphora be overt in CSL; it can also be morphologically distinguished from nominal anaphora (various interesting restrictions on the timelines are also described).","PeriodicalId":46319,"journal":{"name":"Glossa-A Journal of General Linguistics","volume":"34 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.0,"publicationDate":"2021-10-07","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"78762430","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Formal variation in the Kata Kolok lexicon Kata Kolok词汇的正式变体
IF 1 2区 文学 0 LANGUAGE & LINGUISTICS Pub Date : 2021-10-07 DOI: 10.16995/glossa.5880
H. Lutzenberger, Connie de Vos, O. Crasborn, P. Fikkert
Sign language lexicons incorporate phonological specifications. Evidence from emerging sign languages suggests that phonological structure emerges gradually in a new language. In this study, we investigate variation in the form of signs across 20 deaf adult signers of Kata Kolok, a sign language that emerged spontaneously in a Balinese village community. Combining methods previously used for sign comparisons, we introduce a new numeric measure of variation. Our nuanced yet comprehensive approach to form variation integrates three levels (iconic motivation, surface realisation, feature differences) and allows for refinement through weighting the variation score by token and signer frequency. We demonstrate that variation in the form of signs appears in different degrees at different levels. Token frequency in a given dataset greatly affects how much variation can surface, suggesting caution in interpreting previous findings. Different sign variants have different scopes of use among the signing population, with some more widely used than others. Both frequency weightings (token and signer) identify dominant sign variants, i.e., sign forms that are produced frequently or by many signers. We argue that variation does not equal the absence of conventionalisation. Indeed, especially in micro-community sign languages, variation may be key to understanding patterns of language emergence.Appendices:  https://doi.org/10.17605/OSF.IO/FN4XE
手语词汇包含语音规范。来自新兴手语的证据表明,语音结构在一种新语言中逐渐形成。在这项研究中,我们调查了20名成年聋人Kata Kolok手语形式的变化,Kata Kolok是一种自发出现在巴厘岛村庄社区的手语。结合以前用于符号比较的方法,我们引入了一种新的数值测量变化。我们细致而全面的形成变异的方法集成了三个层面(标志性动机、表面实现、特征差异),并允许通过标记和签名者频率加权变异分数来进行细化。我们证明了符号形式的变化在不同层次上出现的程度不同。给定数据集中的令牌频率极大地影响了可能出现的变化,这表明在解释以前的研究结果时要谨慎。不同的手语变体在手语人群中有不同的使用范围,其中一些比其他更广泛使用。频率加权(令牌和签名者)都确定了主要的符号变体,即频繁产生或由许多签名者产生的符号形式。我们认为,变化并不等于没有约定俗成。的确,特别是在微社区手语中,变异可能是理解语言出现模式的关键。附录:https://doi.org/10.17605/OSF.IO/FN4XE
{"title":"Formal variation in the Kata Kolok lexicon","authors":"H. Lutzenberger, Connie de Vos, O. Crasborn, P. Fikkert","doi":"10.16995/glossa.5880","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.16995/glossa.5880","url":null,"abstract":"Sign language lexicons incorporate phonological specifications. Evidence from emerging sign languages suggests that phonological structure emerges gradually in a new language. In this study, we investigate variation in the form of signs across 20 deaf adult signers of Kata Kolok, a sign language that emerged spontaneously in a Balinese village community. Combining methods previously used for sign comparisons, we introduce a new numeric measure of variation. Our nuanced yet comprehensive approach to form variation integrates three levels (iconic motivation, surface realisation, feature differences) and allows for refinement through weighting the variation score by token and signer frequency. We demonstrate that variation in the form of signs appears in different degrees at different levels. Token frequency in a given dataset greatly affects how much variation can surface, suggesting caution in interpreting previous findings. Different sign variants have different scopes of use among the signing population, with some more widely used than others. Both frequency weightings (token and signer) identify dominant sign variants, i.e., sign forms that are produced frequently or by many signers. We argue that variation does not equal the absence of conventionalisation. Indeed, especially in micro-community sign languages, variation may be key to understanding patterns of language emergence.Appendices:  https://doi.org/10.17605/OSF.IO/FN4XE","PeriodicalId":46319,"journal":{"name":"Glossa-A Journal of General Linguistics","volume":"320 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.0,"publicationDate":"2021-10-07","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"90819590","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 7
期刊
Glossa-A Journal of General Linguistics
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1