首页 > 最新文献

Glossa-A Journal of General Linguistics最新文献

英文 中文
Bilinguals have a single computational system but two compartmentalized phonological grammars: Evidence from code-switching 双语者有一个单一的计算系统,但有两个分开的语音语法:来自代码转换的证据
IF 1 2区 文学 0 LANGUAGE & LINGUISTICS Pub Date : 2021-12-02 DOI: 10.16995/glossa.5800
L. Gosselin
Classic linguistic models, such as Chomsky’s minimalist schematization of the human language faculty, were typically based on a ‘monolingual ideal’. More recently, models have been extended to bilingual cognition. For instance, MacSwan (2000) posited that bilinguals possess a single syntactic computational system and, crucially, two (or more) phonological grammars. The current paper examines this possible architecture of the bilingual language faculty by using code-switching data, since this type of speech is unique to bilingual and multilingual individuals. Specifically, the natural speech Maria, a habitual Spanish-English code-switcher from the Bangor Miami Corpus, was examined. For the interface of phonology, an analysis was completed on the frequency of syllabic structures used by Maria. Phonotactics were examined as Spanish and English impose differential restrictions on complex onsets and codas. The results indicated that Maria’s language of use impacted the phonotactics of her speech, but that the context of use (unilingual or code-switched) did not. This suggests that Maria was alternating between two independent phonological grammars when she was code-switching. For the interface of morphosyntax, syntactic dependencies within Maria’s code-switched speech and past literature were consulted. The evidence illustrates that syntactic dependencies are indeed established within code-switched sentences, indicating that such constructions are derived from a single syntactic subset. Thus, the quantitative and qualitative results from this paper wholly support MacSwan’s original conjectures regarding the bilingual language faculty: bilingual cognition appears to be composed of a single computational system which builds multi-language syntactic structures, and more than one phonological grammar.
经典的语言学模型,如乔姆斯基的人类语言能力的极简图式,通常是基于“单语理想”。最近,模型已经扩展到双语认知。例如,MacSwan(2000)假设双语者拥有单一的句法计算系统,更重要的是,拥有两种(或更多)语音语法。本文通过使用代码转换数据来研究双语语言教师的这种可能结构,因为这种类型的言语是双语和多语言个体所特有的。具体来说,对班戈迈阿密语料库中习惯性的西班牙语-英语代码转换者Maria的自然语言进行了研究。在音系界面上,对Maria使用的音节结构的频率进行了分析。由于西班牙语和英语对复杂的起音和尾音施加了不同的限制,因此检查了语音策略。结果表明,玛丽亚的使用语言影响了她说话的语音策略,但使用语境(单语或代码转换)没有影响。这表明玛丽亚在进行语码转换时,在两种独立的语音语法之间交替。对于形态语法的接口,我们参考了Maria的语码转换语音和过去文献中的句法依赖关系。有证据表明,句法依赖关系确实是在代码转换的句子中建立起来的,这表明这种结构是从单个句法子集派生出来的。因此,本文的定量和定性结果完全支持MacSwan关于双语语言能力的原始猜想:双语认知似乎由构建多语言句法结构的单一计算系统和不止一种语音语法组成。
{"title":"Bilinguals have a single computational system but two compartmentalized phonological grammars: Evidence from code-switching","authors":"L. Gosselin","doi":"10.16995/glossa.5800","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.16995/glossa.5800","url":null,"abstract":"Classic linguistic models, such as Chomsky’s minimalist schematization of the human language faculty, were typically based on a ‘monolingual ideal’. More recently, models have been extended to bilingual cognition. For instance, MacSwan (2000) posited that bilinguals possess a single syntactic computational system and, crucially, two (or more) phonological grammars. The current paper examines this possible architecture of the bilingual language faculty by using code-switching data, since this type of speech is unique to bilingual and multilingual individuals. Specifically, the natural speech Maria, a habitual Spanish-English code-switcher from the Bangor Miami Corpus, was examined. For the interface of phonology, an analysis was completed on the frequency of syllabic structures used by Maria. Phonotactics were examined as Spanish and English impose differential restrictions on complex onsets and codas. The results indicated that Maria’s language of use impacted the phonotactics of her speech, but that the context of use (unilingual or code-switched) did not. This suggests that Maria was alternating between two independent phonological grammars when she was code-switching. For the interface of morphosyntax, syntactic dependencies within Maria’s code-switched speech and past literature were consulted. The evidence illustrates that syntactic dependencies are indeed established within code-switched sentences, indicating that such constructions are derived from a single syntactic subset. Thus, the quantitative and qualitative results from this paper wholly support MacSwan’s original conjectures regarding the bilingual language faculty: bilingual cognition appears to be composed of a single computational system which builds multi-language syntactic structures, and more than one phonological grammar.","PeriodicalId":46319,"journal":{"name":"Glossa-A Journal of General Linguistics","volume":"4 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.0,"publicationDate":"2021-12-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"81914197","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Same: structural sources of anaphora and relativization 同样:回指和相对化的结构来源
IF 1 2区 文学 0 LANGUAGE & LINGUISTICS Pub Date : 2021-12-01 DOI: 10.16995/glossa.5811
Emily A. Hanink
This paper offers a unified view of the structures giving rise to anaphora and relativization within same-expressions. Taking as a point of departure the proposal that indices occupy syntactic positions in anaphoric DPs (Schwarz 2009, Simonenko 2014, a.o.), I examine the open issue of how anaphoric modifiers, particularly same, are predicted to interact with this proposed structural complexity. Based on the morphosyntactic behavior of definite descriptions with same, I argue that indices may be housed in DP structure in different ways. I argue moreover that the behavior of same in both anaphoric and non-anaphoric contexts supports the view that same is syntactically an equative degree head (Alrenga 2007, Oxford 2010), whose selectional properties are mirrored in a range of equative constructions. Finally, I propose an analysis of as-relatives selected by same that accounts for their alternation with anaphora, and offer a comparison with restrictive relatives.
本文对相同表达中产生回指和相对化的结构提出了统一的观点。以索引在回指式DPs中占据句法位置的建议为出发点(Schwarz 2009, Simonenko 2014, a.o.),我研究了如何预测回指修辞格,特别是相同修辞格,与这种提出的结构复杂性相互作用的开放性问题。基于相同的确定描述的形态句法行为,我认为索引可以以不同的方式被安置在DP结构中。此外,我认为,在回指和非回指语境中,相同的行为支持了这样一种观点,即相同在句法上是一个等度头(Alrenga 2007, Oxford 2010),其选择特性反映在一系列相等结构中。最后,我提出了相同选择的作为亲属的分析,解释了他们与回指的交替,并提供了与限制性亲属的比较。
{"title":"Same: structural sources of anaphora and relativization","authors":"Emily A. Hanink","doi":"10.16995/glossa.5811","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.16995/glossa.5811","url":null,"abstract":"This paper offers a unified view of the structures giving rise to anaphora and relativization within same-expressions. Taking as a point of departure the proposal that indices occupy syntactic positions in anaphoric DPs (Schwarz 2009, Simonenko 2014, a.o.), I examine the open issue of how anaphoric modifiers, particularly same, are predicted to interact with this proposed structural complexity. Based on the morphosyntactic behavior of definite descriptions with same, I argue that indices may be housed in DP structure in different ways. I argue moreover that the behavior of same in both anaphoric and non-anaphoric contexts supports the view that same is syntactically an equative degree head (Alrenga 2007, Oxford 2010), whose selectional properties are mirrored in a range of equative constructions. Finally, I propose an analysis of as-relatives selected by same that accounts for their alternation with anaphora, and offer a comparison with restrictive relatives.","PeriodicalId":46319,"journal":{"name":"Glossa-A Journal of General Linguistics","volume":"55 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.0,"publicationDate":"2021-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"88505523","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
An alternatives account of 'most’ and 'more than half’ “most”和“more half”的替代说法
IF 1 2区 文学 0 LANGUAGE & LINGUISTICS Pub Date : 2021-12-01 DOI: 10.16995/glossa.5764
Fausto Carcassi, Jakub Szymanik
While 'most' and 'more than half' are generally assumed to be truth-conditionally equivalent, the former is usually interpreted as conveying greater proportions than the latter. Previous work has attempted to explain this difference in terms of pragmatic strengthening or variation in meanings. In this paper, we propose a novel explanation that keeps the truth-conditions equivalence. We argue that the difference in typical sets between the two expressions emerges as a result of two previously independently motivated mechanisms. First, the two expressions have different sets of pragmatic alternatives. Second, listeners tend to minimize the expected distance between their representation of the world and the speaker's observation. We support this explanation with a computational model of usage in the Rational Speech Act framework. Moreover, we report the results of a quantifier production experiment. We find that the difference in typical proportions associated with the two expressions can be explained by our account.
虽然“大多数”和“一半以上”通常被认为是有条件的真理,但前者通常被解释为比后者传达更大的比例。以前的研究试图从语用强化或意义变化的角度来解释这种差异。本文提出了一种保持真值条件等价的新解释。我们认为,这两个表达式之间的典型集合的差异是由于两个先前独立的激励机制而产生的。首先,这两个表达有不同的语用选择。第二,听者倾向于将他们对世界的描述与说话者的观察之间的预期距离最小化。我们用Rational Speech Act框架中使用的计算模型来支持这个解释。此外,我们报告了一个量词生产实验的结果。我们发现,与这两种表达相关的典型比例的差异可以用我们的解释来解释。
{"title":"An alternatives account of 'most’ and 'more than half’","authors":"Fausto Carcassi, Jakub Szymanik","doi":"10.16995/glossa.5764","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.16995/glossa.5764","url":null,"abstract":"While 'most' and 'more than half' are generally assumed to be truth-conditionally equivalent, the former is usually interpreted as conveying greater proportions than the latter. Previous work has attempted to explain this difference in terms of pragmatic strengthening or variation in meanings. In this paper, we propose a novel explanation that keeps the truth-conditions equivalence. We argue that the difference in typical sets between the two expressions emerges as a result of two previously independently motivated mechanisms. First, the two expressions have different sets of pragmatic alternatives. Second, listeners tend to minimize the expected distance between their representation of the world and the speaker's observation. We support this explanation with a computational model of usage in the Rational Speech Act framework. Moreover, we report the results of a quantifier production experiment. We find that the difference in typical proportions associated with the two expressions can be explained by our account.","PeriodicalId":46319,"journal":{"name":"Glossa-A Journal of General Linguistics","volume":"21 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.0,"publicationDate":"2021-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"89141022","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 3
On the synchrony, variation and diachrony of adverbial exceptive clauses in Polish 论波兰语状语例外从句的共时性、变型和历时性
IF 1 2区 文学 0 LANGUAGE & LINGUISTICS Pub Date : 2021-12-01 DOI: 10.16995/glossa.5694
Łukasz Jędrzejowski
In this article, I examine the synchrony and diachrony of adverbial exceptive clauses in Polish headed by the complex complementizer 'chyba że' (unless). Synchronically, I argue that 'chyba-że'-clauses are syntactically nonintegrated adverbial clauses and that they cannot be analyzed as negated conditionals, although both clause types can give rise to an exceptive interpretation. Diachronically, I provide an analysis according to which 'chyba że' (unless) is treated as a complex C-head that is due to head adjunction of the discourse particle 'chyba' (presumably) and of the declarative complementizer 'że' (that). Essentially, there are three main factors that paved the way for the development of the exceptive complementizer in Polish: i) syntactic adjacency of the discourse particle 'chyba' (presumably) and of the declarative complementizer 'że' (that) establishing a subordination relation between the matrix clause and the embedded clause, ii) movement of the conditional clitic 'by' from MoodP to the CP domain, and iii) accommodation of negation of focus alternatives. As it will turn out, this process was completed in Middle Polish (1543-1765).
在这篇文章中,我研究了波兰语中以复合补语“chyba że”(除非)为首的状语从句的共时性和历时性。同时,我认为'chyba-że'-子句在句法上是非整合的状语从句,它们不能被分析为否定条件句,尽管这两种子句类型都可以产生例外的解释。在历时上,我提供了一个分析,根据这个分析,“chyba że”(除非)被视为一个复杂的c字头,这是由于话语助词“chyba”(大概)和陈述补语“że”(that)的头部连接。从本质上讲,有三个主要因素为波兰语中例外补语的发展铺平了道路:1)语篇小品“chyba”(大概)和陈述性补语“że”(可能)在句法上的邻接性(即)在基质分句和嵌入分句之间建立了从属关系;2)条件从句“by”从MoodP到CP域的移动;3)对焦点替代的否定的适应。事实证明,这一过程是在中波兰(1543-1765)完成的。
{"title":"On the synchrony, variation and diachrony of adverbial exceptive clauses in Polish","authors":"Łukasz Jędrzejowski","doi":"10.16995/glossa.5694","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.16995/glossa.5694","url":null,"abstract":"In this article, I examine the synchrony and diachrony of adverbial exceptive clauses in Polish headed by the complex complementizer 'chyba że' (unless). Synchronically, I argue that 'chyba-że'-clauses are syntactically nonintegrated adverbial clauses and that they cannot be analyzed as negated conditionals, although both clause types can give rise to an exceptive interpretation. Diachronically, I provide an analysis according to which 'chyba że' (unless) is treated as a complex C-head that is due to head adjunction of the discourse particle 'chyba' (presumably) and of the declarative complementizer 'że' (that). Essentially, there are three main factors that paved the way for the development of the exceptive complementizer in Polish: i) syntactic adjacency of the discourse particle 'chyba' (presumably) and of the declarative complementizer 'że' (that) establishing a subordination relation between the matrix clause and the embedded clause, ii) movement of the conditional clitic 'by' from MoodP to the CP domain, and iii) accommodation of negation of focus alternatives. As it will turn out, this process was completed in Middle Polish (1543-1765).","PeriodicalId":46319,"journal":{"name":"Glossa-A Journal of General Linguistics","volume":"29 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.0,"publicationDate":"2021-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"91249094","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
An experimental reassessment of complex NP islands with NP-scrambling in Japanese 日本复杂NP岛与NP-争先恐后的实验再评估
IF 1 2区 文学 0 LANGUAGE & LINGUISTICS Pub Date : 2021-11-30 DOI: 10.16995/glossa.5737
Shin-ichi Fukuda, Nozomi Tanaka, Hajime Ono, Jon Sprouse
There is little consensus in the Japanese syntax literature on the question of whether complex NPs with a complement clause headed by to yuu ‘that say’ are islands for NP-scrambling dependencies. To explore this question, we conducted two acceptability judgment experiments using the factorial definition of islands to test the island status of noun complements, relative clauses (which are complex NPs, but uniformly considered islands in the literature), and coordinated NP structures (which are also uniformly considered islands in the literature). Our first experiment yielded clear evidence that relative clauses and coordinated NPs are islands, and that noun complements are not. Our second experiment replicated the relative clause and coordinated NP results, but yielded an inconclusive null result for noun complements. Taken together, our results suggest either that noun complements are not islands, or that noun complements yield a small island effect that cannot be reliably detected at the typical sample sizes of 30-40 participants used here. We also investigated between- and within-participant variability in our results. We observe no evidence of increased between-participant variability for noun complements relative to other islands, and no increase of within-participant variability for noun complements relative to grammatical NP-scrambling, thus corroborating our conclusions. Our results have consequences for a number of issues that have been encoded in current syntactic theories of island effects, including the correlation between syntactic constituent complexity and island status (e.g., number of bounding nodes or phase heads), and the correlation between complementizer deletion and island status (e.g., the complement/adjunct distinction).
在日语语法文献中,对于带有以you’that’为首的补语从句的复杂np是否为np争先恐后依赖关系的岛屿,几乎没有达成共识。为了探讨这个问题,我们使用岛屿的析因定义进行了两个可接受性判断实验,以测试名词补语、关系从句(它们是复杂的NP,但在文献中被统一认为是岛屿)和协调NP结构(在文献中也被统一认为是岛屿)的岛屿状态。我们的第一个实验得到了明确的证据,即关系从句和协调的np是孤立的,而名词补语不是。我们的第二个实验复制了关系从句和协调NP结果,但对名词补语产生了不确定的null结果。综上所述,我们的结果表明,名词补语不是岛屿,或者名词补语产生了一个小岛效应,在这里使用的30-40名参与者的典型样本量中无法可靠地检测到。我们还调查了结果中参与者之间和参与者内部的变异性。我们没有观察到与其他岛屿相比,名词补语在参与者之间的可变性增加的证据,并且相对于语法np混乱,名词补语在参与者内部的可变性也没有增加,从而证实了我们的结论。我们的结果对当前孤岛效应句法理论中编码的许多问题产生了影响,包括句法组成复杂性与孤岛状态之间的相关性(例如,边界节点或相头的数量),以及补语缺失与孤岛状态之间的相关性(例如,补语/补语区分)。
{"title":"An experimental reassessment of complex NP islands with NP-scrambling in Japanese","authors":"Shin-ichi Fukuda, Nozomi Tanaka, Hajime Ono, Jon Sprouse","doi":"10.16995/glossa.5737","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.16995/glossa.5737","url":null,"abstract":"There is little consensus in the Japanese syntax literature on the question of whether complex NPs with a complement clause headed by to yuu ‘that say’ are islands for NP-scrambling dependencies. To explore this question, we conducted two acceptability judgment experiments using the factorial definition of islands to test the island status of noun complements, relative clauses (which are complex NPs, but uniformly considered islands in the literature), and coordinated NP structures (which are also uniformly considered islands in the literature). Our first experiment yielded clear evidence that relative clauses and coordinated NPs are islands, and that noun complements are not. Our second experiment replicated the relative clause and coordinated NP results, but yielded an inconclusive null result for noun complements. Taken together, our results suggest either that noun complements are not islands, or that noun complements yield a small island effect that cannot be reliably detected at the typical sample sizes of 30-40 participants used here. We also investigated between- and within-participant variability in our results. We observe no evidence of increased between-participant variability for noun complements relative to other islands, and no increase of within-participant variability for noun complements relative to grammatical NP-scrambling, thus corroborating our conclusions. Our results have consequences for a number of issues that have been encoded in current syntactic theories of island effects, including the correlation between syntactic constituent complexity and island status (e.g., number of bounding nodes or phase heads), and the correlation between complementizer deletion and island status (e.g., the complement/adjunct distinction).","PeriodicalId":46319,"journal":{"name":"Glossa-A Journal of General Linguistics","volume":"211 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.0,"publicationDate":"2021-11-30","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"84944207","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
The interpretative options of anaphoric complex demonstratives 回指复合指示语的解释选择
IF 1 2区 文学 0 LANGUAGE & LINGUISTICS Pub Date : 2021-11-25 DOI: 10.16995/glossa.5700
S. Hinterwimmer, Umesh Patil
In this paper, we present experimental evidence from a ‘yes’/’no’ judgement task and twoacceptability rating studies (Experiments 1a-c) for the claim made in Hinterwimmer (2019) thatsentences with two anaphorically interpreted complex demonstratives are less acceptable thansentences with two anaphorically interpreted definite descriptions and sentences where one ofthe two previously introduced referents is picked up by a complex demonstrative, while the otherone is picked up by a definite description. The results of Experiment 1a and 1b are in principlecompatible with the account argued for in Hinterwimmer (2019), according to which the (potentiallyabstract) demonstrations presupposed by demonstratives may not have overlapping trajectories.However, sentences with two anaphorically interpreted complex demonstratives are not judgedas unacceptable as would be expected if they involved a presupposition violation. Therefore, wepropose an alternative, economy-based pragmatic account that builds on Ahn (2019) and Nowak(2019). The question of whether the observed pattern is more compatible with the accountproposed by Hinterwimmer (2019) or the alternative pragmatic account is directly addressed in afurther acceptability rating study (Experiment 1c). The design of that study is similar to that ofExperiment 1b, but it includes as fillers both sentences clearly violating a presupposition andsentences violating a pragmatic constraint. Since the ratings for sentences containing twoanaphorically interpreted complex demonstratives are closer to the ratings for sentences violatinga pragmatic constraint than for sentences violating a presupposition, we conclude that thealternative pragmatic account is preferable to the account by Hinterwimmer (2019).
在本文中,我们提供了来自“是”/“否”判断任务和两个可接受性评级研究(实验1a-c)的实验证据,以证明Hinterwimmer(2019)提出的主张,即具有两个回指解释的复杂指示词的句子比具有两个回指解释的确定描述的句子和两个先前引入的指称物之一被一个复杂指示词拾取的句子更容易被接受。而另一个则是由一个明确的描述挑选出来的。实验1a和1b的结果原则上与Hinterwimmer(2019)中提出的解释一致,根据该解释,指示物预设的(潜在抽象的)演示可能没有重叠的轨迹。然而,有两个回指解释的复杂指示的句子并不像预期的那样不可接受,如果它们涉及违反预设。因此,我们在Ahn(2019)和Nowak(2019)的基础上提出了另一种基于经济的实用主义解释。观察到的模式是否更符合Hinterwimmer(2019)提出的解释,还是另一种语用解释,这一问题将在进一步的可接受性评级研究中直接解决(实验1c)。该研究的设计与实验1b相似,但它包括明显违反预设的句子和违反语用约束的句子作为填充。由于包含两个照旧解释的复杂指示语的句子的评分更接近于违反语用约束的句子的评分,而不是违反预设的句子,因此我们得出结论,替代语用解释比Hinterwimmer(2019)的说法更可取。
{"title":"The interpretative options of anaphoric complex demonstratives","authors":"S. Hinterwimmer, Umesh Patil","doi":"10.16995/glossa.5700","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.16995/glossa.5700","url":null,"abstract":"In this paper, we present experimental evidence from a ‘yes’/’no’ judgement task and twoacceptability rating studies (Experiments 1a-c) for the claim made in Hinterwimmer (2019) thatsentences with two anaphorically interpreted complex demonstratives are less acceptable thansentences with two anaphorically interpreted definite descriptions and sentences where one ofthe two previously introduced referents is picked up by a complex demonstrative, while the otherone is picked up by a definite description. The results of Experiment 1a and 1b are in principlecompatible with the account argued for in Hinterwimmer (2019), according to which the (potentiallyabstract) demonstrations presupposed by demonstratives may not have overlapping trajectories.However, sentences with two anaphorically interpreted complex demonstratives are not judgedas unacceptable as would be expected if they involved a presupposition violation. Therefore, wepropose an alternative, economy-based pragmatic account that builds on Ahn (2019) and Nowak(2019). The question of whether the observed pattern is more compatible with the accountproposed by Hinterwimmer (2019) or the alternative pragmatic account is directly addressed in afurther acceptability rating study (Experiment 1c). The design of that study is similar to that ofExperiment 1b, but it includes as fillers both sentences clearly violating a presupposition andsentences violating a pragmatic constraint. Since the ratings for sentences containing twoanaphorically interpreted complex demonstratives are closer to the ratings for sentences violatinga pragmatic constraint than for sentences violating a presupposition, we conclude that thealternative pragmatic account is preferable to the account by Hinterwimmer (2019).","PeriodicalId":46319,"journal":{"name":"Glossa-A Journal of General Linguistics","volume":"19 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.0,"publicationDate":"2021-11-25","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"80747900","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Title Pending 8211 未决所有权8211
IF 1 2区 文学 0 LANGUAGE & LINGUISTICS Pub Date : 2021-11-23 DOI: 10.16995/glossa.8211
One proposed mechanism of language change concerns the role played by second-language (L2) learners in situations of language contact. If sufficiently many L2 speakers are present in a speech community in relation to the number of first-language (L1) speakers, then those features which present a difficulty in L2 acquisition may be prone to disappearing from the language. This paper presents a mathematical account of such contact situations based on a stochastic model of learning and nonlinear population dynamics. The equilibria of a deterministic reduction of the model, describing a mixed population of L1 and L2 speakers, are fully characterized. Whether or not the language changes in response to the introduction of L2 learners turns out to depend on three factors: the overall proportion of L2 learners in the population, the strength of the difficulty speakers face in acquiring the language as an L2, and the language-internal utilities of the competing linguistic variants. These factors are related by a mathematical formula describing a phase transition from retention of the L2-difficult feature to its loss from both speaker populations. This supplies predictions that can be tested against empirical data. Here, the model is evaluated with the help of two case studies, morphological levelling in Afrikaans and the erosion of null subjects in Afro-Peruvian Spanish; the model is found to be broadly in agreement with the historical development in both cases.
{"title":"Title Pending 8211","authors":"","doi":"10.16995/glossa.8211","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.16995/glossa.8211","url":null,"abstract":"One proposed mechanism of language change concerns the role played by second-language (L2) learners in situations of language contact. If sufficiently many L2 speakers are present in a speech community in relation to the number of first-language (L1) speakers, then those features which present a difficulty in L2 acquisition may be prone to disappearing from the language. This paper presents a mathematical account of such contact situations based on a stochastic model of learning and nonlinear population dynamics. The equilibria of a deterministic reduction of the model, describing a mixed population of L1 and L2 speakers, are fully characterized. Whether or not the language changes in response to the introduction of L2 learners turns out to depend on three factors: the overall proportion of L2 learners in the population, the strength of the difficulty speakers face in acquiring the language as an L2, and the language-internal utilities of the competing linguistic variants. These factors are related by a mathematical formula describing a phase transition from retention of the L2-difficult feature to its loss from both speaker populations. This supplies predictions that can be tested against empirical data. Here, the model is evaluated with the help of two case studies, morphological levelling in Afrikaans and the erosion of null subjects in Afro-Peruvian Spanish; the model is found to be broadly in agreement with the historical development in both cases.","PeriodicalId":46319,"journal":{"name":"Glossa-A Journal of General Linguistics","volume":"IA-15 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.0,"publicationDate":"2021-11-23","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"84593845","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
The Credit Problem in parametric stress: A probabilistic approach 参数应力下的信用问题:一种概率方法
IF 1 2区 文学 0 LANGUAGE & LINGUISTICS Pub Date : 2021-11-18 DOI: 10.16995/glossa.5884
A. Nazarov, G. Jarosz
In this paper, we introduce a novel domain-general, statistical learning model for P&P grammars: the Expectation Driven Parameter Learner (EDPL). We show that the EDPL provides a mathematically principled solution to the Credit Problem (Dresher 1999). We present the first systematic tests of the EDPL and an existing and closely related model, the Naïve Parameter Learner (NPL), on a full stress typology, the one generated by Dresher & Kaye’s (1990) stress parameter framework. This framework has figured prominently in the debate about the necessity of domain-specific mechanisms for learning of parametric stress. The essential difference between the two learning models is that the EDPL incorporates a mechanism that directly tackles the Credit Problem, while the NPL does not. We find that the NPL fails to cope with the ambiguity of this stress system both in terms of learning success and data complexity, while the EDPL performs well on both metrics. Based on these results, we argue that probabilistic inference provides a viable domain-general approach to parametric stress learning, but only when learning involves an inferential process that directly addresses the Credit Problem. We also present in-depth analyses of the learning outcomes, showing how learning outcomes depend crucially on the structural ambiguities posited by a particular phonological theory, and how these learning difficulties correspond to typological gaps.
在本文中,我们引入了一种新的领域通用的P&P语法统计学习模型:期望驱动参数学习器(EDPL)。我们证明了EDPL为信用问题提供了一个数学上有原则的解决方案(Dresher 1999)。我们提出了EDPL的第一个系统测试和一个现有的和密切相关的模型,Naïve参数学习器(NPL),在一个完整的应力类型上,由Dresher和Kaye(1990)的应力参数框架生成。该框架在关于参数应力学习的特定领域机制的必要性的辩论中占有突出地位。两种学习模型之间的本质区别在于,EDPL包含了直接解决信用问题的机制,而NPL则没有。我们发现,在学习成功和数据复杂性方面,NPL未能处理这种压力系统的模糊性,而EDPL在这两个指标上都表现良好。基于这些结果,我们认为概率推理为参数应力学习提供了一种可行的域通用方法,但仅当学习涉及直接解决信用问题的推理过程时。我们还对学习结果进行了深入分析,展示了学习结果如何在很大程度上取决于特定音系理论所提出的结构歧义,以及这些学习困难如何与类型学差距相对应。
{"title":"The Credit Problem in parametric stress: A probabilistic approach","authors":"A. Nazarov, G. Jarosz","doi":"10.16995/glossa.5884","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.16995/glossa.5884","url":null,"abstract":"In this paper, we introduce a novel domain-general, statistical learning model for P&P grammars: the Expectation Driven Parameter Learner (EDPL). We show that the EDPL provides a mathematically principled solution to the Credit Problem (Dresher 1999). We present the first systematic tests of the EDPL and an existing and closely related model, the Naïve Parameter Learner (NPL), on a full stress typology, the one generated by Dresher & Kaye’s (1990) stress parameter framework. This framework has figured prominently in the debate about the necessity of domain-specific mechanisms for learning of parametric stress. The essential difference between the two learning models is that the EDPL incorporates a mechanism that directly tackles the Credit Problem, while the NPL does not. We find that the NPL fails to cope with the ambiguity of this stress system both in terms of learning success and data complexity, while the EDPL performs well on both metrics. Based on these results, we argue that probabilistic inference provides a viable domain-general approach to parametric stress learning, but only when learning involves an inferential process that directly addresses the Credit Problem. We also present in-depth analyses of the learning outcomes, showing how learning outcomes depend crucially on the structural ambiguities posited by a particular phonological theory, and how these learning difficulties correspond to typological gaps.","PeriodicalId":46319,"journal":{"name":"Glossa-A Journal of General Linguistics","volume":"18 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.0,"publicationDate":"2021-11-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"72533765","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
The acquisition of adverbs in trilingual children: does transfer play any role? 三语儿童副词习得:迁移是否起作用?
IF 1 2区 文学 0 LANGUAGE & LINGUISTICS Pub Date : 2021-11-17 DOI: 10.16995/glossa.5721
M. Pîrvulescu, Virginia Hill, Nadia Nacif, Rena Helms-Park, M. Petrescu
In this paper, we focus on the acquisition of adverbs in order to identify the role of transfer in the context of trilingual language acquisition. The trilingual data for our study comes from children born in Canada who have Romanian as heritage language and are exposed to French in school and to English as the societal language. As these children are of school age, our study provides important insights since trilingualism in children is less studied. The variable under scrutiny is the position of adverbs that conform to Cinque’s (1999) hierarchy in the adult grammar of each language. The results indicate that children have adult competence for adverb placement in all three languages. However, some errors arise in French signaling transfer of parametric settings from Romanian. We conclude that this is a non-facilitative transfer justified by the typological proximity in parametric settings between Romanian and French.
本文以副词的习得为研究对象,探讨迁移在三语语言习得中的作用。我们研究的三语数据来自出生在加拿大的孩子,他们的传统语言是罗马尼亚语,在学校接触法语,在社会上接触英语。由于这些儿童处于学龄,我们的研究提供了重要的见解,因为儿童的三语能力研究较少。仔细研究的变量是符合Cinque(1999)在每种语言的成人语法中的层次结构的副词的位置。结果表明,儿童在这三种语言中都具有副词放置的成人能力。然而,在从罗马尼亚语参数设置的法语信号传输中出现一些错误。我们得出结论,这是一种非促进性迁移,罗马尼亚语和法语在参数设置中的类型学接近证明了这一点。
{"title":"The acquisition of adverbs in trilingual children: does transfer play any role?","authors":"M. Pîrvulescu, Virginia Hill, Nadia Nacif, Rena Helms-Park, M. Petrescu","doi":"10.16995/glossa.5721","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.16995/glossa.5721","url":null,"abstract":"In this paper, we focus on the acquisition of adverbs in order to identify the role of transfer in the context of trilingual language acquisition. The trilingual data for our study comes from children born in Canada who have Romanian as heritage language and are exposed to French in school and to English as the societal language. As these children are of school age, our study provides important insights since trilingualism in children is less studied. The variable under scrutiny is the position of adverbs that conform to Cinque’s (1999) hierarchy in the adult grammar of each language. The results indicate that children have adult competence for adverb placement in all three languages. However, some errors arise in French signaling transfer of parametric settings from Romanian. We conclude that this is a non-facilitative transfer justified by the typological proximity in parametric settings between Romanian and French.","PeriodicalId":46319,"journal":{"name":"Glossa-A Journal of General Linguistics","volume":"16 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.0,"publicationDate":"2021-11-17","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"82762967","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Not all diatheses are created equal: Evidence from semantic drifts 并非所有的素质都是平等的:来自语义漂移的证据
IF 1 2区 文学 0 LANGUAGE & LINGUISTICS Pub Date : 2021-11-15 DOI: 10.16995/glossa.5814
Noa Brandel
This paper examines the distribution of Modern Hebrew semantic drifts across four diatheses (voices): transitives, unaccusatives (anticausatives), adjectival (stative) passives, and verbal (eventive) passives. A quantitative survey of dictionaries reveals a discrepancy between these diatheses: Only transitives, unaccusatives, and adjectival passives can give rise to unique semantic drifts, unshared with their related root counterparts, while verbal passives cannot. A corpus-based study shows that frequency is unable to account for this finding; nor can approaches demarcating a syntactic domain for special meanings. I argue that semantic drifts are stored as subentries of the entries from which they evolved, as long as the drift’s frequency remains comparably small. Once its frequency surpasses that of the original entry, the drift is stored as an independent lexical entry. In light of that, I suggest that predicates giving rise to unique semantic drifts have to constitute lexical entries. It thus follows that transitives, unaccusatives, and adjectival passives are formed and listed in the lexicon, while verbal passives are not. Consequently, the lexicon is argued to function as an active (operational) component of the grammar, contra syntacticocentric approaches.
本文考察了现代希伯来语语义漂移的分布,横跨四种特征(语态):及物、非宾格(反谓语)、形容词(静态)被动和动词(事件)被动。对词典的定量调查揭示了这些特征之间的差异:只有及物、非宾格和形容词被动语态会产生独特的语义漂移,与相关的词根对等物不共享,而动词性被动语态则不会。一项基于语料库的研究表明,频率无法解释这一发现;方法也不能为特殊意义划分句法领域。我认为,只要语义漂移的频率保持相对较小,语义漂移就会被存储为它们进化而来的条目的子条目。一旦漂移的频率超过原始条目的频率,它就被存储为一个独立的词法条目。鉴于此,我建议引起独特语义漂移的谓词必须构成词汇条目。因此,及物、非宾格和形容词被动语态形成并被列在词典中,而动词性被动语态则不被列在词典中。因此,与以句法为中心的方法相反,词典被认为是语法的一个主动(操作)组成部分。
{"title":"Not all diatheses are created equal: Evidence from semantic drifts","authors":"Noa Brandel","doi":"10.16995/glossa.5814","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.16995/glossa.5814","url":null,"abstract":"This paper examines the distribution of Modern Hebrew semantic drifts across four diatheses (voices): transitives, unaccusatives (anticausatives), adjectival (stative) passives, and verbal (eventive) passives. A quantitative survey of dictionaries reveals a discrepancy between these diatheses: Only transitives, unaccusatives, and adjectival passives can give rise to unique semantic drifts, unshared with their related root counterparts, while verbal passives cannot. A corpus-based study shows that frequency is unable to account for this finding; nor can approaches demarcating a syntactic domain for special meanings. I argue that semantic drifts are stored as subentries of the entries from which they evolved, as long as the drift’s frequency remains comparably small. Once its frequency surpasses that of the original entry, the drift is stored as an independent lexical entry. In light of that, I suggest that predicates giving rise to unique semantic drifts have to constitute lexical entries. It thus follows that transitives, unaccusatives, and adjectival passives are formed and listed in the lexicon, while verbal passives are not. Consequently, the lexicon is argued to function as an active (operational) component of the grammar, contra syntacticocentric approaches.","PeriodicalId":46319,"journal":{"name":"Glossa-A Journal of General Linguistics","volume":"123 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.0,"publicationDate":"2021-11-15","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"91112087","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Glossa-A Journal of General Linguistics
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1