首页 > 最新文献

Cognitive Research-Principles and Implications最新文献

英文 中文
Where scrollbars are clicked, and why. 点击滚动条的位置和原因。
IF 4.1 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-04-19 DOI: 10.1186/s41235-024-00551-z
Oliver Herbort, Philipp Raßbach, Wilfried Kunde

Scrolling is a widely used mean to interact with visual displays, usually to move content to a certain target location on the display. Understanding how user scroll might identify potentially suboptimal use and allows to infer users' intentions. In the present study, we examined where users click on a scrollbar depending on the intended scrolling action. In two online experiments, click positions were systematically adapted to the intended scrolling action. Click position selection could not be explained as strict optimization of the distance traveled with the cursor, memory load, or motor-cognitive factors. By contrast, for identical scrolling actions click positions strongly depended on the context and on previous scrolls. The behavior of our participants closely resembled behavior observed for manipulation of other physical devices and suggested a simple heuristic of movement planning. The results have implications for modeling human-computer interaction and may contribute to predicting user behavior.

滚动是一种广泛使用的与视觉显示器交互的方式,通常是将内容移动到显示器上的某个目标位置。了解用户滚动的方式可以识别潜在的次优使用方式,并推断用户的意图。在本研究中,我们考察了用户根据预期滚动操作点击滚动条的位置。在两个在线实验中,点击位置被系统地调整以适应预期的滚动操作。点击位置的选择不能解释为光标移动距离的严格优化、记忆负担或运动认知因素。相反,对于相同的滚动动作,点击位置在很大程度上取决于上下文和之前的滚动。我们的参与者的行为与操作其他物理设备时观察到的行为十分相似,这表明他们有一个简单的运动规划启发式。这些结果对人机交互建模具有重要意义,并可能有助于预测用户行为。
{"title":"Where scrollbars are clicked, and why.","authors":"Oliver Herbort, Philipp Raßbach, Wilfried Kunde","doi":"10.1186/s41235-024-00551-z","DOIUrl":"10.1186/s41235-024-00551-z","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Scrolling is a widely used mean to interact with visual displays, usually to move content to a certain target location on the display. Understanding how user scroll might identify potentially suboptimal use and allows to infer users' intentions. In the present study, we examined where users click on a scrollbar depending on the intended scrolling action. In two online experiments, click positions were systematically adapted to the intended scrolling action. Click position selection could not be explained as strict optimization of the distance traveled with the cursor, memory load, or motor-cognitive factors. By contrast, for identical scrolling actions click positions strongly depended on the context and on previous scrolls. The behavior of our participants closely resembled behavior observed for manipulation of other physical devices and suggested a simple heuristic of movement planning. The results have implications for modeling human-computer interaction and may contribute to predicting user behavior.</p>","PeriodicalId":46827,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Research-Principles and Implications","volume":"9 1","pages":"23"},"PeriodicalIF":4.1,"publicationDate":"2024-04-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11026321/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140865390","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The effect of task load, information reliability and interdependency on anticipation performance. 任务负荷、信息可靠性和相互依赖性对预测绩效的影响。
IF 4.1 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-04-14 DOI: 10.1186/s41235-024-00548-8
Colm P Murphy, Oliver R Runswick, N Viktor Gredin, David P Broadbent

In sport, coaches often explicitly provide athletes with stable contextual information related to opponent action preferences to enhance anticipation performance. This information can be dependent on, or independent of, dynamic contextual information that only emerges during the sequence of play (e.g. opponent positioning). The interdependency between contextual information sources, and the associated cognitive demands of integrating information sources during anticipation, has not yet been systematically examined. We used a temporal occlusion paradigm to alter the reliability of contextual and kinematic information during the early, mid- and final phases of a two-versus-two soccer anticipation task. A dual-task paradigm was incorporated to investigate the impact of task load on skilled soccer players' ability to integrate information and update their judgements in each phase. Across conditions, participants received no contextual information (control) or stable contextual information (opponent preferences) that was dependent on, or independent of, dynamic contextual information (opponent positioning). As predicted, participants used reliable contextual and kinematic information to enhance anticipation. Further exploratory analysis suggested that increased task load detrimentally affected anticipation accuracy but only when both reliable contextual and kinematic information were available for integration in the final phase. This effect was observed irrespective of whether the stable contextual information was dependent on, or independent of, dynamic contextual information. Findings suggest that updating anticipatory judgements in the final phase of a sequence of play based on the integration of reliable contextual and kinematic information requires cognitive resources.

在体育运动中,教练通常会明确地向运动员提供与对手动作偏好相关的稳定情境信息,以提高预判能力。这些信息可能依赖于,也可能独立于只有在比赛过程中才会出现的动态背景信息(如对手位置)。语境信息源之间的相互依存关系,以及在预测过程中整合信息源的相关认知要求,尚未得到系统研究。我们使用了时间遮挡范式来改变二对二足球预测任务早期、中期和最后阶段的上下文信息和运动学信息的可靠性。我们还采用了双任务范式来研究任务负荷对熟练足球运动员在每个阶段整合信息和更新判断能力的影响。在不同的条件下,被试没有收到任何上下文信息(对照组),或收到依赖于或独立于动态上下文信息(对手定位)的稳定上下文信息(对手偏好)。正如预测的那样,参与者利用可靠的上下文和运动学信息来增强预测能力。进一步的探索性分析表明,任务负荷的增加会对预测准确性产生不利影响,但只有当可靠的上下文信息和运动学信息都可在最后阶段进行整合时才会产生这种影响。无论稳定的上下文信息是依赖于动态上下文信息还是独立于动态上下文信息,都会产生这种影响。研究结果表明,在一连串游戏的最后阶段,根据可靠的上下文信息和运动学信息的整合来更新预期判断需要认知资源。
{"title":"The effect of task load, information reliability and interdependency on anticipation performance.","authors":"Colm P Murphy, Oliver R Runswick, N Viktor Gredin, David P Broadbent","doi":"10.1186/s41235-024-00548-8","DOIUrl":"10.1186/s41235-024-00548-8","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>In sport, coaches often explicitly provide athletes with stable contextual information related to opponent action preferences to enhance anticipation performance. This information can be dependent on, or independent of, dynamic contextual information that only emerges during the sequence of play (e.g. opponent positioning). The interdependency between contextual information sources, and the associated cognitive demands of integrating information sources during anticipation, has not yet been systematically examined. We used a temporal occlusion paradigm to alter the reliability of contextual and kinematic information during the early, mid- and final phases of a two-versus-two soccer anticipation task. A dual-task paradigm was incorporated to investigate the impact of task load on skilled soccer players' ability to integrate information and update their judgements in each phase. Across conditions, participants received no contextual information (control) or stable contextual information (opponent preferences) that was dependent on, or independent of, dynamic contextual information (opponent positioning). As predicted, participants used reliable contextual and kinematic information to enhance anticipation. Further exploratory analysis suggested that increased task load detrimentally affected anticipation accuracy but only when both reliable contextual and kinematic information were available for integration in the final phase. This effect was observed irrespective of whether the stable contextual information was dependent on, or independent of, dynamic contextual information. Findings suggest that updating anticipatory judgements in the final phase of a sequence of play based on the integration of reliable contextual and kinematic information requires cognitive resources.</p>","PeriodicalId":46827,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Research-Principles and Implications","volume":"9 1","pages":"22"},"PeriodicalIF":4.1,"publicationDate":"2024-04-14","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11016527/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140867431","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
On investigating drivers' attention allocation during partially-automated driving. 研究驾驶员在部分自动驾驶过程中的注意力分配。
IF 4.1 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-04-10 DOI: 10.1186/s41235-024-00549-7
Reem Jalal Eddine, Claudio Mulatti, Francesco N Biondi

The use of partially-automated systems require drivers to supervise the system functioning and resume manual control whenever necessary. Yet literature on vehicle automation show that drivers may spend more time looking away from the road when the partially-automated system is operational. In this study we answer the question of whether this pattern is a manifestation of inattentional blindness or, more dangerously, it is also accompanied by a greater attentional processing of the driving scene. Participants drove a simulated vehicle in manual or partially-automated mode. Fixations were recorded by means of a head-mounted eye-tracker. A surprise two-alternative forced-choice recognition task was administered at the end of the data collection whereby participants were quizzed on the presence of roadside billboards that they encountered during the two drives. Data showed that participants were more likely to fixate and recognize billboards when the automated system was operational. Furthermore, whereas fixations toward billboards decreased toward the end of the automated drive, the performance in the recognition task did not suffer. Based on these findings, we hypothesize that the use of the partially-automated driving system may result in an increase in attention allocation toward peripheral objects in the road scene which is detrimental to the drivers' ability to supervise the automated system and resume manual control of the vehicle.

使用部分自动化系统需要驾驶员监督系统运行,并在必要时恢复手动控制。然而,有关车辆自动化的文献显示,当部分自动化系统运行时,驾驶员可能会花更多的时间将视线从道路上移开。在本研究中,我们要回答的问题是,这种模式是否是注意力不集中的盲目表现,或者更危险的是,它是否也伴随着对驾驶场景的更多注意力处理。参与者以手动或部分自动模式驾驶模拟车辆。通过头戴式眼动追踪器记录固定视线。在数据收集的最后阶段,还进行了一项突击性的两选一强迫选择识别任务,测试参与者在两次驾驶过程中所遇到的路边广告牌的存在情况。数据显示,当自动系统运行时,受试者更有可能固定并识别广告牌。此外,虽然在自动驾驶结束时,受试者对广告牌的固着率有所下降,但识别任务的成绩却没有受到影响。基于这些发现,我们推测,使用部分自动驾驶系统可能会导致驾驶员的注意力更多地分配到道路场景中的外围物体上,这不利于驾驶员监督自动驾驶系统和恢复手动控制车辆的能力。
{"title":"On investigating drivers' attention allocation during partially-automated driving.","authors":"Reem Jalal Eddine, Claudio Mulatti, Francesco N Biondi","doi":"10.1186/s41235-024-00549-7","DOIUrl":"10.1186/s41235-024-00549-7","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The use of partially-automated systems require drivers to supervise the system functioning and resume manual control whenever necessary. Yet literature on vehicle automation show that drivers may spend more time looking away from the road when the partially-automated system is operational. In this study we answer the question of whether this pattern is a manifestation of inattentional blindness or, more dangerously, it is also accompanied by a greater attentional processing of the driving scene. Participants drove a simulated vehicle in manual or partially-automated mode. Fixations were recorded by means of a head-mounted eye-tracker. A surprise two-alternative forced-choice recognition task was administered at the end of the data collection whereby participants were quizzed on the presence of roadside billboards that they encountered during the two drives. Data showed that participants were more likely to fixate and recognize billboards when the automated system was operational. Furthermore, whereas fixations toward billboards decreased toward the end of the automated drive, the performance in the recognition task did not suffer. Based on these findings, we hypothesize that the use of the partially-automated driving system may result in an increase in attention allocation toward peripheral objects in the road scene which is detrimental to the drivers' ability to supervise the automated system and resume manual control of the vehicle.</p>","PeriodicalId":46827,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Research-Principles and Implications","volume":"9 1","pages":"21"},"PeriodicalIF":4.1,"publicationDate":"2024-04-10","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11006638/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140871987","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Human and AI collaboration in the higher education environment: opportunities and concerns. 高等教育环境中的人类与人工智能合作:机遇与担忧。
IF 4.1 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-04-08 DOI: 10.1186/s41235-024-00547-9
Paul Atchley, Hannah Pannell, Kaelyn Wofford, Michael Hopkins, Ruth Ann Atchley

In service of the goal of examining how cognitive science can facilitate human-computer interactions in complex systems, we explore how cognitive psychology research might help educators better utilize artificial intelligence and AI supported tools as facilitatory to learning, rather than see these emerging technologies as a threat. We also aim to provide historical perspective, both on how automation and technology has generated unnecessary apprehension over time, and how generative AI technologies such as ChatGPT are a product of the discipline of cognitive science. We introduce a model for how higher education instruction can adapt to the age of AI by fully capitalizing on the role that metacognition knowledge and skills play in determining learning effectiveness. Finally, we urge educators to consider how AI can be seen as a critical collaborator to be utilized in our efforts to educate around the critical workforce skills of effective communication and collaboration.

为了研究认知科学如何促进复杂系统中的人机交互,我们探讨了认知心理学研究如何帮助教育工作者更好地利用人工智能和人工智能辅助工具来促进学习,而不是将这些新兴技术视为一种威胁。我们还旨在提供历史视角,即自动化和技术如何随着时间的推移产生不必要的忧虑,以及 ChatGPT 等生成性人工智能技术如何成为认知科学学科的产物。我们介绍了高等教育教学如何适应人工智能时代的模式,充分利用元认知知识和技能在决定学习效果方面的作用。最后,我们敦促教育工作者考虑如何将人工智能视为一个重要的合作者,在我们围绕有效沟通与协作的关键工作技能开展教育的过程中加以利用。
{"title":"Human and AI collaboration in the higher education environment: opportunities and concerns.","authors":"Paul Atchley, Hannah Pannell, Kaelyn Wofford, Michael Hopkins, Ruth Ann Atchley","doi":"10.1186/s41235-024-00547-9","DOIUrl":"10.1186/s41235-024-00547-9","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>In service of the goal of examining how cognitive science can facilitate human-computer interactions in complex systems, we explore how cognitive psychology research might help educators better utilize artificial intelligence and AI supported tools as facilitatory to learning, rather than see these emerging technologies as a threat. We also aim to provide historical perspective, both on how automation and technology has generated unnecessary apprehension over time, and how generative AI technologies such as ChatGPT are a product of the discipline of cognitive science. We introduce a model for how higher education instruction can adapt to the age of AI by fully capitalizing on the role that metacognition knowledge and skills play in determining learning effectiveness. Finally, we urge educators to consider how AI can be seen as a critical collaborator to be utilized in our efforts to educate around the critical workforce skills of effective communication and collaboration.</p>","PeriodicalId":46827,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Research-Principles and Implications","volume":"9 1","pages":"20"},"PeriodicalIF":4.1,"publicationDate":"2024-04-08","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11001814/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140852325","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Application of artificial intelligence to eyewitness identification. 人工智能在目击者识别中的应用。
IF 4.3 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-04-03 DOI: 10.1186/s41235-024-00542-0
Heather Kleider-Offutt, Beth Stevens, Laura Mickes, Stewart Boogert

Artificial intelligence is already all around us, and its usage will only increase. Knowing its capabilities is critical. A facial recognition system (FRS) is a tool for law enforcement during suspect searches and when presenting photos to eyewitnesses for identification. However, there are no comparisons between eyewitness and FRS accuracy using video, so it is unknown whether FRS face matches are more accurate than eyewitness memory when identifying a perpetrator. Ours is the first application of artificial intelligence to an eyewitness experience, using a comparative psychology approach. As a first step to test system accuracy relative to eyewitness accuracy, participants and an open-source FRS (FaceNet) attempted perpetrator identification/match from lineup photos (target-present, target-absent) after exposure to real crime videos with varied clarity and perpetrator race. FRS used video probe images of each perpetrator to achieve similarity ratings for each corresponding lineup member. Using receiver operating characteristic analysis to measure discriminability, FRS performance was superior to eyewitness performance, regardless of video clarity or perpetrator race. Video clarity impacted participant performance, with the unclear videos yielding lower performance than the clear videos. Using confidence-accuracy characteristic analysis to measure reliability (i.e., the likelihood the identified suspect is the actual perpetrator), when the FRS identified faces with the highest similarity values, they were accurate. The results suggest FaceNet, or similarly performing systems, may supplement eyewitness memory for suspect searches and subsequent lineup construction and knowing the system's strengths and weaknesses is critical.

人工智能已经在我们身边普及,而且其使用量只会越来越大。了解其功能至关重要。人脸识别系统(FRS)是执法部门在搜查嫌疑人和向目击者出示照片进行识别时的一种工具。然而,目前还没有使用视频对目击者和 FRS 的准确性进行比较,因此,在识别罪犯时,FRS 的人脸匹配是否比目击者的记忆更准确还不得而知。我们首次采用比较心理学方法,将人工智能应用于目击经验。作为测试系统相对于目击者准确性的第一步,参与者和开放源码的 FRS(FaceNet)在观看了不同清晰度和犯罪者种族的真实犯罪视频后,尝试根据列队照片(目标在,目标不在)进行犯罪者识别/匹配。FRS 使用每个犯罪者的视频探针图像来为每个相应的列队成员进行相似度评级。利用接收器操作特性分析来衡量辨别能力,无论视频清晰度或犯罪者种族如何,FRS 的表现都优于目击者的表现。视频清晰度影响参与者的表现,不清晰视频的表现低于清晰视频。利用置信度-准确度特性分析来衡量可靠性(即被识别的嫌疑人是真正作案者的可能性),当 FRS 识别出相似度值最高的人脸时,他们的识别是准确的。结果表明,FaceNet 或性能类似的系统可以在疑犯搜索和随后的列队构建中补充目击者记忆,了解系统的优缺点至关重要。
{"title":"Application of artificial intelligence to eyewitness identification.","authors":"Heather Kleider-Offutt, Beth Stevens, Laura Mickes, Stewart Boogert","doi":"10.1186/s41235-024-00542-0","DOIUrl":"10.1186/s41235-024-00542-0","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Artificial intelligence is already all around us, and its usage will only increase. Knowing its capabilities is critical. A facial recognition system (FRS) is a tool for law enforcement during suspect searches and when presenting photos to eyewitnesses for identification. However, there are no comparisons between eyewitness and FRS accuracy using video, so it is unknown whether FRS face matches are more accurate than eyewitness memory when identifying a perpetrator. Ours is the first application of artificial intelligence to an eyewitness experience, using a comparative psychology approach. As a first step to test system accuracy relative to eyewitness accuracy, participants and an open-source FRS (FaceNet) attempted perpetrator identification/match from lineup photos (target-present, target-absent) after exposure to real crime videos with varied clarity and perpetrator race. FRS used video probe images of each perpetrator to achieve similarity ratings for each corresponding lineup member. Using receiver operating characteristic analysis to measure discriminability, FRS performance was superior to eyewitness performance, regardless of video clarity or perpetrator race. Video clarity impacted participant performance, with the unclear videos yielding lower performance than the clear videos. Using confidence-accuracy characteristic analysis to measure reliability (i.e., the likelihood the identified suspect is the actual perpetrator), when the FRS identified faces with the highest similarity values, they were accurate. The results suggest FaceNet, or similarly performing systems, may supplement eyewitness memory for suspect searches and subsequent lineup construction and knowing the system's strengths and weaknesses is critical.</p>","PeriodicalId":46827,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Research-Principles and Implications","volume":"9 1","pages":"19"},"PeriodicalIF":4.3,"publicationDate":"2024-04-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10991253/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140858749","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Inattentional blindness in medicine. 医学中的 "注意力盲区"。
IF 4.1 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-03-27 DOI: 10.1186/s41235-024-00537-x
Connor M Hults, Yifan Ding, Geneva G Xie, Rishi Raja, William Johnson, Alexis Lee, Daniel J Simons

People often fail to notice unexpected stimuli when their attention is directed elsewhere. Most studies of this "inattentional blindness" have been conducted using laboratory tasks with little connection to real-world performance. Medical case reports document examples of missed findings in radiographs and CT images, unintentionally retained guidewires following surgery, and additional conditions being overlooked after making initial diagnoses. These cases suggest that inattentional blindness might contribute to medical errors, but relatively few studies have directly examined inattentional blindness in realistic medical contexts. We review the existing literature, much of which focuses on the use of augmented reality aids or inspection of medical images. Although these studies suggest a role for inattentional blindness in errors, most of the studies do not provide clear evidence that these errors result from inattentional blindness as opposed to other mechanisms. We discuss the design, analysis, and reporting practices that can make the contributions of inattentional blindness unclear, and we describe guidelines for future research in medicine and similar contexts that could provide clearer evidence for the role of inattentional blindness.

当人们的注意力被转移到其他地方时,往往无法注意到意想不到的刺激。对这种 "注意力不集中的盲目性 "的大多数研究都是通过实验室任务进行的,与现实世界的表现几乎没有联系。医学病例报告中记录了一些例子,如在射线照片和 CT 图像中遗漏了检查结果,手术后无意中保留了导丝,以及在做出初步诊断后忽略了其他病症。这些案例表明,注意力不集中可能会导致医疗失误,但在现实医疗环境中直接研究注意力不集中的研究相对较少。我们回顾了现有的文献,其中大部分集中于使用增强现实辅助工具或检查医学图像。虽然这些研究表明了注意力不集中导致的错误,但大多数研究并没有提供明确的证据证明这些错误是注意力不集中导致的,而不是其他机制造成的。我们讨论了设计、分析和报告实践,这些实践可能会使注意力不集中导致的误差不明确,我们还描述了未来医学和类似领域研究的指导方针,这些指导方针可以为注意力不集中导致的误差提供更明确的证据。
{"title":"Inattentional blindness in medicine.","authors":"Connor M Hults, Yifan Ding, Geneva G Xie, Rishi Raja, William Johnson, Alexis Lee, Daniel J Simons","doi":"10.1186/s41235-024-00537-x","DOIUrl":"10.1186/s41235-024-00537-x","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>People often fail to notice unexpected stimuli when their attention is directed elsewhere. Most studies of this \"inattentional blindness\" have been conducted using laboratory tasks with little connection to real-world performance. Medical case reports document examples of missed findings in radiographs and CT images, unintentionally retained guidewires following surgery, and additional conditions being overlooked after making initial diagnoses. These cases suggest that inattentional blindness might contribute to medical errors, but relatively few studies have directly examined inattentional blindness in realistic medical contexts. We review the existing literature, much of which focuses on the use of augmented reality aids or inspection of medical images. Although these studies suggest a role for inattentional blindness in errors, most of the studies do not provide clear evidence that these errors result from inattentional blindness as opposed to other mechanisms. We discuss the design, analysis, and reporting practices that can make the contributions of inattentional blindness unclear, and we describe guidelines for future research in medicine and similar contexts that could provide clearer evidence for the role of inattentional blindness.</p>","PeriodicalId":46827,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Research-Principles and Implications","volume":"9 1","pages":"18"},"PeriodicalIF":4.1,"publicationDate":"2024-03-27","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10973299/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140307381","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Toward viewing behavior for aerial scene categorization. 空中场景分类的观看行为。
IF 4.1 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-03-26 DOI: 10.1186/s41235-024-00541-1
Chenxi Jiang, Zhenzhong Chen, Jeremy M Wolfe

Previous work has demonstrated similarities and differences between aerial and terrestrial image viewing. Aerial scene categorization, a pivotal visual processing task for gathering geoinformation, heavily depends on rotation-invariant information. Aerial image-centered research has revealed effects of low-level features on performance of various aerial image interpretation tasks. However, there are fewer studies of viewing behavior for aerial scene categorization and of higher-level factors that might influence that categorization. In this paper, experienced subjects' eye movements were recorded while they were asked to categorize aerial scenes. A typical viewing center bias was observed. Eye movement patterns varied among categories. We explored the relationship of nine image statistics to observers' eye movements. Results showed that if the images were less homogeneous, and/or if they contained fewer or no salient diagnostic objects, viewing behavior became more exploratory. Higher- and object-level image statistics were predictive at both the image and scene category levels. Scanpaths were generally organized and small differences in scanpath randomness could be roughly captured by critical object saliency. Participants tended to fixate on critical objects. Image statistics included in this study showed rotational invariance. The results supported our hypothesis that the availability of diagnostic objects strongly influences eye movements in this task. In addition, this study provides supporting evidence for Loschky et al.'s (Journal of Vision, 15(6), 11, 2015) speculation that aerial scenes are categorized on the basis of image parts and individual objects. The findings were discussed in relation to theories of scene perception and their implications for automation development.

以往的研究已经证明了航空和地面图像观看的异同。航空场景分类是收集地理信息的一项重要视觉处理任务,在很大程度上依赖于旋转不变信息。以航空图像为中心的研究揭示了低层次特征对各种航空图像解读任务的影响。然而,有关航空场景分类的观看行为以及可能影响分类的高层次因素的研究却较少。本文记录了经验丰富的受试者在被要求对空中场景进行分类时的眼球运动。观察到典型的观看中心偏差。不同类别的眼动模式各不相同。我们探讨了九种图像统计数据与观察者眼动之间的关系。结果显示,如果图像的同质性较低,和/或如果图像中包含的突出诊断对象较少或没有,观察者的观察行为就会变得更具探索性。在图像和场景类别两个层面上,较高级别的图像统计和对象统计都具有预测性。扫描路径一般是有组织的,扫描路径随机性的微小差异可以通过临界物体显著性大致捕捉到。参与者倾向于固定在关键物体上。本研究中的图像统计显示了旋转不变性。研究结果支持了我们的假设,即在这项任务中,诊断对象的可用性对眼球运动有很大影响。此外,本研究还为 Loschky 等人(《视觉杂志》,15(6), 11, 2015 年)的推测提供了支持性证据,即空中场景是根据图像部分和单个物体进行分类的。研究结果与场景感知理论及其对自动化发展的影响进行了讨论。
{"title":"Toward viewing behavior for aerial scene categorization.","authors":"Chenxi Jiang, Zhenzhong Chen, Jeremy M Wolfe","doi":"10.1186/s41235-024-00541-1","DOIUrl":"10.1186/s41235-024-00541-1","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Previous work has demonstrated similarities and differences between aerial and terrestrial image viewing. Aerial scene categorization, a pivotal visual processing task for gathering geoinformation, heavily depends on rotation-invariant information. Aerial image-centered research has revealed effects of low-level features on performance of various aerial image interpretation tasks. However, there are fewer studies of viewing behavior for aerial scene categorization and of higher-level factors that might influence that categorization. In this paper, experienced subjects' eye movements were recorded while they were asked to categorize aerial scenes. A typical viewing center bias was observed. Eye movement patterns varied among categories. We explored the relationship of nine image statistics to observers' eye movements. Results showed that if the images were less homogeneous, and/or if they contained fewer or no salient diagnostic objects, viewing behavior became more exploratory. Higher- and object-level image statistics were predictive at both the image and scene category levels. Scanpaths were generally organized and small differences in scanpath randomness could be roughly captured by critical object saliency. Participants tended to fixate on critical objects. Image statistics included in this study showed rotational invariance. The results supported our hypothesis that the availability of diagnostic objects strongly influences eye movements in this task. In addition, this study provides supporting evidence for Loschky et al.'s (Journal of Vision, 15(6), 11, 2015) speculation that aerial scenes are categorized on the basis of image parts and individual objects. The findings were discussed in relation to theories of scene perception and their implications for automation development.</p>","PeriodicalId":46827,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Research-Principles and Implications","volume":"9 1","pages":"17"},"PeriodicalIF":4.1,"publicationDate":"2024-03-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10965882/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140294944","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Errors in visual search: Are they stochastic or deterministic? 视觉搜索中的错误:它们是随机的还是确定的?
IF 4.1 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-03-19 DOI: 10.1186/s41235-024-00543-z
Aoqi Li, Johan Hulleman, Jeremy M Wolfe

In any visual search task in the lab or in the world, observers will make errors. Those errors can be categorized as "deterministic": If you miss this target in this display once, you will definitely miss it again. Alternatively, errors can be "stochastic", occurring randomly with some probability from trial to trial. Researchers and practitioners have sought to reduce errors in visual search, but different types of errors might require different techniques for mitigation. To empirically categorize errors in a simple search task, our observers searched for the letter "T" among "L" distractors, with each display presented twice. When the letters were clearly visible (white letters on a gray background), the errors were almost completely stochastic (Exp 1). An error made on the first appearance of a display did not predict that an error would be made on the second appearance. When the visibility of the letters was manipulated (letters of different gray levels on a noisy background), the errors became a mix of stochastic and deterministic. Unsurprisingly, lower contrast targets produced more deterministic errors. (Exp 2). Using the stimuli of Exp 2, we tested whether errors could be reduced using cues that guided attention around the display but knew nothing about the content of that display (Exp3a, b). This had no effect, but cueing all item locations did succeed in reducing deterministic errors (Exp3c).

无论是在实验室还是在世界上的任何视觉搜索任务中,观察者都会犯错误。这些错误可归类为 "确定性 "错误:如果你错过了这个显示屏中的这个目标一次,那么你肯定会再次错过。另外,错误也可以是 "随机的",即在每次试验中都以某种概率随机出现。研究人员和从业人员一直在努力减少视觉搜索中的误差,但不同类型的误差可能需要不同的缓解技术。为了对简单搜索任务中的错误进行实证分类,我们的观察者在 "L "干扰物中搜索字母 "T",每个干扰物显示两次。当字母清晰可见时(灰底白字),错误几乎完全是随机的(实验 1)。第一次出现错误并不能预示第二次会出现错误。当字母的可视性受到影响时(在嘈杂背景上显示不同灰度的字母),错误就变成了随机和确定性的混合。毫不奇怪,对比度较低的目标会产生更多的确定性错误。(实验 2)。使用实验 2 的刺激物,我们测试了使用围绕显示屏引导注意力但对显示内容一无所知的线索是否可以减少错误(实验 3a、b)。这种方法没有任何效果,但提示所有物品的位置却能成功减少确定性错误(Exp3c)。
{"title":"Errors in visual search: Are they stochastic or deterministic?","authors":"Aoqi Li, Johan Hulleman, Jeremy M Wolfe","doi":"10.1186/s41235-024-00543-z","DOIUrl":"10.1186/s41235-024-00543-z","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>In any visual search task in the lab or in the world, observers will make errors. Those errors can be categorized as \"deterministic\": If you miss this target in this display once, you will definitely miss it again. Alternatively, errors can be \"stochastic\", occurring randomly with some probability from trial to trial. Researchers and practitioners have sought to reduce errors in visual search, but different types of errors might require different techniques for mitigation. To empirically categorize errors in a simple search task, our observers searched for the letter \"T\" among \"L\" distractors, with each display presented twice. When the letters were clearly visible (white letters on a gray background), the errors were almost completely stochastic (Exp 1). An error made on the first appearance of a display did not predict that an error would be made on the second appearance. When the visibility of the letters was manipulated (letters of different gray levels on a noisy background), the errors became a mix of stochastic and deterministic. Unsurprisingly, lower contrast targets produced more deterministic errors. (Exp 2). Using the stimuli of Exp 2, we tested whether errors could be reduced using cues that guided attention around the display but knew nothing about the content of that display (Exp3a, b). This had no effect, but cueing all item locations did succeed in reducing deterministic errors (Exp3c).</p>","PeriodicalId":46827,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Research-Principles and Implications","volume":"9 1","pages":"15"},"PeriodicalIF":4.1,"publicationDate":"2024-03-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10951178/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140177158","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The effect of fingerprint expertise on visual short-term memory. 指纹专业知识对视觉短时记忆的影响
IF 4.1 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-03-19 DOI: 10.1186/s41235-024-00539-9
Brooklyn J Corbett, Jason M Tangen, Rachel A Searston, Matthew B Thompson

Expert fingerprint examiners demonstrate impressive feats of memory that may support their accuracy when making high-stakes identification decisions. Understanding the interplay between expertise and memory is therefore critical. Across two experiments, we tested fingerprint examiners and novices on their visual short-term memory for fingerprints. In Experiment 1, experts showed substantially higher memory performance compared to novices for fingerprints from their domain of expertise. In Experiment 2, we manipulated print distinctiveness and found that while both groups benefited from distinctive prints, experts still outperformed novices. This indicates that beyond stimulus qualities, expertise itself enhances short-term memory, likely through more effective organisational processing and sensitivity to meaningful patterns. Taken together, these findings shed light on the cognitive mechanisms that may explain fingerprint examiners' superior memory performance within their domain of expertise. They further suggest that training to improve memory for diverse fingerprints could practically boost examiner performance. Given the high-stakes nature of forensic identification, characterising psychological processes like memory that potentially contribute to examiner accuracy has important theoretical and practical implications.

指纹鉴定专家的记忆力令人印象深刻,这可能有助于他们在做出高风险鉴定决定时保持准确性。因此,了解专业知识与记忆之间的相互作用至关重要。在两项实验中,我们测试了指纹鉴定专家和新手对指纹的视觉短时记忆能力。在实验 1 中,与新手相比,专家对其专业领域内的指纹表现出更高的记忆能力。在实验 2 中,我们对指纹的独特性进行了处理,结果发现虽然两组人都能从独特的指纹中获益,但专家的表现仍然优于新手。这表明,除了刺激质量外,专业知识本身也能增强短时记忆,这可能是通过更有效的组织处理和对有意义模式的敏感性实现的。综上所述,这些研究结果揭示了指纹检验员在其专业领域内具有超强记忆力的认知机制。这些研究结果还表明,通过培训提高对不同指纹的记忆能力可以切实提高检验员的工作表现。鉴于法医鉴定的高风险性质,描述可能有助于检验员准确性的记忆等心理过程具有重要的理论和实践意义。
{"title":"The effect of fingerprint expertise on visual short-term memory.","authors":"Brooklyn J Corbett, Jason M Tangen, Rachel A Searston, Matthew B Thompson","doi":"10.1186/s41235-024-00539-9","DOIUrl":"10.1186/s41235-024-00539-9","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Expert fingerprint examiners demonstrate impressive feats of memory that may support their accuracy when making high-stakes identification decisions. Understanding the interplay between expertise and memory is therefore critical. Across two experiments, we tested fingerprint examiners and novices on their visual short-term memory for fingerprints. In Experiment 1, experts showed substantially higher memory performance compared to novices for fingerprints from their domain of expertise. In Experiment 2, we manipulated print distinctiveness and found that while both groups benefited from distinctive prints, experts still outperformed novices. This indicates that beyond stimulus qualities, expertise itself enhances short-term memory, likely through more effective organisational processing and sensitivity to meaningful patterns. Taken together, these findings shed light on the cognitive mechanisms that may explain fingerprint examiners' superior memory performance within their domain of expertise. They further suggest that training to improve memory for diverse fingerprints could practically boost examiner performance. Given the high-stakes nature of forensic identification, characterising psychological processes like memory that potentially contribute to examiner accuracy has important theoretical and practical implications.</p>","PeriodicalId":46827,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Research-Principles and Implications","volume":"9 1","pages":"14"},"PeriodicalIF":4.1,"publicationDate":"2024-03-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10951190/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140177160","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
How is GPS used? Understanding navigation system use and its relation to spatial ability. 如何使用 GPS?了解导航系统的使用及其与空间能力的关系。
IF 4.1 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-03-19 DOI: 10.1186/s41235-024-00545-x
Alexis Topete, Chuanxiuyue He, John Protzko, Jonathan Schooler, Mary Hegarty

Given how commonly GPS is now used in everyday navigation, it is surprising how little research has been dedicated to investigating variations in its use and how such variations may relate to navigation ability. The present study investigated general GPS dependence, how people report using GPS in various navigational scenarios, and the relationship between these measures and spatial abilities (assessed by self-report measures and the ability to learn the layout of a novel environment). GPS dependence is an individual's perceived need to use GPS in navigation, and GPS usage is the frequency with which they report using different functions of GPS. The study also assessed whether people modulate reported use of GPS as a function of their familiarity with the location in which they are navigating. In 249 participants over two preregistered studies, reported GPS dependence was negatively correlated with objective navigation performance and self-reported sense of direction, and positively correlated with spatial anxiety. Greater reported use of GPS for turn-by-turn directions was associated with a poorer sense of direction and higher spatial anxiety. People reported using GPS most frequently for time and traffic estimation, regardless of ability. Finally, people reported using GPS less, regardless of ability, when they were more familiar with an environment. Collectively these findings suggest that people moderate their use of GPS, depending on their knowledge, ability, and confidence in their own abilities, and often report using GPS to augment rather than replace spatial environmental knowledge.

鉴于全球定位系统目前在日常导航中的使用如此普遍,令人惊讶的是,专门用于调查其使用差异以及这些差异与导航能力之间关系的研究却少之又少。本研究调查了人们对全球定位系统的一般依赖性、人们如何报告在各种导航场景中使用全球定位系统,以及这些措施与空间能力(通过自我报告措施和学习新环境布局的能力来评估)之间的关系。GPS依赖性是指个人认为在导航中需要使用GPS,GPS使用率是指个人报告使用GPS不同功能的频率。该研究还评估了人们是否会根据对导航地点的熟悉程度来调节所报告的 GPS 使用情况。在两次预先登记的 249 名参与者中,报告的 GPS 依赖性与客观导航性能和自我报告的方向感呈负相关,与空间焦虑呈正相关。据报告,更多地使用 GPS 进行转弯导航与方向感较差和空间焦虑较高有关。无论能力如何,人们使用 GPS 最多的目的是估算时间和交通流量。最后,当人们对环境更加熟悉时,无论能力如何,使用 GPS 的频率都会降低。总之,这些研究结果表明,人们会根据自己的知识、能力和对自己能力的信心来适度使用 GPS,而且他们经常说 GPS 是用来增强而不是取代空间环境知识的。
{"title":"How is GPS used? Understanding navigation system use and its relation to spatial ability.","authors":"Alexis Topete, Chuanxiuyue He, John Protzko, Jonathan Schooler, Mary Hegarty","doi":"10.1186/s41235-024-00545-x","DOIUrl":"10.1186/s41235-024-00545-x","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Given how commonly GPS is now used in everyday navigation, it is surprising how little research has been dedicated to investigating variations in its use and how such variations may relate to navigation ability. The present study investigated general GPS dependence, how people report using GPS in various navigational scenarios, and the relationship between these measures and spatial abilities (assessed by self-report measures and the ability to learn the layout of a novel environment). GPS dependence is an individual's perceived need to use GPS in navigation, and GPS usage is the frequency with which they report using different functions of GPS. The study also assessed whether people modulate reported use of GPS as a function of their familiarity with the location in which they are navigating. In 249 participants over two preregistered studies, reported GPS dependence was negatively correlated with objective navigation performance and self-reported sense of direction, and positively correlated with spatial anxiety. Greater reported use of GPS for turn-by-turn directions was associated with a poorer sense of direction and higher spatial anxiety. People reported using GPS most frequently for time and traffic estimation, regardless of ability. Finally, people reported using GPS less, regardless of ability, when they were more familiar with an environment. Collectively these findings suggest that people moderate their use of GPS, depending on their knowledge, ability, and confidence in their own abilities, and often report using GPS to augment rather than replace spatial environmental knowledge.</p>","PeriodicalId":46827,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Research-Principles and Implications","volume":"9 1","pages":"16"},"PeriodicalIF":4.1,"publicationDate":"2024-03-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10951145/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140177159","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Cognitive Research-Principles and Implications
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1