首页 > 最新文献

Journal of Linguistics最新文献

英文 中文
LIN volume 58 issue 4 Cover and Back matter LIN第58卷第4期封面和封底
IF 1.1 2区 文学 0 LANGUAGE & LINGUISTICS Pub Date : 2022-10-12 DOI: 10.1017/s002222672200041x
{"title":"LIN volume 58 issue 4 Cover and Back matter","authors":"","doi":"10.1017/s002222672200041x","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1017/s002222672200041x","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":47027,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Linguistics","volume":"58 1","pages":"b1 - b2"},"PeriodicalIF":1.1,"publicationDate":"2022-10-12","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41628633","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
LIN volume 58 issue 4 Cover and Front matter LIN第58卷第4期封面和封面
IF 1.1 2区 文学 0 LANGUAGE & LINGUISTICS Pub Date : 2022-10-12 DOI: 10.1017/s0022226722000408
{"title":"LIN volume 58 issue 4 Cover and Front matter","authors":"","doi":"10.1017/s0022226722000408","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1017/s0022226722000408","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":47027,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Linguistics","volume":" ","pages":"f1 - f2"},"PeriodicalIF":1.1,"publicationDate":"2022-10-12","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"49344790","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Maria Polinsky (ed.), The Oxford Handbook of Languages of the Caucasus (Oxford Handbooks). Oxford: Oxford University Press, 2020. Pp. xxx + 1158. 玛丽亚·波林斯基(编),高加索语言的牛津手册(牛津手册)。牛津:牛津大学出版社,2020。Pp. xxx + 1158。
IF 1.1 2区 文学 0 LANGUAGE & LINGUISTICS Pub Date : 2022-09-14 DOI: 10.1017/S002222672200038X
Anaïd Donabédian
This volume is intended to be a reference manual on the linguistics of the Caucasian languages, targeted at researchers, students, and the initiated public. The general outline of the volume, the choice of authors, and the quality of their contributions greatly contribute to achieving this ambition. The book is well-crafted and thoroughly edited byMaria Polinsky. Its chapters are designed to make a whole, with a coherent and effective plan, a unique consolidated list of references at the end of the volume, and a 68-page index (which is, however, far from satisfactory, in terms of relevance and accuracy). It successfully fulfills the dual mission of being an effective handbook and a milestone in scientific research in its field, providing stimulating reading for scholars in typological, areal, and theoretical terms. The chapters are based on a substantial body of data, compiled in grammars or collected first-hand by the authors. Most of the 28 contributors are experts in North-West Caucasian languages (hereafter NWC), Nakh-Daghestanian languages (hereafter ND), or Kartvelian, sometimes the leading authors for one or more specific language. Yuri Koryakov, a recognized expert in linguistic cartography of the region, provides four maps representing administrative divisions, language families, languages, and one specific map detailing the ND languages. No contributor has specific expertise in Armenian or Azerbaijani. The book has six parts. The first part, together with the general introduction from the editor, provides a comprehensive linguistic, sociolinguistic, and demographic overview of the region (1–86). The following four parts (12 chapters of varying granularity in terms of languages covered) contain a sociolinguistic, linguistic, and typological description for each main family: ND (87–368), NWC (369–490), Kartvelian (491–572), and Indo-European (IE) (573–688). The last part, entitled ‘Phenomena’, cross-linguistically addresses ten descriptive and theoretical issues (689–1002). All references are compiled in a single bibliography section. The book is completed by two appendices ‘Languages and language names’ and ‘Transliteration tables’ by Yuri Koryakov as well as an Index (1091–1158). In her general introduction, Maria Polinsky outlines the scope of the book, namely non-extinct languages of the Caucasus. She presents the languages and linguistic families of the region, its linguistic history, sociolinguistic and typological challenges, the state of research, and the structure of the handbook. Thewidefield covered makes a few factual errors or problematic formulations unavoidable, but this
本卷的目的是对高加索语言的语言学的参考手册,针对研究人员,学生和发起的公众。卷的总体轮廓,作者的选择,和他们的贡献的质量大大有助于实现这一目标。这本书是由玛丽亚·波林斯基精心设计和彻底编辑的。它的章节被设计成一个整体,有一个连贯而有效的计划,在卷的末尾有一个独特的参考文献综合列表,以及一个68页的索引(然而,就相关性和准确性而言,这远远不能令人满意)。它成功地完成了作为一本有效的手册和该领域科学研究的里程碑的双重使命,为学者提供了类型,区域和理论方面的刺激阅读。这些章节是基于大量的数据,在语法编译或收集作者的第一手资料。28位贡献者中的大多数是西北高加索语言(以下简称NWC),纳卡-达吉斯坦语言(以下简称ND)或卡特维利亚语的专家,有时是一种或多种特定语言的主要作者。Yuri Koryakov是该地区公认的语言制图专家,他提供了四张代表行政区划、语族、语言的地图,以及一张详细描述北高加索地区语言的具体地图。没有任何捐助者具有亚美尼亚语或阿塞拜疆语的专门知识。这本书有六个部分。第一部分,连同编辑的一般介绍,提供了该地区全面的语言学、社会语言学和人口统计学概述(1-86)。以下四个部分(根据所涵盖语言的不同粒度的12章)包含每个主要家庭的社会语言学,语言学和类型描述:ND (87-368), NWC (369-490), Kartvelian(491-572)和印欧语系(IE)(573-688)。最后一部分,题为“现象”,跨语言解决十个描述性和理论问题(689-1002)。所有参考文献汇编在一个单独的参考书目部分。这本书是由两个附录“语言和语言名称”和“音译表”由尤里Koryakov以及索引(1091-1158)完成。在她的总介绍中,玛丽亚·波林斯基概述了这本书的范围,即高加索地区未灭绝的语言。她介绍了该地区的语言和语族,其语言历史,社会语言学和类型学挑战,研究状况以及手册的结构。所涵盖的广泛领域使一些事实错误或有问题的表述不可避免,但这
{"title":"Maria Polinsky (ed.), The Oxford Handbook of Languages of the Caucasus (Oxford Handbooks). Oxford: Oxford University Press, 2020. Pp. xxx + 1158.","authors":"Anaïd Donabédian","doi":"10.1017/S002222672200038X","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1017/S002222672200038X","url":null,"abstract":"This volume is intended to be a reference manual on the linguistics of the Caucasian languages, targeted at researchers, students, and the initiated public. The general outline of the volume, the choice of authors, and the quality of their contributions greatly contribute to achieving this ambition. The book is well-crafted and thoroughly edited byMaria Polinsky. Its chapters are designed to make a whole, with a coherent and effective plan, a unique consolidated list of references at the end of the volume, and a 68-page index (which is, however, far from satisfactory, in terms of relevance and accuracy). It successfully fulfills the dual mission of being an effective handbook and a milestone in scientific research in its field, providing stimulating reading for scholars in typological, areal, and theoretical terms. The chapters are based on a substantial body of data, compiled in grammars or collected first-hand by the authors. Most of the 28 contributors are experts in North-West Caucasian languages (hereafter NWC), Nakh-Daghestanian languages (hereafter ND), or Kartvelian, sometimes the leading authors for one or more specific language. Yuri Koryakov, a recognized expert in linguistic cartography of the region, provides four maps representing administrative divisions, language families, languages, and one specific map detailing the ND languages. No contributor has specific expertise in Armenian or Azerbaijani. The book has six parts. The first part, together with the general introduction from the editor, provides a comprehensive linguistic, sociolinguistic, and demographic overview of the region (1–86). The following four parts (12 chapters of varying granularity in terms of languages covered) contain a sociolinguistic, linguistic, and typological description for each main family: ND (87–368), NWC (369–490), Kartvelian (491–572), and Indo-European (IE) (573–688). The last part, entitled ‘Phenomena’, cross-linguistically addresses ten descriptive and theoretical issues (689–1002). All references are compiled in a single bibliography section. The book is completed by two appendices ‘Languages and language names’ and ‘Transliteration tables’ by Yuri Koryakov as well as an Index (1091–1158). In her general introduction, Maria Polinsky outlines the scope of the book, namely non-extinct languages of the Caucasus. She presents the languages and linguistic families of the region, its linguistic history, sociolinguistic and typological challenges, the state of research, and the structure of the handbook. Thewidefield covered makes a few factual errors or problematic formulations unavoidable, but this","PeriodicalId":47027,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Linguistics","volume":"58 1","pages":"916 - 921"},"PeriodicalIF":1.1,"publicationDate":"2022-09-14","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"45732891","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Case and agreement as contextually manipulable properties of functional heads 情况和一致作为功能头的上下文可操作属性
IF 1.1 2区 文学 0 LANGUAGE & LINGUISTICS Pub Date : 2022-09-09 DOI: 10.1017/S0022226722000329
M. Tyler
Some recent work has argued that agreement and case-assignment dependencies between a functional head and a nearby NP are not part of the syntactic derivation proper, but take place in the postsyntactic, morphological component of the grammar. I argue that this view is correct, by showing that one of its largely unexplored predictions has real empirical payout. The prediction is that the dependency-forming properties of functional heads, being morphological in nature, are mutable, and may be conditioned by nearby roots and functional structure. I focus here on Voice heads in Choctaw, and my starting assumption is that, by default, $ {mathrm{Voice}}_{left[+mathrm{N}right]} $ (the Voice head which introduces a specifier) agrees with its specifier (the external argument) and $ {mathrm{Voice}}_{left[-mathrm{N}right]} $ (i.e. specifier-less Voice, found in unaccusatives) does not agree with anything. However, I propose that in some environments, $ {mathrm{Voice}}_{left[-mathrm{N}right]} $ does launch a $ phi $ -probe, and it results in $ {mathrm{Voice}}_{left[-mathrm{N}right]} $ agreeing with the internal argument. I refer to these configurations as ‘low ergatives’. A small survey of previous work on case and agreement dependencies suggests (a) that the case-assignment properties of functional heads are mutable in the same way, and (b) that the reverse is attested – in some environments $ {mathrm{Voice}}_{left[+mathrm{N}right]} $ fails to launch a $ phi $ -probe. This is consistent with a purely morphological model of agreement and case-assignment: just as the exponence and interpretation of functional heads can be conditioned by adjacent roots and functional material, so too can the dependency-forming properties of those heads be conditioned in the same way.
最近的一些研究认为,功能词头和附近的NP之间的一致和格配依赖关系不是句法派生的一部分,而是发生在语法的后句法、形态成分中。我认为,这种观点是正确的,因为它表明,其中一个基本上未经探索的预测具有实际的实证回报。预测功能头的依赖形成特性本质上是形态学的,是可变的,可能受到附近根和功能结构的制约。我在这里关注的是Choctaw中的Voice头,我的假设是,默认情况下,$ {mathrm{Voice}}_{left[+mathrm{N}right]} $(引入说明符的Voice头)与其说明符(外部参数)一致,而$ {mathrm{Voice}}_{left[-mathrm{N}right]} $(即,在非宾格中发现的无说明符的Voice)不同意任何东西。然而,我建议在某些环境中,$ {mathrm{Voice}}_{left[-mathrm{N}right]} $确实启动了$ phi $ -探测,并且它的结果是$ {mathrm{Voice}}_{left[-mathrm{N}right]} $与内部参数一致。我把这些配置称为“低负性”。对先前关于案例和协议依赖关系的工作进行的一项小型调查表明(A)功能头的案例分配属性以同样的方式是可变的,并且(b)证明了相反的情况-在某些环境中$ {mathrm{Voice}}_{left[+mathrm{N}right]} $未能启动$ phi $ -探针。这与纯粹形态学的一致性和案例分配模型是一致的:正如功能头部的指数和解释可以由相邻的根和功能材料来决定一样,这些头部的依赖形成特性也可以以同样的方式来决定。
{"title":"Case and agreement as contextually manipulable properties of functional heads","authors":"M. Tyler","doi":"10.1017/S0022226722000329","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1017/S0022226722000329","url":null,"abstract":"Some recent work has argued that agreement and case-assignment dependencies between a functional head and a nearby NP are not part of the syntactic derivation proper, but take place in the postsyntactic, morphological component of the grammar. I argue that this view is correct, by showing that one of its largely unexplored predictions has real empirical payout. The prediction is that the dependency-forming properties of functional heads, being morphological in nature, are mutable, and may be conditioned by nearby roots and functional structure. I focus here on Voice heads in Choctaw, and my starting assumption is that, by default, $ {mathrm{Voice}}_{left[+mathrm{N}right]} $ (the Voice head which introduces a specifier) agrees with its specifier (the external argument) and $ {mathrm{Voice}}_{left[-mathrm{N}right]} $ (i.e. specifier-less Voice, found in unaccusatives) does not agree with anything. However, I propose that in some environments, $ {mathrm{Voice}}_{left[-mathrm{N}right]} $ does launch a $ phi $ -probe, and it results in $ {mathrm{Voice}}_{left[-mathrm{N}right]} $ agreeing with the internal argument. I refer to these configurations as ‘low ergatives’. A small survey of previous work on case and agreement dependencies suggests (a) that the case-assignment properties of functional heads are mutable in the same way, and (b) that the reverse is attested – in some environments $ {mathrm{Voice}}_{left[+mathrm{N}right]} $ fails to launch a $ phi $ -probe. This is consistent with a purely morphological model of agreement and case-assignment: just as the exponence and interpretation of functional heads can be conditioned by adjacent roots and functional material, so too can the dependency-forming properties of those heads be conditioned in the same way.","PeriodicalId":47027,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Linguistics","volume":"59 1","pages":"831 - 879"},"PeriodicalIF":1.1,"publicationDate":"2022-09-09","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"44429280","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Filler–gap dependencies and the remnant–correlate dependency in backward sprouting: Sensitivity to distance and islands 反向发芽中的补隙依赖关系和残余相关依赖关系:对距离和岛屿的敏感性
IF 1.1 2区 文学 0 LANGUAGE & LINGUISTICS Pub Date : 2022-08-30 DOI: 10.1017/S0022226722000366
DUK-HO Jung, G. Goodall
The relationship between the wh-remnant and the null correlate in the type of ellipsis known as backward sprouting is superficially almost identical to the relation between a wh-filler and a gap in a wh-question. In both cases, there is a dependency between the wh-phrase and a later null element. We conduct a sentence acceptability experiment to test whether the remnant–correlate dependency in backward sprouting exhibits two well-known properties of a filler–gap dependency in wh-questions: sensitivity to clause boundaries (distance) and sensitivity to islands. The results show that both dependency types are sensitive to clause boundaries, although the effect is larger in the case of filler–gap dependencies, but that only filler–gap dependencies are sensitive to islands. These results present a challenge to analyses of sprouting in which the ellipsis site contains a full representation of the structure of the antecedent clause, since such analyses predict island-sensitivity for remnant–correlate dependencies. The results also suggest that island-sensitivity cannot be reduced to simple processing demands without regard to the syntactic representation of the dependency, since such a view would predict greater similarity between filler–gap dependencies and remnant–correlate dependencies than is found.
在被称为向后发芽的省略号类型中,残余的wh和零相关之间的关系表面上几乎与wh问题中填充的wh和空白之间的关系相同。在这两种情况下,在wh-phrase和后面的null元素之间存在依赖关系。我们进行了一个句子可接受性实验,以测试后向生长中的残余相关依赖是否表现出whh疑问句中填充-间隙依赖的两个众所周知的特性:对子句边界(距离)的敏感性和对岛屿的敏感性。结果表明,两种依赖类型都对子句边界敏感,但填充-间隙依赖对子句边界的影响更大,而只有填充-间隙依赖对岛屿敏感。这些结果对发芽分析提出了挑战,其中省略位点包含先行句结构的完整表示,因为这种分析预测了残余相关依赖关系的岛屿敏感性。结果还表明,孤岛敏感性不能在不考虑依赖关系的语法表示的情况下简化为简单的处理需求,因为这样的视图将预测填充间隙依赖关系和剩余相关依赖关系之间的相似性比发现的更大。
{"title":"Filler–gap dependencies and the remnant–correlate dependency in backward sprouting: Sensitivity to distance and islands","authors":"DUK-HO Jung, G. Goodall","doi":"10.1017/S0022226722000366","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1017/S0022226722000366","url":null,"abstract":"The relationship between the wh-remnant and the null correlate in the type of ellipsis known as backward sprouting is superficially almost identical to the relation between a wh-filler and a gap in a wh-question. In both cases, there is a dependency between the wh-phrase and a later null element. We conduct a sentence acceptability experiment to test whether the remnant–correlate dependency in backward sprouting exhibits two well-known properties of a filler–gap dependency in wh-questions: sensitivity to clause boundaries (distance) and sensitivity to islands. The results show that both dependency types are sensitive to clause boundaries, although the effect is larger in the case of filler–gap dependencies, but that only filler–gap dependencies are sensitive to islands. These results present a challenge to analyses of sprouting in which the ellipsis site contains a full representation of the structure of the antecedent clause, since such analyses predict island-sensitivity for remnant–correlate dependencies. The results also suggest that island-sensitivity cannot be reduced to simple processing demands without regard to the syntactic representation of the dependency, since such a view would predict greater similarity between filler–gap dependencies and remnant–correlate dependencies than is found.","PeriodicalId":47027,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Linguistics","volume":"59 1","pages":"235 - 255"},"PeriodicalIF":1.1,"publicationDate":"2022-08-30","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"46594716","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
David R. Olson ,Making sense: What it means to understand. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 2022. Pp. xii +196. 大卫·r·奥尔森:《创造意义:理解意味着什么》。剑桥:剑桥大学出版社,2022。Pp. xii +196
IF 1.1 2区 文学 0 LANGUAGE & LINGUISTICS Pub Date : 2022-08-30 DOI: 10.1017/S0022226722000378
Norbert Francis
program as a whole may indeed have been (at best) premature’ (9). Yet ‘talk of optimization’ is very much still at the heart of minimalist thinking. ROLE provides no reason why minimalist discussions of optimized computational machinery are incompatible with biology. Conversely, nor are we told why Boeckx’s gradualist account of the evolution of syntax can have no place for such concerns of computational efficiency. We are also given no concrete rebuttal of earlier ideas espoused by Boeckx. There is a clear discontinuity between Boeckx’s earlier writings and his current position in ROLE, but little clarity with respect to which pieces we are supposed to pick up, and which pieces we are supposed to leave behind.
程序作为一个整体可能确实(充其量)是不成熟的”(9)。然而,“谈论优化”仍然是极简主义思想的核心。ROLE没有解释为什么最优化计算机制的极简讨论与生物学不相容。相反,我们也没有被告知,为什么Boeckx关于语法进化的渐进主义解释不能考虑计算效率。我们也没有得到对博克克斯早期观点的具体反驳。在Boeckx早期的作品和他目前在ROLE中的地位之间有一个明显的间断,但关于我们应该捡起哪些碎片,哪些碎片我们应该留下,这一点很少清晰。
{"title":"David R. Olson ,Making sense: What it means to understand. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 2022. Pp. xii +196.","authors":"Norbert Francis","doi":"10.1017/S0022226722000378","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1017/S0022226722000378","url":null,"abstract":"program as a whole may indeed have been (at best) premature’ (9). Yet ‘talk of optimization’ is very much still at the heart of minimalist thinking. ROLE provides no reason why minimalist discussions of optimized computational machinery are incompatible with biology. Conversely, nor are we told why Boeckx’s gradualist account of the evolution of syntax can have no place for such concerns of computational efficiency. We are also given no concrete rebuttal of earlier ideas espoused by Boeckx. There is a clear discontinuity between Boeckx’s earlier writings and his current position in ROLE, but little clarity with respect to which pieces we are supposed to pick up, and which pieces we are supposed to leave behind.","PeriodicalId":47027,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Linguistics","volume":"58 1","pages":"911 - 915"},"PeriodicalIF":1.1,"publicationDate":"2022-08-30","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"48165964","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Remnant connectivity in pseudogapping: Experimental evidence for a direct generation approach 伪映射中的残余连通性:直接生成方法的实验证据
IF 1.1 2区 文学 0 LANGUAGE & LINGUISTICS Pub Date : 2022-08-23 DOI: 10.1017/S0022226722000354
Till Poppels, P. Miller
This paper reports the results of two acceptability judgment experiments that examine the effect of PP remnants with mismatching correlates in the antecedent clause (either a PP, with a distinct preposition, or an NP) on the acceptability of pseudogapping as well as non-elliptical controls. Across both experiments, three novel findings emerge: First, utterances with mismatching PPs across the ellipsis clause and its antecedent were consistently degraded relative to their preposition-matched counterparts. Second, this mismatch penalty arose for elliptical and non-elliptical variants alike with only minor differences between the two. Finally, a significant portion of the mismatch penalties was explained away by the degree of semantic similarity between the thematic relations established by the mismatching prepositions with respect to the antecedent verb which was measured in a separate norming experiment. We examine the consequences of these new empirical results for current theories of pseudogapping, namely (i) the remnant-raising analysis, according to which the remnant XP is raised leftward out of the VP prior to VP ellipsis, licensed under identity with its antecedent; and (ii) the direct generation analysis, under which auxiliaries are verbal proforms that recover their referent anaphorically without the need for remnant movement or syntactic identity between the verb and its antecedent. We conclude that the data are more naturally accounted for under the direct generation approach.
本文报告了两个可接受性判断实验的结果,该实验检验了前句中具有不匹配相关性的PP残余(PP、具有不同介词的PP或NP)对假间隙和非椭圆对照的可接受性的影响。在这两个实验中,出现了三个新的发现:首先,相对于介词匹配的对应词,省略从句及其先行词中PP不匹配的话语持续退化。其次,这种失配惩罚适用于椭圆和非椭圆变体,两者之间只有微小差异。最后,在一个单独的规范化实验中,通过不匹配介词与先行动词建立的主位关系之间的语义相似程度来解释不匹配惩罚的很大一部分。我们检验了这些新的经验结果对当前伪缺口理论的影响,即(i)残差提升分析,根据该分析,残差XP在VP省略之前从VP中向左提升,并与其先行词一致;以及(ii)直接生成分析,在该分析中,助词是指在不需要动词与其先行词之间的残余运动或句法同一性的情况下,在回指中恢复其指称对象的动词形式。我们得出的结论是,直接生成方法更自然地解释了数据。
{"title":"Remnant connectivity in pseudogapping: Experimental evidence for a direct generation approach","authors":"Till Poppels, P. Miller","doi":"10.1017/S0022226722000354","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1017/S0022226722000354","url":null,"abstract":"This paper reports the results of two acceptability judgment experiments that examine the effect of PP remnants with mismatching correlates in the antecedent clause (either a PP, with a distinct preposition, or an NP) on the acceptability of pseudogapping as well as non-elliptical controls. Across both experiments, three novel findings emerge: First, utterances with mismatching PPs across the ellipsis clause and its antecedent were consistently degraded relative to their preposition-matched counterparts. Second, this mismatch penalty arose for elliptical and non-elliptical variants alike with only minor differences between the two. Finally, a significant portion of the mismatch penalties was explained away by the degree of semantic similarity between the thematic relations established by the mismatching prepositions with respect to the antecedent verb which was measured in a separate norming experiment. We examine the consequences of these new empirical results for current theories of pseudogapping, namely (i) the remnant-raising analysis, according to which the remnant XP is raised leftward out of the VP prior to VP ellipsis, licensed under identity with its antecedent; and (ii) the direct generation analysis, under which auxiliaries are verbal proforms that recover their referent anaphorically without the need for remnant movement or syntactic identity between the verb and its antecedent. We conclude that the data are more naturally accounted for under the direct generation approach.","PeriodicalId":47027,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Linguistics","volume":"59 1","pages":"293 - 325"},"PeriodicalIF":1.1,"publicationDate":"2022-08-23","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"49077728","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Attributional versus identificational: A dichotomous analysis of appositives in Mandarin Chinese 归因与认同:普通话同位格的二分分析
IF 1.1 2区 文学 0 LANGUAGE & LINGUISTICS Pub Date : 2022-08-15 DOI: 10.1017/S0022226722000342
Jing Jin
Among various types of nominal phrases, the appositive construction has long been a relatively under-researched subject in the literature of Chinese linguistics. This paper centers on the use of the appositive construction [P(ronoun)-Num(eral)-Cl(assifier)-Nominal Phrase (NP)] in Mandarin Chinese. Upon revealing a series of asymmetries in syntax, semantics, and pragmatics, this paper proposes an attributional versus identificational distinction for Chinese appositives. Specifically, for the attributional case, the apposition (i.e. [Num-Cl-NP]) is property-denoting and serves to rationalize the speaker’s evaluation about the referent denoted by the anchor (i.e. the P); for the identificational case, the apposition is individual-denoting and serves to facilitate referent identification of the anchor by picking out an identifiable quantified set of discourse referents from the given context. To formally capture this distinction, this paper develops a dichotomous analysis for the syntax of Chinese appositives. The non-unified treatment not only offers an effective explanation for the asymmetries exhibited by the appositives under different cases, but also contributes to a better understanding of Chinese appositives in general.
在各种类型的名词短语中,同位语结构一直是中国语言学文献中研究较少的一个课题。本文主要研究汉语中同位结构[P(ronoun)-Num(eral)-Cl(assifier)-Nnominal Phrase(NP)]的用法。在揭示句法、语义和语用学上的一系列不对称性的基础上,本文提出了汉语同位语的归因与识别区别。具体来说,对于归因情况,同位语(即[Num Cl NP])是表示性质的,用于合理化说话者对锚(即P)所表示的指称的评价;对于识别情况,同位语是个体表示,并通过从给定的上下文中挑选出一组可识别的量化话语指称来促进锚的指称识别。为了正式捕捉这种区别,本文对汉语同位语的句法进行了二分分析。非统一处理不仅为不同情况下同位语表现出的不对称性提供了有效的解释,而且有助于更好地理解汉语同位语。
{"title":"Attributional versus identificational: A dichotomous analysis of appositives in Mandarin Chinese","authors":"Jing Jin","doi":"10.1017/S0022226722000342","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1017/S0022226722000342","url":null,"abstract":"Among various types of nominal phrases, the appositive construction has long been a relatively under-researched subject in the literature of Chinese linguistics. This paper centers on the use of the appositive construction [P(ronoun)-Num(eral)-Cl(assifier)-Nominal Phrase (NP)] in Mandarin Chinese. Upon revealing a series of asymmetries in syntax, semantics, and pragmatics, this paper proposes an attributional versus identificational distinction for Chinese appositives. Specifically, for the attributional case, the apposition (i.e. [Num-Cl-NP]) is property-denoting and serves to rationalize the speaker’s evaluation about the referent denoted by the anchor (i.e. the P); for the identificational case, the apposition is individual-denoting and serves to facilitate referent identification of the anchor by picking out an identifiable quantified set of discourse referents from the given context. To formally capture this distinction, this paper develops a dichotomous analysis for the syntax of Chinese appositives. The non-unified treatment not only offers an effective explanation for the asymmetries exhibited by the appositives under different cases, but also contributes to a better understanding of Chinese appositives in general.","PeriodicalId":47027,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Linguistics","volume":"59 1","pages":"763 - 794"},"PeriodicalIF":1.1,"publicationDate":"2022-08-15","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"42742095","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
A gradualist view of word meaning in language acquisition and language use 语言习得和语言使用中词义的渐进观
IF 1.1 2区 文学 0 LANGUAGE & LINGUISTICS Pub Date : 2022-08-02 DOI: 10.1017/S0022226722000330
E. Clark
For both children and adults, communicating with each other effectively depends on having enough knowledge about particular entities, actions, or relations to understand and produce the words being used. Speakers draw on conventional meanings shared with their interlocutors, but do they share every detail of word meaning? They need not have identical, or fully specified, representations for the meanings of all the terms they make use of. Rather, they need only have represented enough about the meanings of the words used by another speaker to understand what is intended in context on a particular occasion. Reliance on partial meanings is common in both children and adults. More detailed, shared, representations of word meanings for a domain depend on acquiring additional knowledge about that domain and its contents.
对于儿童和成年人来说,有效地相互沟通取决于对特定实体、行为或关系有足够的了解,以理解和产生所使用的词语。演讲者利用与对话者共享的传统含义,但他们是否共享单词含义的每一个细节?他们不需要对他们使用的所有术语的含义有相同或完全指定的表述。相反,他们只需要对另一个说话者使用的单词的含义有足够的表达,就可以理解特定场合的上下文意图。对偏义的依赖在儿童和成人中都很常见。一个领域的词义的更详细、更共享的表示取决于获得关于该领域及其内容的额外知识。
{"title":"A gradualist view of word meaning in language acquisition and language use","authors":"E. Clark","doi":"10.1017/S0022226722000330","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1017/S0022226722000330","url":null,"abstract":"For both children and adults, communicating with each other effectively depends on having enough knowledge about particular entities, actions, or relations to understand and produce the words being used. Speakers draw on conventional meanings shared with their interlocutors, but do they share every detail of word meaning? They need not have identical, or fully specified, representations for the meanings of all the terms they make use of. Rather, they need only have represented enough about the meanings of the words used by another speaker to understand what is intended in context on a particular occasion. Reliance on partial meanings is common in both children and adults. More detailed, shared, representations of word meanings for a domain depend on acquiring additional knowledge about that domain and its contents.","PeriodicalId":47027,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Linguistics","volume":"59 1","pages":"737 - 762"},"PeriodicalIF":1.1,"publicationDate":"2022-08-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"46140390","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Clitic left dislocation and inverse scope: Plain indefinites versus numerals Clitic左错位和逆范围:简单的不确定性与数字
IF 1.1 2区 文学 0 LANGUAGE & LINGUISTICS Pub Date : 2022-07-28 DOI: 10.1017/S0022226722000135
Despina Oikonomou, Felix Golcher, A. Alexiadou
The syntax of Clitic Left Dislocation (CLLD) has been widely debated due to its mixed properties, which in some cases indicate movement (e.g. island sensitivity, certain connectivity effects) and in other cases base generation of the CLLD-ed phrase (wide scope, lack of weak crossover). In this paper we discuss scope facts with CLLD in Greek, revealing a contrast depending on the type of quantifier. We present experimental evidence that whereas CLLD-ed plain indefinites take wide scope, CLLD-ed numerals can get a low scope interpretation. We argue that the inverse scope interpretation with CLLD-ed numerals is only apparent, presenting, in fact, an instance of split scope between the degree quantifier and the existential operator. This analysis presents evidence in favor of a movement analysis for CLLD, thus patterning with the observation that binding reconstruction is possible. At the same time, the non-availability of scope reconstruction with CLLD is attributed to stricter locality constraints which have been discussed for quantifier raising as opposed to other types of movement and dependencies.
Clitic Left Dislocation(CLLD)的语法因其混合性质而广受争议,在某些情况下,它表示运动(例如岛屿敏感性、某些连接效应),而在其他情况下,CLLD ed短语的基本生成(范围广,缺乏弱交叉)。在本文中,我们用希腊语的CLLD讨论了范围事实,揭示了根据量词类型的对比。我们提出的实验证据表明,虽然CLLD ed的纯不确定性具有广泛的范围,但CLLD ed数字可以得到低范围的解释。我们认为,CLLD ed数字的反范围解释只是显而易见的,事实上,它呈现了程度量词和存在算子之间的范围分裂的例子。这一分析提供了有利于CLLD运动分析的证据,从而使结合重建成为可能。同时,CLLD的范围重建的不可用性归因于更严格的局部约束,与其他类型的移动和依赖性相比,这些局部约束已被讨论用于量词提升。
{"title":"Clitic left dislocation and inverse scope: Plain indefinites versus numerals","authors":"Despina Oikonomou, Felix Golcher, A. Alexiadou","doi":"10.1017/S0022226722000135","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1017/S0022226722000135","url":null,"abstract":"The syntax of Clitic Left Dislocation (CLLD) has been widely debated due to its mixed properties, which in some cases indicate movement (e.g. island sensitivity, certain connectivity effects) and in other cases base generation of the CLLD-ed phrase (wide scope, lack of weak crossover). In this paper we discuss scope facts with CLLD in Greek, revealing a contrast depending on the type of quantifier. We present experimental evidence that whereas CLLD-ed plain indefinites take wide scope, CLLD-ed numerals can get a low scope interpretation. We argue that the inverse scope interpretation with CLLD-ed numerals is only apparent, presenting, in fact, an instance of split scope between the degree quantifier and the existential operator. This analysis presents evidence in favor of a movement analysis for CLLD, thus patterning with the observation that binding reconstruction is possible. At the same time, the non-availability of scope reconstruction with CLLD is attributed to stricter locality constraints which have been discussed for quantifier raising as opposed to other types of movement and dependencies.","PeriodicalId":47027,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Linguistics","volume":"58 1","pages":"847 - 886"},"PeriodicalIF":1.1,"publicationDate":"2022-07-28","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"45381598","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
期刊
Journal of Linguistics
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1