首页 > 最新文献

Infant and Child Development最新文献

英文 中文
Infant-directed communication in Tanna, Vanuatu and Vancouver, Canada 瓦努阿图塔纳和加拿大温哥华的婴儿引导式交流
IF 2.8 4区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-02-22 DOI: 10.1002/icd.2498
Zahra Halavani, H. Henny Yeung, Senay Cebioğlu, Tanya Broesch

It is known that infant-directed speech (IDS) plays a key role in language development. Previous research, however, has also identified significant variability across societies in terms of how often IDS occurs. For example, some studies report very little IDS in non-western, small-scale societies – including children growing up in small-scale societies in Tanna, Vanuatu. This is surprising given that IDS is widely assumed as a common feature of human caregiving based on research conducted in urbanised populations which are more well-studied. Here, we propose that IDS is only one of a suite of important caregiving behaviours that are produced during interactions with infants, which may vary by culture, perhaps being replaced by other, non-verbal infant-directed behaviours (IDB). We will examine previously collected data consisting of 94 semi-structured 10-min video observations of caregivers and their 18–24 month-old children in rural Tanna, Vanuatu and urban Vancouver, Canada to identify and compare the proportion of time caregivers spend engaging in IDS and IDB during these interactions, both within and between societies. We define IDS as caregiver speech or vocalisations during the interaction with the infant, and we define IDB as non-verbal behaviours that are produced with the infant during the interaction. This study aims to take a step towards a more generalised understanding of language development in children, moving beyond the urban and western societies in which our understanding of development is currently based, and the predicted results will aid in recognising different developmental pathways within multi-cultural communities.

众所周知,婴儿指导言语(IDS)在语言发展中起着关键作用。然而,以往的研究也发现,不同社会的婴儿指导言语发生的频率存在很大差异。例如,一些研究报告显示,在非西方的小规模社会中,包括在瓦努阿图坦纳小规模社会中成长的儿童,婴儿指导言语(IDS)的发生率非常低。这种情况令人惊讶,因为根据对城市化人群的研究,IDS 被普遍认为是人类照料的共同特征,而城市化人群的研究则更为充分。在这里,我们提出,IDS 只是在与婴儿互动过程中产生的一系列重要护理行为之一,这些行为可能因文化而异,也许会被其他非语言婴儿指导行为(IDB)所取代。我们将研究以前收集的 94 个半结构式 10 分钟视频数据,观察瓦努阿图塔纳农村地区和加拿大温哥华市区的照顾者及其 18-24 个月大的孩子,以确定并比较照顾者在社会内部和不同社会之间的互动过程中参与 IDS 和 IDB 的时间比例。我们将 IDS 定义为照顾者在与婴儿互动过程中的言语或发声,将 IDB 定义为在互动过程中与婴儿一起做出的非言语行为。这项研究旨在超越我们目前对儿童语言发展的理解所基于的城市和西方社会,朝着更广泛地理解儿童语言发展迈出一步。
{"title":"Infant-directed communication in Tanna, Vanuatu and Vancouver, Canada","authors":"Zahra Halavani,&nbsp;H. Henny Yeung,&nbsp;Senay Cebioğlu,&nbsp;Tanya Broesch","doi":"10.1002/icd.2498","DOIUrl":"10.1002/icd.2498","url":null,"abstract":"<p>It is known that infant-directed speech (IDS) plays a key role in language development. Previous research, however, has also identified significant variability across societies in terms of how often IDS occurs. For example, some studies report very little IDS in non-western, small-scale societies – including children growing up in small-scale societies in Tanna, Vanuatu. This is surprising given that IDS is widely assumed as a common feature of human caregiving based on research conducted in urbanised populations which are more well-studied. Here, we propose that IDS is only one of a suite of important caregiving behaviours that are produced during interactions with infants, which may vary by culture, perhaps being replaced by other, non-verbal infant-directed behaviours (IDB). We will examine previously collected data consisting of 94 semi-structured 10-min video observations of caregivers and their 18–24 month-old children in rural Tanna, Vanuatu and urban Vancouver, Canada to identify and compare the proportion of time caregivers spend engaging in IDS and IDB during these interactions, both within and between societies. We define IDS as caregiver speech or vocalisations during the interaction with the infant, and we define IDB as non-verbal behaviours that are produced with the infant during the interaction. This study aims to take a step towards a more generalised understanding of language development in children, moving beyond the urban and western societies in which our understanding of development is currently based, and the predicted results will aid in recognising different developmental pathways within multi-cultural communities.</p>","PeriodicalId":47820,"journal":{"name":"Infant and Child Development","volume":"33 4","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.8,"publicationDate":"2024-02-22","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/icd.2498","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139922798","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Open science and metascience in developmental psychology: Introduction to the special issue 发展心理学中的开放科学和元科学:特刊简介
IF 2.2 4区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-02-14 DOI: 10.1002/icd.2495
Priya Silverstein, Christina Bergmann, Moin Syed
<p>It has been over 10 years since the replicability crisis and open science movement entered the mainstream of psychology (e.g., Simmons et al., <span>2011</span>). In that time, psychologists have been identifying and describing the nature of the problems with how we do our science (e.g., a lack of transparency, replicability and diversity) and debating proposed solutions for how to right the course. Two main themes emerged in this conversation: open science as a means to increase transparency and accountability and metascience as a way to identify sources of the observed problems by studying science with its own methods.</p><p>However, there seems to be an asymmetric focus across subfields within psychological sciences and previously, only a few papers examining developmental psychology existed. At the same time, the specific conditions of developmental research might make the field particularly vulnerable to findings that cannot form a solid basis for theorising, such as difficulty in recruiting populations leading to small samples and indirect tests leading to large amounts of noise (Davis-Kean & Ellis, <span>2019</span>; Frank et al., <span>2017</span>). Thus the field might be at risk of falling behind the latest developments in examining the process of generating knowledge with implications for theory, methods and measurement. This risk stands in contrast with existing open science traditions within developmental science, such as a rich history of data sharing (e.g., making language corpus data publicly available since 1984 on CHILDES; MacWhinney, <span>2000</span>) and the influential big team science collaboration ManyBabies (Frank et al., <span>2017</span>).</p><p>The purpose of this Special Issue was to provide a forum for work on metascience and open science within developmental psychology. We are very pleased to introduce 16 papers – a mix of empirical reports, commentaries, reviews, methodological articles and theoretical articles.</p><p>One of the barriers to adopting open science can be not knowing where to start (Kathawalla et al., <span>2021</span>). Luckily, this special issue includes several helpful ‘how-to’ guides! Kalandadze and Hart (<span>2022</span>) is a great place to start, with an annotated reading list on open developmental science. Turoman et al. (<span>2022</span>) present a workflow for applying open science principles in a developmental psychology lab, using their own lab as an example. Regarding data analysis, Visser et al. (<span>2023</span>) present a tutorial for using Bayesian sequential testing designs and Woods et al. (<span>2023</span>) present best practices for addressing missing data through multiple imputations.</p><p>Several articles address best practices when using different methodologies in developmental psychology. Two articles outline guidelines for applying open science practices to descriptive research (Kosie & Lew-Williams, <span>2022</span>) and longitudinal research (Petersen et a
以开放科学为重点的特刊历来对发展心理学的发展非常重要。事实上,Kalandadze 和 Hart(2022 年)的注释阅读清单中包含的两篇重要文章实际上来自《婴儿行为与发展》(Infant Behavior and Development,2019 年;Lundwall,2019 年)上一期关于 "婴儿研究中的可复制性、合作与最佳实践 "的特刊。同样,注册报告格式(Chambers &amp; Tzavella, 2021)也通过特刊(Syed et al.我们希望以同样的方式,本期特刊中的文章日后将被视为思考和撰写发展心理学开放科学和元科学时的关键文章。从拥抱开放科学的实用 "如何做 "指南,到关于提高严谨性和解决可推广性问题的讨论,本期特刊的文章内容丰富,发人深省。在发展心理学不断发展的过程中,拥抱开放科学实践、提高方法论的严谨性、促进包容性和多样性对于该领域的持续发展和相关性至关重要。因此,我们希望这里提出的观点能够激励研究人员开展更加开放、严谨和包容的发展科学研究。
{"title":"Open science and metascience in developmental psychology: Introduction to the special issue","authors":"Priya Silverstein,&nbsp;Christina Bergmann,&nbsp;Moin Syed","doi":"10.1002/icd.2495","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1002/icd.2495","url":null,"abstract":"&lt;p&gt;It has been over 10 years since the replicability crisis and open science movement entered the mainstream of psychology (e.g., Simmons et al., &lt;span&gt;2011&lt;/span&gt;). In that time, psychologists have been identifying and describing the nature of the problems with how we do our science (e.g., a lack of transparency, replicability and diversity) and debating proposed solutions for how to right the course. Two main themes emerged in this conversation: open science as a means to increase transparency and accountability and metascience as a way to identify sources of the observed problems by studying science with its own methods.&lt;/p&gt;&lt;p&gt;However, there seems to be an asymmetric focus across subfields within psychological sciences and previously, only a few papers examining developmental psychology existed. At the same time, the specific conditions of developmental research might make the field particularly vulnerable to findings that cannot form a solid basis for theorising, such as difficulty in recruiting populations leading to small samples and indirect tests leading to large amounts of noise (Davis-Kean &amp; Ellis, &lt;span&gt;2019&lt;/span&gt;; Frank et al., &lt;span&gt;2017&lt;/span&gt;). Thus the field might be at risk of falling behind the latest developments in examining the process of generating knowledge with implications for theory, methods and measurement. This risk stands in contrast with existing open science traditions within developmental science, such as a rich history of data sharing (e.g., making language corpus data publicly available since 1984 on CHILDES; MacWhinney, &lt;span&gt;2000&lt;/span&gt;) and the influential big team science collaboration ManyBabies (Frank et al., &lt;span&gt;2017&lt;/span&gt;).&lt;/p&gt;&lt;p&gt;The purpose of this Special Issue was to provide a forum for work on metascience and open science within developmental psychology. We are very pleased to introduce 16 papers – a mix of empirical reports, commentaries, reviews, methodological articles and theoretical articles.&lt;/p&gt;&lt;p&gt;One of the barriers to adopting open science can be not knowing where to start (Kathawalla et al., &lt;span&gt;2021&lt;/span&gt;). Luckily, this special issue includes several helpful ‘how-to’ guides! Kalandadze and Hart (&lt;span&gt;2022&lt;/span&gt;) is a great place to start, with an annotated reading list on open developmental science. Turoman et al. (&lt;span&gt;2022&lt;/span&gt;) present a workflow for applying open science principles in a developmental psychology lab, using their own lab as an example. Regarding data analysis, Visser et al. (&lt;span&gt;2023&lt;/span&gt;) present a tutorial for using Bayesian sequential testing designs and Woods et al. (&lt;span&gt;2023&lt;/span&gt;) present best practices for addressing missing data through multiple imputations.&lt;/p&gt;&lt;p&gt;Several articles address best practices when using different methodologies in developmental psychology. Two articles outline guidelines for applying open science practices to descriptive research (Kosie &amp; Lew-Williams, &lt;span&gt;2022&lt;/span&gt;) and longitudinal research (Petersen et a","PeriodicalId":47820,"journal":{"name":"Infant and Child Development","volume":"33 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-02-14","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/icd.2495","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139732296","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Parents' views and experiences of the home mathematics environment: A cross-country study 家长对家庭数学环境的看法和体验:跨国研究
IF 2.8 4区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-02-14 DOI: 10.1002/icd.2497
Abbie Cahoon, Yanet Campver, Nancy Estévez, Carolina Jiménez Lira, Daniela Susana Paz García, Elia Veronica Benavides Pando, Victoria Simms

The purpose of the current study was to qualitatively explore the home mathematical environment across two regions in two different countries (i.e., Cuba and Mexico), replicating a qualitative study previously conducted in Northern Ireland (NI), United Kingdom (Cahoon et al., 2017). Semi-structured interviews with parents/caregivers of children (3- to 5-year-olds) in both Mexico (n = 13) and Cuba (n = 40) were completed to investigate their views, experiences and attitudes towards the home mathematical environment. Thematic analysis was used to explore themes relevant to the home mathematical environment. Three consistent themes were found in the Mexican and Cuban data: Numeracy Environment Structure, Expectations and Attitudes and Views of Technology. Two unique themes were found in the Mexico data: Interactions Related to Reading or Mathematics, and Child's Attitudes in Relation to Mathematics. One unique theme was found in the Cuban interviews: Interactions for Learning. Although diverse themes were identified, consistencies were also observed. This suggests that some home numeracy practices may be universal in nature. This research increases the understanding of human development in context.

本研究旨在对两个不同国家(即古巴和墨西哥)两个地区的家庭数学环境进行定性探索,复制之前在英国北爱尔兰(NI)进行的一项定性研究(Cahoon et al.)对墨西哥(n = 13)和古巴(n = 40)儿童(3-5 岁)的父母/照顾者进行了半结构式访谈,以调查他们对家庭数学环境的看法、经验和态度。专题分析用于探讨与家庭数学环境相关的主题。在墨西哥和古巴的数据中发现了三个一致的主题:数学环境结构、期望和态度以及对技术的看法。在墨西哥的数据中发现了两个独特的主题:与阅读或数学有关的互动,以及儿童对数学的态度。古巴的访谈中有一个独特的主题:为学习而互动。虽然发现了不同的主题,但也观察到了一致性。这表明,一些家庭算术实践可能具有普遍性。这项研究加深了人们对人类发展背景的了解。
{"title":"Parents' views and experiences of the home mathematics environment: A cross-country study","authors":"Abbie Cahoon,&nbsp;Yanet Campver,&nbsp;Nancy Estévez,&nbsp;Carolina Jiménez Lira,&nbsp;Daniela Susana Paz García,&nbsp;Elia Veronica Benavides Pando,&nbsp;Victoria Simms","doi":"10.1002/icd.2497","DOIUrl":"10.1002/icd.2497","url":null,"abstract":"<p>The purpose of the current study was to qualitatively explore the home mathematical environment across two regions in two different countries (i.e., Cuba and Mexico), replicating a qualitative study previously conducted in Northern Ireland (NI), United Kingdom (Cahoon et al., 2017). Semi-structured interviews with parents/caregivers of children (3- to 5-year-olds) in both Mexico (<i>n</i> = 13) and Cuba (<i>n</i> = 40) were completed to investigate their views, experiences and attitudes towards the home mathematical environment. Thematic analysis was used to explore themes relevant to the home mathematical environment. Three consistent themes were found in the Mexican and Cuban data: Numeracy Environment Structure, Expectations and Attitudes and Views of Technology. Two unique themes were found in the Mexico data: Interactions Related to Reading or Mathematics, and Child's Attitudes in Relation to Mathematics. One unique theme was found in the Cuban interviews: Interactions for Learning. Although diverse themes were identified, consistencies were also observed. This suggests that some home numeracy practices may be universal in nature. This research increases the understanding of human development in context.</p>","PeriodicalId":47820,"journal":{"name":"Infant and Child Development","volume":"33 4","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.8,"publicationDate":"2024-02-14","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/icd.2497","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139776745","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Investigating the association between socioeconomic status and language skills in children with autism spectrum disorder or other developmental delays 调查自闭症谱系障碍或其他发育迟缓儿童的社会经济地位与语言技能之间的关系
IF 2.8 4区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-02-07 DOI: 10.1002/icd.2493
Meredith Pecukonis, Julia Levinson, Andrea Chu, Sarabeth Broder-Fingert, Emily Feinberg, Howard Cabral, Helen Tager-Flusberg

Numerous studies have reported that socioeconomic status (SES) predicts language skills in typically developing children. However, this association has been less systematically studied in children with developmental disabilities, such as autism spectrum disorder (ASD) or other developmental delays (DD). In the present study, we examined the association between SES, operationalized as maternal education attainment and health insurance status, and receptive and expressive language skills in a sample of children from lower SES, racial/ethnic minority families at increased ‘clinical risk’ for ASD based on early screening. Neither maternal education attainment nor health insurance status were significantly associated with children's language skills. Expressive and receptive language skills were significantly higher in children with DD compared to children with ASD. Findings differ from previously published work, highlighting the importance of study replication. Further research is needed to understand why the association between SES and children's language skills might vary across samples.

Highlights

  • Examined association between socioeconomic status (SES) and language skills in children at ‘clinical risk’ for autism based on early screening.
  • Found no associations between measures of SES and children's language skills.
  • Results differ from previously published work, highlighting the importance of replicating studies with lower SES, racial/ethnic minority families.
大量研究表明,社会经济地位(SES)可预测发育正常儿童的语言能力。然而,对自闭症谱系障碍(ASD)或其他发育迟缓(DD)等发育障碍儿童的这种关联性却较少进行系统研究。在本研究中,我们研究了来自社会经济地位较低、种族/族裔少数、根据早期筛查ASD "临床风险 "较高的家庭的儿童样本中,社会经济地位与接受性和表达性语言技能之间的关系,社会经济地位具体表现为母亲的教育程度和医疗保险状况。母亲的教育程度和医疗保险状况均与儿童的语言技能无明显关联。与 ASD 儿童相比,DD 儿童的表达性和接受性语言技能明显更高。研究结果与之前发表的研究结果不同,这凸显了重复研究的重要性。研究发现,社会经济地位的衡量标准与儿童的语言能力之间没有关联。研究结果与之前发表的研究结果不同,这凸显了对社会经济地位较低、种族/少数民族家庭进行重复研究的重要性。
{"title":"Investigating the association between socioeconomic status and language skills in children with autism spectrum disorder or other developmental delays","authors":"Meredith Pecukonis,&nbsp;Julia Levinson,&nbsp;Andrea Chu,&nbsp;Sarabeth Broder-Fingert,&nbsp;Emily Feinberg,&nbsp;Howard Cabral,&nbsp;Helen Tager-Flusberg","doi":"10.1002/icd.2493","DOIUrl":"10.1002/icd.2493","url":null,"abstract":"<div>\u0000 \u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <p>Numerous studies have reported that socioeconomic status (SES) predicts language skills in typically developing children. However, this association has been less systematically studied in children with developmental disabilities, such as autism spectrum disorder (ASD) or other developmental delays (DD). In the present study, we examined the association between SES, operationalized as maternal education attainment and health insurance status, and receptive and expressive language skills in a sample of children from lower SES, racial/ethnic minority families at increased ‘clinical risk’ for ASD based on early screening. Neither maternal education attainment nor health insurance status were significantly associated with children's language skills. Expressive and receptive language skills were significantly higher in children with DD compared to children with ASD. Findings differ from previously published work, highlighting the importance of study replication. Further research is needed to understand why the association between SES and children's language skills might vary across samples.</p>\u0000 </section>\u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <h3> Highlights</h3>\u0000 \u0000 <div>\u0000 <ul>\u0000 \u0000 <li>Examined association between socioeconomic status (SES) and language skills in children at ‘clinical risk’ for autism based on early screening.</li>\u0000 \u0000 <li>Found no associations between measures of SES and children's language skills.</li>\u0000 \u0000 <li>Results differ from previously published work, highlighting the importance of replicating studies with lower SES, racial/ethnic minority families.</li>\u0000 </ul>\u0000 </div>\u0000 </section>\u0000 </div>","PeriodicalId":47820,"journal":{"name":"Infant and Child Development","volume":"33 4","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.8,"publicationDate":"2024-02-07","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139854707","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Associations among family socioeconomic status, parenting and sustained attention for socioeconomically disadvantaged children at age 5 years 5 岁社会经济弱势儿童的家庭社会经济地位、养育方式和持续注意力之间的关系
IF 2.8 4区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-02-07 DOI: 10.1002/icd.2491
Wanqiu Meng, Caroline F. D. Black, Min Feng

Children whose families experience socioeconomic disadvantage are at risk for poor sustained attention, a foundational skill related to goal-oriented behaviour, self-regulation and kindergarten readiness. Maternal parenting behaviours and parenting stress are theorised developmental pathways linking socioeconomic status (SES) to children's sustained attention. However, research has yet to empirically test for these indirect pathways, thus limiting the relevance of extant findings to inform targets of parent-mediated programmes addressing attentional disparities at school entry for low-income children. Using a sample of mostly low-income children from the Future of Families and Child Wellbeing Study (N = 2214) and the process of parenting model, this study tested whether maternal parenting stress, harsh parenting and cognitive stimulation uniquely explained associations between SES and child sustained attention at age 5 years. Results from structural equation modelling revealed that SES was positively and directly associated with child sustained attention. Some of these effects were indirectly transmitted through the pathway of maternal cognitive stimulation; however, neither harsh parenting nor parenting stress helped to explain the relationship between SES and child sustained attention. Implications of the findings are explored in the context of early childhood prevention and promotion programmes serving low-income families.

Highlights

  • This study explores the direct and indirect paths of family SES on child sustained attention through the pathways of maternal cognitive stimulation, harsh parenting and parenting stress amongst socioeconomically disadvantaged children.
  • Results from SEM revealed that SES was positively and directly associated with child-sustained attention and some of these effects were indirectly transmitted through the pathway of maternal cognitive stimulation.
  • Cognitive stimulation is a crucial pathway through which SES may promote or impede the development of children's sustained attention skills.
持续注意力是一项与目标导向行为、自我调节和幼儿园准备相关的基础技能。理论上,母亲的养育行为和养育压力是将社会经济地位(SES)与儿童的持续注意力联系起来的发展途径。然而,目前的研究还没有对这些间接途径进行实证检验,因此限制了现有研究结果的相关性,无法为针对低收入儿童入学时注意力差异的以父母为中介的计划提供目标信息。本研究利用 "家庭未来和儿童福祉研究"(N = 2214)中的低收入儿童样本和养育过程模型,检验了母亲的养育压力、严厉的养育方式和认知刺激是否能独特解释社会经济地位与 5 岁儿童持续注意力之间的关联。结构方程模型的结果显示,社会经济地位与儿童的持续注意力直接呈正相关。其中一些影响通过母亲的认知刺激途径间接传递;然而,严厉的养育方式和养育压力都无助于解释社会经济地位与儿童持续注意力之间的关系。研究结果对服务于低收入家庭的幼儿预防和促进计划的影响进行了探讨。
{"title":"Associations among family socioeconomic status, parenting and sustained attention for socioeconomically disadvantaged children at age 5 years","authors":"Wanqiu Meng,&nbsp;Caroline F. D. Black,&nbsp;Min Feng","doi":"10.1002/icd.2491","DOIUrl":"10.1002/icd.2491","url":null,"abstract":"<div>\u0000 \u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <p>Children whose families experience socioeconomic disadvantage are at risk for poor sustained attention, a foundational skill related to goal-oriented behaviour, self-regulation and kindergarten readiness. Maternal parenting behaviours and parenting stress are theorised developmental pathways linking socioeconomic status (SES) to children's sustained attention. However, research has yet to empirically test for these indirect pathways, thus limiting the relevance of extant findings to inform targets of parent-mediated programmes addressing attentional disparities at school entry for low-income children. Using a sample of mostly low-income children from the Future of Families and Child Wellbeing Study (<i>N</i> = 2214) and the process of parenting model, this study tested whether maternal parenting stress, harsh parenting and cognitive stimulation uniquely explained associations between SES and child sustained attention at age 5 years. Results from structural equation modelling revealed that SES was positively and directly associated with child sustained attention. Some of these effects were indirectly transmitted through the pathway of maternal cognitive stimulation; however, neither harsh parenting nor parenting stress helped to explain the relationship between SES and child sustained attention. Implications of the findings are explored in the context of early childhood prevention and promotion programmes serving low-income families.</p>\u0000 </section>\u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <h3> Highlights</h3>\u0000 \u0000 <div>\u0000 <ul>\u0000 \u0000 <li>This study explores the direct and indirect paths of family SES on child sustained attention through the pathways of maternal cognitive stimulation, harsh parenting and parenting stress amongst socioeconomically disadvantaged children.</li>\u0000 \u0000 <li>Results from SEM revealed that SES was positively and directly associated with child-sustained attention and some of these effects were indirectly transmitted through the pathway of maternal cognitive stimulation.</li>\u0000 \u0000 <li>Cognitive stimulation is a crucial pathway through which SES may promote or impede the development of children's sustained attention skills.</li>\u0000 </ul>\u0000 </div>\u0000 </section>\u0000 </div>","PeriodicalId":47820,"journal":{"name":"Infant and Child Development","volume":"33 4","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.8,"publicationDate":"2024-02-07","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139750372","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Don't look any further: How a model's linguistic group membership limits exploration and discovery in preschool children 不要再往下看了模型的语言群体成员身份如何限制学龄前儿童的探索和发现
IF 2.8 4区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-01-30 DOI: 10.1002/icd.2494
Nazlı Altınok, Rebeka Anna Zsoldos, Krisztina Andrási, Ildikó Király, Marco F. H. Schmidt
<p>From an early age, children are highly motivated to learn from others how ‘we’ do things in our cultural communities, and in a variety of domains (Csibra & Gergely, <span>2009</span>; Schmidt & Tomasello, <span>2012</span>; Tomasello, <span>2016</span>). The human unique propensity for rapid acquisition of knowledge from others allows us to develop and maintain a vast array of belief systems, arbitrary conventions, cultural tools and complex institutions (Boyd et al., <span>2011</span>; Gergely & Csibra, <span>2006</span>; Legare, <span>2019</span>; Schmidt & Rakoczy, <span>2019</span>; Tomasello, <span>1996</span>). However, when children observe others' behaviours, some actions they observe may be erroneous, purposefully deceptive or, in some cases, simply irrelevant. Think of human-made artefacts whose causal structure and intended function(s) are often opaque to naïve learners (Hernik & Csibra, <span>2009</span>; Schmidt & Rakoczy, <span>2023</span>). Chopsticks might be designed for eating, but they also have other potential functions: you can scratch your back with them, you can use them to tidy up your hair, you can use them to stir food or pick up out-of-reach objects. Observing all these different functions of the very same object, how do children decide which of these is the proper and culturally right way to use the artefact?</p><p>According to current theories of socio-cultural learning, human children have a propensity to engage in shared and collective forms (‘we-mode’) of cognition that allow for acquiring and constructing cultural-normative knowledge (Schmidt & Rakoczy, <span>2018</span>, <span>2019</span>; Tomasello, <span>2019</span>). Children are also attuned to ostensive communicative signals (e.g., eye contact and infant-directed speech) displayed by others that indicate their pedagogical intention (Natural Pedagogy Theory, Csibra & Gergely, <span>2009</span>, Csibra & Gergely, <span>2011</span>). Ostensive communicative signals instantiate a presumption of relevance (Sperber & Wilson, <span>1986</span>) and epistemic trust in their addressees (Gergely et al., <span>2007</span>). Hence recognising the teaching intention of others helps young children to selectively attend to and acquire knowledge from trustworthy resources in a noisy social learning environment. Computational modelling work on teaching further indicates that a naïve learner expects a teacher to purposefully generate samples to increase their belief in the hypothesis that is to be learned: there is a basic epistemic expectation in naïve learners that a teacher would sample useful data for them (Bonawitz et al., <span>2011</span>; Buchsbaum et al., <span>2011</span>; Shafto & Goodman, <span>2008</span>).</p><p>Whilst to date, there are several lines of empirical evidence showing that children indeed construe teachers as selectively demonstrating evidence to promote efficient and rapid information gain that allows
儿童从小就有强烈的动机向他人学习 "我们 "是如何在我们的文化社区和各种领域做事的(Csibra &amp; Gergely, 2009; Schmidt &amp; Tomasello, 2012; Tomasello, 2016)。人类从他人那里快速获取知识的独特倾向使我们能够发展并维持大量的信仰体系、任意约定、文化工具和复杂的制度(Boyd et al.)然而,当儿童观察他人的行为时,他们所观察到的某些行为可能是错误的、故意欺骗的,或者在某些情况下根本就是无关紧要的。想想人类制造的人工制品,其因果结构和预期功能对于天真的学习者来说往往是不透明的(Hernik &amp; Csibra, 2009; Schmidt &amp; Rakoczy, 2023)。筷子可能是为吃饭而设计的,但它还有其他潜在的功能:你可以用它来挠背,可以用它来整理头发,可以用它来搅拌食物或拿起够不着的东西。根据当前的社会文化学习理论,人类儿童倾向于以共享和集体的形式("我们模式")进行认知,从而获取和建构文化规范性知识(Schmidt &amp; Rakoczy, 2018, 2019; Tomasello, 2019)。儿童还能注意到他人显示的、表明其教学意图的表面交流信号(如眼神接触和婴儿引导式言语)(自然教学法理论,Csibra &amp; Gergely, 2009; Csibra &amp; Gergely, 2011)。密集交际信号体现了对相关性的假定(Sperber &amp; Wilson, 1986)和对受众的认识信任(Gergely et al.)因此,识别他人的教学意图有助于幼儿在嘈杂的社会学习环境中选择性地关注并从值得信赖的资源中获取知识。有关教学的计算建模工作进一步表明,天真的学习者期望教师有目的地生成样本,以增加他们对所要学习的假设的信念:天真的学习者有一个基本的认识论期望,即教师会为他们提供有用的数据样本(Bonawitz 等人,2011 年;Buchsbaum 等人,2011 年;Shafto &amp; Pennsylvania 等人,2011 年;Shafto &amp; Pennsylvania 等人,2011 年;Shafto &amp; Pennsylvania 等人,2011 年)、虽然迄今为止,有多种实证证据表明,儿童确实认为教师会有选择地展示证据,以促进高效、快速地获取信息,从而进行归纳推理(例如,Butler &amp; Markman, 2012, 2014),但先前的研究也发现,教学情境限制了儿童的探索行为(Bonawitz et al.)然而,一个重要的问题依然存在:儿童是否会考虑到自己的语言群体成员身份,进而考虑到教师示范的社会相关性,对教师行使情境敏感的认识警觉?我们认为,当涉及到人工制品功能(即一种规范性文化知识;Casler 等人,2009;Schmidt &amp; Rakoczy, 2018)的演示时,儿童会利用有关教师社会群体成员身份的明确(语言)线索来迅速获取相关文化知识,从而在看到与他们来自同一社会群体的教师演示人工制品功能时限制他们对人工制品的探索行为。换句话说,我们预测,如果儿童观察到演示者用自己的母语在具有多种潜在能力的新奇人工制品上演示特定功能,那么他们就更有可能在自己的重演中优先考虑所演示的功能。另一方面,当他们看到会说外语的人对人工制品进行同样的功能演示时,他们会更倾向于探索人工制品以实现其他潜在功能,因为他们不会把演示理解为与他们相关的物品的常规、规范使用。在此,我们将重点放在语言群体成员身份上,因为与种族或性别不同,基于语言的社会类别能可靠地标记不同社会群体的边界(Baker,2001;Giles &amp; Billings,2004;Henrich &amp; Henrich,2007;Moya &amp; Henrich,2016;Pietraszewski &amp; Schwartz,2014a;Pietraszewski &amp; Schwartz,2014b)。即使婴儿在很小的时候就表现出对种族或性别维度的敏感性,正如他们对这些社会类别区别的不同视觉关注所反映的那样(Bar-Haim 等人,2006 年;Quinn 等人,2002 年),我们仍然认为,语言群体成员身份在儿童向他人学习的过程中扮演着特殊的角色。 考虑到越来越多的证据表明,母语对儿童将功能映射到新奇人工制品的方式具有影响作用,以及人工制品的使用方式是如何根植于文化学习的,因为它们的使用方式是由传统协议形成的,在此,我们采用 Bonawitz 等人(2011 年)的经典范式,研究儿童在语言内群体与语言外群体的演示之后,如何限制他们对新奇人工制品的探索行为。这项研究有两个目的。我们的第一个目的是在不同的文化背景下,以[匿名]儿童为样本,从概念上复制 Bonawitz 等人(2011 年)的原始研究。其次,我们旨在扩展对原始研究结果的解释,认为儿童自发地限制对新奇人工制品的探索,可能是由于他们有强烈的动机从自己的语言群体中快速获取相关的文化知识。不同的研究表明,幼儿倾向于过度赋予新人工制品以功能和正确使用的规范,即使该人工制品有其他同样可行的使用方法(promiscuous teleology and normativity; Casler &amp; Kelemen, Casler &amp; Kelemen, 2005; Kelemen, Kelemen, 1999, 2004; Schmidt et al、施密特等人,2011;施密特等人,2016)。考虑到这一点,我们建议使用具有多种功能的陌生仪器,就像博纳维茨等人(2011 年)最初研究中使用的仪器一样。我们认为,这种器具模仿的是具有一种预期功能但可能有多种用途的日常用品(例如,叉子是用来吃饭的,但也可以用来梳头或从抽屉下面拿出东西),因此,它可以成为研究文化学习的合适工具。因此,在 Bonawitz 及其同事的研究中,孩子们会看到一个具有四种不同功能的新工具。在同种语言演示中,他们将观察一位与自己说同种语言的女性成人演示该人工制品的一种功能(以下称 "语言同群 "条件)。在外语演示中,他们将观察另一位与他们说不同语言的成年女性演示相同的功能("语言外群 "条件)。重要的是,这些示范将是非语言的,但会结合暗示信号来揭示示范者的教学意图。示范阶段结束后,儿童将获得人工制品,我们将测量儿童对人工制品的探索行为。我们预测,如果示范者是语言上的同组教师,那么儿童就会更多地关注人工制品的功能,从而限制了他们的探索行为和发现。我们预计,当语言外群教师进行演示时,儿童会广泛探索人工制品,并发现其非演示功能。
{"title":"Don't look any further: How a model's linguistic group membership limits exploration and discovery in preschool children","authors":"Nazlı Altınok,&nbsp;Rebeka Anna Zsoldos,&nbsp;Krisztina Andrási,&nbsp;Ildikó Király,&nbsp;Marco F. H. Schmidt","doi":"10.1002/icd.2494","DOIUrl":"10.1002/icd.2494","url":null,"abstract":"&lt;p&gt;From an early age, children are highly motivated to learn from others how ‘we’ do things in our cultural communities, and in a variety of domains (Csibra &amp; Gergely, &lt;span&gt;2009&lt;/span&gt;; Schmidt &amp; Tomasello, &lt;span&gt;2012&lt;/span&gt;; Tomasello, &lt;span&gt;2016&lt;/span&gt;). The human unique propensity for rapid acquisition of knowledge from others allows us to develop and maintain a vast array of belief systems, arbitrary conventions, cultural tools and complex institutions (Boyd et al., &lt;span&gt;2011&lt;/span&gt;; Gergely &amp; Csibra, &lt;span&gt;2006&lt;/span&gt;; Legare, &lt;span&gt;2019&lt;/span&gt;; Schmidt &amp; Rakoczy, &lt;span&gt;2019&lt;/span&gt;; Tomasello, &lt;span&gt;1996&lt;/span&gt;). However, when children observe others' behaviours, some actions they observe may be erroneous, purposefully deceptive or, in some cases, simply irrelevant. Think of human-made artefacts whose causal structure and intended function(s) are often opaque to naïve learners (Hernik &amp; Csibra, &lt;span&gt;2009&lt;/span&gt;; Schmidt &amp; Rakoczy, &lt;span&gt;2023&lt;/span&gt;). Chopsticks might be designed for eating, but they also have other potential functions: you can scratch your back with them, you can use them to tidy up your hair, you can use them to stir food or pick up out-of-reach objects. Observing all these different functions of the very same object, how do children decide which of these is the proper and culturally right way to use the artefact?&lt;/p&gt;&lt;p&gt;According to current theories of socio-cultural learning, human children have a propensity to engage in shared and collective forms (‘we-mode’) of cognition that allow for acquiring and constructing cultural-normative knowledge (Schmidt &amp; Rakoczy, &lt;span&gt;2018&lt;/span&gt;, &lt;span&gt;2019&lt;/span&gt;; Tomasello, &lt;span&gt;2019&lt;/span&gt;). Children are also attuned to ostensive communicative signals (e.g., eye contact and infant-directed speech) displayed by others that indicate their pedagogical intention (Natural Pedagogy Theory, Csibra &amp; Gergely, &lt;span&gt;2009&lt;/span&gt;, Csibra &amp; Gergely, &lt;span&gt;2011&lt;/span&gt;). Ostensive communicative signals instantiate a presumption of relevance (Sperber &amp; Wilson, &lt;span&gt;1986&lt;/span&gt;) and epistemic trust in their addressees (Gergely et al., &lt;span&gt;2007&lt;/span&gt;). Hence recognising the teaching intention of others helps young children to selectively attend to and acquire knowledge from trustworthy resources in a noisy social learning environment. Computational modelling work on teaching further indicates that a naïve learner expects a teacher to purposefully generate samples to increase their belief in the hypothesis that is to be learned: there is a basic epistemic expectation in naïve learners that a teacher would sample useful data for them (Bonawitz et al., &lt;span&gt;2011&lt;/span&gt;; Buchsbaum et al., &lt;span&gt;2011&lt;/span&gt;; Shafto &amp; Goodman, &lt;span&gt;2008&lt;/span&gt;).&lt;/p&gt;&lt;p&gt;Whilst to date, there are several lines of empirical evidence showing that children indeed construe teachers as selectively demonstrating evidence to promote efficient and rapid information gain that allows ","PeriodicalId":47820,"journal":{"name":"Infant and Child Development","volume":"33 4","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.8,"publicationDate":"2024-01-30","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/icd.2494","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139670405","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Into the unknown: Maternal responsiveness influences young children's support-giving expectations of fathers but not unfamiliar others 进入未知世界:母亲的反应能力会影响幼儿对父亲给予支持的期望,但不会影响陌生的其他人
IF 2.8 4区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-01-29 DOI: 10.1002/icd.2496
Chenglong Jia, Yuan Shen, Ling Zhu, Li Sun, Kelin Du, Jinliang Qin

Interactions with primary caregivers shape children's expectations of interactions with others, but the mechanism is not clear yet. We included Han Chinese children aged 5–6 (N = 121, 61 boys) from a city in East China and tested their support-giving expectations of mothers, fathers and unfamiliar others after observing mother–child interactions with different levels of maternal responsiveness. Across two studies, we found that children's support-giving behaviour and willingness expectations decreased significantly in the unresponsive condition for mothers and fathers but not for unfamiliar others; while these expectations did not change significantly for all partners in the responsive condition. The results suggest that relationship type between children and interaction partners moderates the effects of maternal responsiveness on children's expectations of partners' support-giving. They shed light on how young children understand and represent interpersonal interactions.

与主要照顾者的互动会影响儿童对与他人互动的期望,但其机制尚不清楚。我们纳入了华东某市 5-6 岁的汉族儿童(121 人,61 名男孩),测试了他们在观察不同程度的母亲回应性的母子互动后,对母亲、父亲和陌生他人给予支持的期望。在两项研究中,我们发现在无反应条件下,儿童对母亲和父亲的支持给予行为和意愿期望显著下降,但对陌生他人的支持给予行为和意愿期望却没有显著下降;而在有反应条件下,儿童对所有伙伴的支持给予行为和意愿期望都没有显著变化。结果表明,儿童与互动伙伴之间的关系类型调节了母亲的反应性对儿童对伙伴给予支持的期望的影响。这些研究揭示了幼儿如何理解和表现人际互动。
{"title":"Into the unknown: Maternal responsiveness influences young children's support-giving expectations of fathers but not unfamiliar others","authors":"Chenglong Jia,&nbsp;Yuan Shen,&nbsp;Ling Zhu,&nbsp;Li Sun,&nbsp;Kelin Du,&nbsp;Jinliang Qin","doi":"10.1002/icd.2496","DOIUrl":"10.1002/icd.2496","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Interactions with primary caregivers shape children's expectations of interactions with others, but the mechanism is not clear yet. We included Han Chinese children aged 5–6 (<i>N</i> = 121, 61 boys) from a city in East China and tested their support-giving expectations of mothers, fathers and unfamiliar others after observing mother–child interactions with different levels of maternal responsiveness. Across two studies, we found that children's support-giving behaviour and willingness expectations decreased significantly in the unresponsive condition for mothers and fathers but not for unfamiliar others; while these expectations did not change significantly for all partners in the responsive condition. The results suggest that relationship type between children and interaction partners moderates the effects of maternal responsiveness on children's expectations of partners' support-giving. They shed light on how young children understand and represent interpersonal interactions.</p>","PeriodicalId":47820,"journal":{"name":"Infant and Child Development","volume":"33 4","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.8,"publicationDate":"2024-01-29","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140488765","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Peer groups, academic achievement and the behaviour of elementary school-aged children: A strength-based perspective 同伴群体、学业成绩和小学生的行为:基于力量的视角
IF 2.8 4区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-01-09 DOI: 10.1002/icd.2489
Marleene Rytioja, Kristiina Lappalainen, Hannu Savolainen

The purpose of this study was to examine how the members of children's peer groups resemble each other in terms of behavioural and emotional strengths, academic achievement and behaviour at school. The participants were 739 9- to 10-year-old children (354 boys, 385 girls) from 30 Finnish elementary schools. 431 children (241 girls, 190 boys) were part of peer groups. Less than 5% of participants were other than Finnish origin. The children self-assessed their behavioural and emotional strengths, and teachers evaluated the children's academic achievement and behaviour at school. Children also filled the sociometric questionnaire and sociograms were drawn to identify peer groups. Standardized beta values of regression analyses were used as indicators of effect sizes. The results showed that when observed as a strength, children's school functioning was positively linked to both academic achievement and behaviour at school. The intraclass correlations (ICC) revealed that members of peer groups resembled each other in terms of behavioural strengths, and behaviour at school, but not in academic achievement. The gender composition of peer groups explained the variance in behaviour at school between peer groups. The results of this study highlight the importance to consider children's peer group also when implementing behavioural interventions.

本研究旨在考察儿童同伴群体成员在行为和情感优势、学业成绩和在校表现方面的相似性。研究对象是来自芬兰 30 所小学的 739 名 9 至 10 岁儿童(354 名男孩,385 名女孩)。431名儿童(241名女孩,190名男孩)参加了同伴小组。只有不到5%的参与者不是芬兰人。孩子们对自己的行为和情感优势进行了自我评估,教师则对孩子们的学习成绩和在校表现进行了评价。孩子们还填写了社会测量问卷,并绘制了社会图以确定同伴群体。回归分析的标准化贝塔值被用作效应大小的指标。结果表明,如果将儿童的学校功能作为一种优势来观察,则其与学业成绩和在校行为均呈正相关。类内相关(ICC)显示,同伴群体成员在行为优势和在校表现方面彼此相似,但在学业成绩方面并不相似。朋辈群体的性别组成解释了朋辈群体之间在校行为的差异。这项研究的结果突显了在实施行为干预时考虑儿童同伴群体的重要性。
{"title":"Peer groups, academic achievement and the behaviour of elementary school-aged children: A strength-based perspective","authors":"Marleene Rytioja,&nbsp;Kristiina Lappalainen,&nbsp;Hannu Savolainen","doi":"10.1002/icd.2489","DOIUrl":"10.1002/icd.2489","url":null,"abstract":"<p>The purpose of this study was to examine how the members of children's peer groups resemble each other in terms of behavioural and emotional strengths, academic achievement and behaviour at school. The participants were 739 9- to 10-year-old children (354 boys, 385 girls) from 30 Finnish elementary schools. 431 children (241 girls, 190 boys) were part of peer groups. Less than 5% of participants were other than Finnish origin. The children self-assessed their behavioural and emotional strengths, and teachers evaluated the children's academic achievement and behaviour at school. Children also filled the sociometric questionnaire and sociograms were drawn to identify peer groups. Standardized beta values of regression analyses were used as indicators of effect sizes. The results showed that when observed as a strength, children's school functioning was positively linked to both academic achievement and behaviour at school. The intraclass correlations (ICC) revealed that members of peer groups resembled each other in terms of behavioural strengths, and behaviour at school, but not in academic achievement. The gender composition of peer groups explained the variance in behaviour at school between peer groups. The results of this study highlight the importance to consider children's peer group also when implementing behavioural interventions.</p>","PeriodicalId":47820,"journal":{"name":"Infant and Child Development","volume":"33 4","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.8,"publicationDate":"2024-01-09","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/icd.2489","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139407938","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Grounding understanding of the home numeracy environment: Within-group variation in Latine families 了解家庭算术环境的基础:拉丁语系家庭的组内差异
IF 2.8 4区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-01-07 DOI: 10.1002/icd.2481
Jimena Cosso, Gala Campos Oaxaca, David J. Purpura

The home numeracy environment is an essential construct that helps explain children's numeracy skills. However, this field has been developed mostly focusing on monolingual English-speaking families, and cultural differences that contextualize the home environment have not been considered. This study describes the home numeracy environment of Latine families by (1) identifying the more and less common numeracy activities in the home of Latine families; (2) exploring differences, such as family members involved in the activities and generation in the United States; and (3) examining the association between activities and math anxiety and beliefs. Participants were 341 caregiver-child dyads (M age = 2.9 years, SD = 0.7 months). We found that the less common numeracy activities in the home of Latine families are items commonly used in the literature to measure numeracy home environment. In addition, the role of family members varies across generations. This study provides a foundation for understanding the home numeracy environment of Latine families.

家庭算术环境是有助于解释儿童算术技能的重要因素。然而,这一领域的研究主要集中在单语英语家庭,并未考虑到家庭环境的文化差异。本研究通过(1)确定拉美家庭中较常见和较少见的算术活动;(2)探索差异,如参与活动的家庭成员和在美国的世代;以及(3)研究活动与数学焦虑和信念之间的关联,来描述拉美家庭的算术环境。参与者为 341 个照顾者-儿童二元组合(中位年龄 = 2.9 岁,标差 = 0.7 个月)。我们发现,拉美裔家庭中较少见的算术活动是文献中常用来衡量算术家庭环境的项目。此外,家庭成员在不同代际中扮演的角色也不尽相同。这项研究为了解拉丁裔家庭的算术环境奠定了基础。
{"title":"Grounding understanding of the home numeracy environment: Within-group variation in Latine families","authors":"Jimena Cosso,&nbsp;Gala Campos Oaxaca,&nbsp;David J. Purpura","doi":"10.1002/icd.2481","DOIUrl":"10.1002/icd.2481","url":null,"abstract":"<p>The home numeracy environment is an essential construct that helps explain children's numeracy skills. However, this field has been developed mostly focusing on monolingual English-speaking families, and cultural differences that contextualize the home environment have not been considered. This study describes the home numeracy environment of Latine families by (1) identifying the more and less common numeracy activities in the home of Latine families; (2) exploring differences, such as family members involved in the activities and generation in the United States; and (3) examining the association between activities and math anxiety and beliefs. Participants were 341 caregiver-child dyads (<i>M</i> age = 2.9 years, SD = 0.7 months). We found that the less common numeracy activities in the home of Latine families are items commonly used in the literature to measure numeracy home environment. In addition, the role of family members varies across generations. This study provides a foundation for understanding the home numeracy environment of Latine families.</p>","PeriodicalId":47820,"journal":{"name":"Infant and Child Development","volume":"33 4","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.8,"publicationDate":"2024-01-07","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139400761","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The feasibility of remote measurement of infant sleep and motor development 远程测量婴儿睡眠和运动发育的可行性
IF 2.8 4区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-01-04 DOI: 10.1002/icd.2488
Aaron DeMasi, Mali Waugh, Wei Wang, Sarah E. Berger

In response to the COVID-19 pandemic shut-down of in-person research laboratories, remote data collection became mainstream in developmental psychology research. The aim of this study was to test the feasibility of two remote data collection methods for studying sleep and motor development in infancy and the relationship between the two. We asked 1371 parents of infants aged 4–17 months who used the Nanit baby monitor to take the online Survey of Well-being of Young Children (SWYC) about their infants' motor experience. Nanit uses auto-videosomnography and computer vision algorithms to calculate and report summary statistics of infants' sleep. Nanit replicated all expected developmental trends in sleep, while the SWYC only replicated some, appearing to underestimate infants at the transition between motor milestones. However, we replicated some well-established findings regarding the relationship between sleep and motor development using both measures, suggesting that both Nanit and the SWYC can feasibly be used for remote data collection, especially if some modifications were made to how researchers explained using the SWYC to caregivers. Researchers should consider alternatives to traditional research methods not only when in-person participation is difficult or impossible, but also when large samples are needed and/or simple measurement tools are sufficient.

由于 COVID-19 大流行而关闭了亲临现场的研究实验室,远程数据收集成为发展心理学研究的主流。本研究旨在测试两种远程数据收集方法在研究婴儿期睡眠和运动发育以及两者之间关系方面的可行性。我们邀请了 1371 位使用 Nanit 婴儿监视器的 4-17 个月大婴儿的父母参加在线幼儿福利调查 (SWYC),了解他们婴儿的运动经验。Nanit 采用自动视频监测和计算机视觉算法来计算和报告婴儿睡眠的简要统计数据。Nanit 复制了所有预期的睡眠发育趋势,而 SWYC 仅复制了部分趋势,似乎低估了处于运动里程碑过渡阶段的婴儿。不过,我们利用这两种测量方法复制了一些有关睡眠与运动发育之间关系的成熟研究结果,这表明 Nanit 和 SWYC 都可以用于远程数据收集,尤其是如果研究人员在向照护者解释如何使用 SWYC 时进行一些修改的话。研究人员不仅应在难以或无法亲自参与时考虑传统研究方法的替代方法,而且应在需要大量样本和/或只需简单测量工具时考虑替代方法。
{"title":"The feasibility of remote measurement of infant sleep and motor development","authors":"Aaron DeMasi,&nbsp;Mali Waugh,&nbsp;Wei Wang,&nbsp;Sarah E. Berger","doi":"10.1002/icd.2488","DOIUrl":"10.1002/icd.2488","url":null,"abstract":"<p>In response to the COVID-19 pandemic shut-down of in-person research laboratories, remote data collection became mainstream in developmental psychology research. The aim of this study was to test the feasibility of two remote data collection methods for studying sleep and motor development in infancy and the relationship between the two. We asked 1371 parents of infants aged 4–17 months who used the Nanit baby monitor to take the online Survey of Well-being of Young Children (SWYC) about their infants' motor experience. Nanit uses auto-videosomnography and computer vision algorithms to calculate and report summary statistics of infants' sleep. Nanit replicated all expected developmental trends in sleep, while the SWYC only replicated some, appearing to underestimate infants at the transition between motor milestones. However, we replicated some well-established findings regarding the relationship between sleep and motor development using both measures, suggesting that both Nanit and the SWYC can feasibly be used for remote data collection, especially if some modifications were made to how researchers explained using the SWYC to caregivers. Researchers should consider alternatives to traditional research methods not only when in-person participation is difficult or impossible, but also when large samples are needed and/or simple measurement tools are sufficient.</p>","PeriodicalId":47820,"journal":{"name":"Infant and Child Development","volume":"33 3","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.8,"publicationDate":"2024-01-04","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139112214","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Infant and Child Development
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1