N. Keita Christophe, Michelle Y. Martin Romero, Gabriela L. Stein
Shift-&-persist (S&P) coping has been shown to buffer against the effects of discrimination on psychosocial functioning in racially and ethnically minoritized youth. However, existing measures of S&P refer broadly to coping with stress and are not specifically tailored to the type of stressor individuals are coping with (e.g., discrimination). The current study evaluated the measurement properties, utility, and validity of a discrimination-specific adapted measure of S&P relative to an existing, general measure among a sample of 327 minoritized youth (Mage = 18.80, SD = 1.28, 78.6% female, 50.5% Black) recruited from a large public minority-serving institution in the southeastern United States. Contrary to our hypotheses, when the item stem was changed to refer to coping specifically with discrimination, the measurement properties of a validated S&P scale (Lam et al., 2018) were worse relative to the original measure. Overall, the general S&P measure produced larger main effects and explained two times more variance in depressive symptoms than discrimination-specific S&P. Findings do not rule out the idea that context-specific measures may better characterize coping with discrimination experiences than ‘trait-like’ general coping measures. However, results highlight that small adaptations to current measures may not be sufficient and may compromise predictive validity. Coping with discrimination measurement recommendations is discussed.
{"title":"Evaluating the utility and validity of a discrimination-specific measure of shift-&-persist coping","authors":"N. Keita Christophe, Michelle Y. Martin Romero, Gabriela L. Stein","doi":"10.1002/icd.2490","DOIUrl":"10.1002/icd.2490","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Shift-&-persist (S&P) coping has been shown to buffer against the effects of discrimination on psychosocial functioning in racially and ethnically minoritized youth. However, existing measures of S&P refer broadly to coping with stress and are not specifically tailored to the type of stressor individuals are coping with (e.g., discrimination). The current study evaluated the measurement properties, utility, and validity of a discrimination-specific adapted measure of S&P relative to an existing, general measure among a sample of 327 minoritized youth (<i>M</i><sub>age</sub> = 18.80, SD = 1.28, 78.6% female, 50.5% Black) recruited from a large public minority-serving institution in the southeastern United States. Contrary to our hypotheses, when the item stem was changed to refer to coping specifically with discrimination, the measurement properties of a validated S&P scale (Lam et al., 2018) were worse relative to the original measure. Overall, the general S&P measure produced larger main effects and explained two times more variance in depressive symptoms than discrimination-specific S&P. Findings do not rule out the idea that context-specific measures may better characterize coping with discrimination experiences than ‘trait-like’ general coping measures. However, results highlight that small adaptations to current measures may not be sufficient and may compromise predictive validity. Coping with discrimination measurement recommendations is discussed.</p>","PeriodicalId":47820,"journal":{"name":"Infant and Child Development","volume":"33 3","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.8,"publicationDate":"2023-12-29","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/icd.2490","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139061267","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Reading comprehension depends on oral language competence and word reading ability, as per the simple view of reading. While this theory has been studied in Western, Educated, Industrialized, Rich and Democratic contexts, its applicability in different educational settings characterized by multilingualism, a lack of explicit reading instruction, and the challenges of poverty (as in South Africa) remains under-explored. We use secondary data from approximately 263 emergent bilinguals (approximately one-quarter isiZulu-English, three quarters Siswati-English) from the Second Early Grade Reading Study intervention. Participants completed tests of word reading fluency, vocabulary and reading comprehension in both of their school languages. Using within-language path models, we examine the effects of word reading fluency and vocabulary on reading comprehension and explore whether these relations differ by first language group or intervention group. Thus, we tease out the role of the language and educational context in influencing the skills related to reading comprehension in first language and English additional language.
阅读理解能力取决于口语能力和单词阅读能力,这是简单阅读理论的观点。虽然这一理论已在西方、教育发达、工业化、富裕和民主背景下进行过研究,但其在以多语言、缺乏明确的阅读教学和贫困挑战(如南非)为特点的不同教育环境中的适用性仍未得到充分探讨。我们使用了 "第二次低年级阅读研究"(Second Early Grade Reading Study)干预项目中约 263 名新兴双语学生(约四分之一为伊苏鲁语-英语,四分之三为锡斯瓦蒂语-英语)的二手数据。参加者用两种学校语言完成了单词阅读流利度、词汇量和阅读理解测试。通过语言内路径模型,我们研究了单词阅读流利性和词汇量对阅读理解的影响,并探讨了这些关系是否因第一语言组或干预组而有所不同。因此,我们揭示了语言和教育环境对母语和英语附加语阅读理解能力的影响。
{"title":"Examining the generalisability of the simple view of reading comprehension for emergent bilinguals","authors":"Maxine Schaefer, Nompumelelo Mohohlwane, Carien Wilsenach","doi":"10.1002/icd.2487","DOIUrl":"10.1002/icd.2487","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Reading comprehension depends on oral language competence and word reading ability, as per the simple view of reading. While this theory has been studied in Western, Educated, Industrialized, Rich and Democratic contexts, its applicability in different educational settings characterized by multilingualism, a lack of explicit reading instruction, and the challenges of poverty (as in South Africa) remains under-explored. We use secondary data from approximately 263 emergent bilinguals (approximately one-quarter isiZulu-English, three quarters Siswati-English) from the Second Early Grade Reading Study intervention. Participants completed tests of word reading fluency, vocabulary and reading comprehension in both of their school languages. Using within-language path models, we examine the effects of word reading fluency and vocabulary on reading comprehension and explore whether these relations differ by first language group or intervention group. Thus, we tease out the role of the language and educational context in influencing the skills related to reading comprehension in first language and English additional language.</p>","PeriodicalId":47820,"journal":{"name":"Infant and Child Development","volume":"33 3","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.8,"publicationDate":"2023-12-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139051043","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Object retrieval skills requiring means-end exploration are an area of relative weakness in young children with Down syndrome (DS). The current pilot study examined the object retrieval skills of a small sample of young children with DS for tasks with varying level of complexity. Thirteen children with DS (31.55 ± 4.14 months) and 13 mental age-matched typically developing (TD) children (17.31 ± 1.64 months) completed three tasks involving retrieving a toy from a transparent box (Task 1), an opaque box (Task 2) and a transparent box hidden inside an opaque box (Task 3). Both DS and TD group performed similarly for the simpler Task 1, but for Tasks 2 and 3, the DS group had a lower success rate, less frequent use of complex asymmetric hand actions and/or longer execution times compared to the TD group. Future studies should examine the developmental trends and specificity of means-end exploratory behaviours in children with DS.
{"title":"Performance during object retrieval tasks in young children with and without Down syndrome: A pilot study","authors":"Maninderjit Kaur, Amy Work Needham","doi":"10.1002/icd.2486","DOIUrl":"10.1002/icd.2486","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Object retrieval skills requiring means-end exploration are an area of relative weakness in young children with Down syndrome (DS). The current pilot study examined the object retrieval skills of a small sample of young children with DS for tasks with varying level of complexity. Thirteen children with DS (31.55 ± 4.14 months) and 13 mental age-matched typically developing (TD) children (17.31 ± 1.64 months) completed three tasks involving retrieving a toy from a transparent box (Task 1), an opaque box (Task 2) and a transparent box hidden inside an opaque box (Task 3). Both DS and TD group performed similarly for the simpler Task 1, but for Tasks 2 and 3, the DS group had a lower success rate, less frequent use of complex asymmetric hand actions and/or longer execution times compared to the TD group. Future studies should examine the developmental trends and specificity of means-end exploratory behaviours in children with DS.</p>","PeriodicalId":47820,"journal":{"name":"Infant and Child Development","volume":"33 3","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.8,"publicationDate":"2023-12-25","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139051030","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Hassana Ojonuba Shuaibu, Haliza Abdul Rahman, Nor Afiah Mohd Zulkefli
Adolescents living in peri-urban communities are more vulnerable to risky behaviours such as substance use. This was a cross-sectional study carried out to determine the prevalence, risk and protective factors of adolescent substance use in peri-urban communities in Abuja. Data on lifetime and current substance use were collected using the Youth Risk Behaviour Survey Instrument. Data on risk and protective factors of adolescent substance use, based on the Problem Behaviour Theory, were also collected. Using the SPSS software, descriptive analysis was conducted to summarise the data. Bivariate analysis was conducted using chi-square and independent t-tests, and multivariate analysis was conducted using logistic regression. A p-value of less than 0.05 was considered statistically significant. The prevalence of lifetime substance use was 22.8%, whilst current substance use was 18.5%. The consumption of mixed drinks was the most currently used form substance at 8.1%, followed by alcohol (5.7%) and shisha (4.7%). Multivariate analysis showed that self-esteem (0.007), delinquency (p < 0.001) and parent alcohol use (p = 0.045) were independently associated with adolescent substance use. There is a high prevalence of substance use amongst adolescents living in peri-urban communities in Abuja, Nigeria, as well as a new pattern of substance use consumption. The findings showed that some risk factors identified by the problem behaviour theory were independently associated with adolescent substance use, whilst the protective factors were not. There is a dire need for adolescent substance use interventions in peri-urban communities that addresses certain risk factors.
{"title":"Prevalence, risk and protective factors of substance use amongst adolescents living in peri-urban communities in Abuja, Nigeria","authors":"Hassana Ojonuba Shuaibu, Haliza Abdul Rahman, Nor Afiah Mohd Zulkefli","doi":"10.1002/icd.2485","DOIUrl":"10.1002/icd.2485","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Adolescents living in peri-urban communities are more vulnerable to risky behaviours such as substance use. This was a cross-sectional study carried out to determine the prevalence, risk and protective factors of adolescent substance use in peri-urban communities in Abuja. Data on lifetime and current substance use were collected using the Youth Risk Behaviour Survey Instrument. Data on risk and protective factors of adolescent substance use, based on the Problem Behaviour Theory, were also collected. Using the SPSS software, descriptive analysis was conducted to summarise the data. Bivariate analysis was conducted using chi-square and independent <i>t</i>-tests, and multivariate analysis was conducted using logistic regression. A <i>p</i>-value of less than 0.05 was considered statistically significant. The prevalence of lifetime substance use was 22.8%, whilst current substance use was 18.5%. The consumption of mixed drinks was the most currently used form substance at 8.1%, followed by alcohol (5.7%) and shisha (4.7%). Multivariate analysis showed that self-esteem (0.007), delinquency (<i>p</i> < 0.001) and parent alcohol use (<i>p</i> = 0.045) were independently associated with adolescent substance use. There is a high prevalence of substance use amongst adolescents living in peri-urban communities in Abuja, Nigeria, as well as a new pattern of substance use consumption. The findings showed that some risk factors identified by the problem behaviour theory were independently associated with adolescent substance use, whilst the protective factors were not. There is a dire need for adolescent substance use interventions in peri-urban communities that addresses certain risk factors.</p>","PeriodicalId":47820,"journal":{"name":"Infant and Child Development","volume":"33 3","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.8,"publicationDate":"2023-12-25","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139061243","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
May Ling D. Halim, Jessica J. Glazier, M. Anais Martinez, Adam Stanaland, Sarah E. Gaither, Yarrow Dunham, Kristin Pauker, Kristina R. Olson
Despite increasing advocacy for gender equality, gender prejudice and discrimination persist. The origins of these biases develop in early childhood, but it is less clear whether (1) children's gender attitudes predict discrimination and (2) gender attitudes and discrimination vary by ethnicity and US region. We examine these questions with an ethnically (Asian, Black, Latinx and White) and geographically (Northeast, Pacific Northwest, West, Southeast and Hawaii) diverse sample of 4- to 6-year-old children (N = 605) who completed measures of gender attitudes and discrimination in a preregistered study. Children, across groups, demonstrated more positive attitudes towards their gender ingroup. Children who showed more pro-ingroup attitudes also showed more pro-ingroup behavioural discrimination. Girls showed stronger ingroup favouritism than boys, but ethnic and regional groups generally did not vary in levels of bias. These findings contribute to our understanding of how gender intergroup biases develop and highlight the generalizability of these processes.
{"title":"Gender attitudes and gender discrimination among ethnically and geographically diverse young children","authors":"May Ling D. Halim, Jessica J. Glazier, M. Anais Martinez, Adam Stanaland, Sarah E. Gaither, Yarrow Dunham, Kristin Pauker, Kristina R. Olson","doi":"10.1002/icd.2482","DOIUrl":"10.1002/icd.2482","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Despite increasing advocacy for gender equality, gender prejudice and discrimination persist. The origins of these biases develop in early childhood, but it is less clear whether (1) children's gender attitudes predict discrimination and (2) gender attitudes and discrimination vary by ethnicity and US region. We examine these questions with an ethnically (Asian, Black, Latinx and White) and geographically (Northeast, Pacific Northwest, West, Southeast and Hawaii) diverse sample of 4- to 6-year-old children (<i>N</i> = 605) who completed measures of gender attitudes and discrimination in a preregistered study. Children, across groups, demonstrated more positive attitudes towards their gender ingroup. Children who showed more pro-ingroup attitudes also showed more pro-ingroup behavioural discrimination. Girls showed stronger ingroup favouritism than boys, but ethnic and regional groups generally did not vary in levels of bias. These findings contribute to our understanding of how gender intergroup biases develop and highlight the generalizability of these processes.</p>","PeriodicalId":47820,"journal":{"name":"Infant and Child Development","volume":"33 3","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.8,"publicationDate":"2023-12-22","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139034820","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Rosalie Odean, Carla Abad, Yvonne Ralph, Shannon M. Pruden
The current study explores whether individual differences in the dimensional adjectives (e.g., big, tall) children understand, relates to individual differences in two non-verbal spatial abilities, spatial scaling and mental transformations, in bilingual children. The inclusion of English–Spanish bilingual children broadens the work in this area which has previously focused strictly on English language. Ninety-two English–Spanish bilingual children between 37.65 and 71.87 months (42 male) participated in the study. Results show number of dimensional adjectives preschool children comprehend utilizing a new interactive, tablet-based task relates to performance on non-verbal spatial tasks. This research supports hypothesized relations between spatial language comprehension and spatial abilities, introduces an effective tool for examining spatial language comprehension in young children, and improves generalizability by including a bilingual sample and testing comprehension in both English and Spanish.
{"title":"Individual differences in preschoolers' spatial thinking: Comprehension of dimensional adjectives and their relation to children's performance on non-verbal spatial tasks","authors":"Rosalie Odean, Carla Abad, Yvonne Ralph, Shannon M. Pruden","doi":"10.1002/icd.2484","DOIUrl":"10.1002/icd.2484","url":null,"abstract":"<p>The current study explores whether individual differences in the dimensional adjectives (e.g., big, tall) children <i>understand</i>, relates to individual differences in two non-verbal spatial abilities, spatial scaling and mental transformations, in bilingual children. The inclusion of English–Spanish bilingual children broadens the work in this area which has previously focused strictly on English language. Ninety-two English–Spanish bilingual children between 37.65 and 71.87 months (42 male) participated in the study. Results show number of dimensional adjectives preschool children <i>comprehend</i> utilizing a new interactive, tablet-based task relates to performance on non-verbal spatial tasks. This research supports hypothesized relations between spatial language <i>comprehension</i> and spatial abilities, introduces an effective tool for examining spatial language comprehension in young children, and improves generalizability by including a bilingual sample and testing comprehension in both English and Spanish.</p>","PeriodicalId":47820,"journal":{"name":"Infant and Child Development","volume":"33 3","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.8,"publicationDate":"2023-12-22","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138946229","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Sarah Raza, Lori-Ann R. Sacrey, Lonnie Zwaigenbaum
Emotion regulation (ER) refers to the ability to regulate emotional reactions in response to stimuli. Social communication involves the knowledge and skills to engage in social interactions. Both processes develop in the first years of life and form the foundation for later functioning. Literature suggests a bidirectional relationship between ER and social communication; however, the majority of research examines these constructs independently. This review provides an in-depth examination of research that has measured the relations between ER and social communication in children from 2 years of age and onwards. Findings revealed an age-related pattern, where ER was related to later social communication and vice versa. However, there was no consensus regarding direct relationship(s) between ER and social communication due to heterogeneity among studies when defining and measuring these constructs. This review illustrates the importance of understanding the pathways between ER and social communication and may inform future studies, both in typical and atypical development.
{"title":"The bidirectional relationship between emotion regulation and social communication in childhood: A systematic review","authors":"Sarah Raza, Lori-Ann R. Sacrey, Lonnie Zwaigenbaum","doi":"10.1002/icd.2480","DOIUrl":"10.1002/icd.2480","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Emotion regulation (ER) refers to the ability to regulate emotional reactions in response to stimuli. Social communication involves the knowledge and skills to engage in social interactions. Both processes develop in the first years of life and form the foundation for later functioning. Literature suggests a bidirectional relationship between ER and social communication; however, the majority of research examines these constructs independently. This review provides an in-depth examination of research that has measured the relations between ER and social communication in children from 2 years of age and onwards. Findings revealed an age-related pattern, where ER was related to later social communication and vice versa. However, there was no consensus regarding direct relationship(s) between ER and social communication due to heterogeneity among studies when defining and measuring these constructs. This review illustrates the importance of understanding the pathways between ER and social communication and may inform future studies, both in typical and atypical development.</p>","PeriodicalId":47820,"journal":{"name":"Infant and Child Development","volume":"33 3","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.8,"publicationDate":"2023-12-21","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138953248","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
H. M. McAnally, E. Iosua, J. Belsky, J. L. Sligo, P. Letcher, C. J. Greenwood, E. Spry, K. C. Thomson, J. A. Macdonald, A. E. Bolton, C. A. Olsson, R. J. Hancox
The effects of academic achievement may extend beyond economic success to influence social functioning, including future parenting. To evaluate whether adolescent academic achievement forecasts future parenting (both positive and negative) and the family home environment of parents. We used prospectively gathered intergenerational data from a population-based birth cohort born in 1972/1973 in Dunedin, New Zealand, including data from Generation 1 (parents of the birth cohort), the birth cohort (Generation 2; G2), and G2's children (Generation 3). Adolescent academic achievement in G2 was used to predict observed and reported parenting outcomes when offspring (G3) were aged 3 years after controlling for a range of covariates, including G2's adolescent wellbeing, early childhood socioeconomic status (collected from G1), and G2's age at child's birth. We also evaluated 2-way interactions between academic achievement and G2 parent sex, G3 child behaviour, and G2 adolescent wellbeing. Greater G2 academic achievement, net of controls, predicted more positive and less negative parenting (for mothers only), and a more positive home environment. For the home environment outcome, the effect of adolescent academic achievement was moderated by wellbeing. Adolescent academic achievement may positively influence parenting behaviour and the quality of the home environment.
{"title":"Does adolescent academic achievement predict future parenting?","authors":"H. M. McAnally, E. Iosua, J. Belsky, J. L. Sligo, P. Letcher, C. J. Greenwood, E. Spry, K. C. Thomson, J. A. Macdonald, A. E. Bolton, C. A. Olsson, R. J. Hancox","doi":"10.1002/icd.2483","DOIUrl":"10.1002/icd.2483","url":null,"abstract":"<p>The effects of academic achievement may extend beyond economic success to influence social functioning, including future parenting. To evaluate whether adolescent academic achievement forecasts future parenting (both positive and negative) and the family home environment of parents. We used prospectively gathered intergenerational data from a population-based birth cohort born in 1972/1973 in Dunedin, New Zealand, including data from Generation 1 (parents of the birth cohort), the birth cohort (Generation 2; G2), and G2's children (Generation 3). Adolescent academic achievement in G2 was used to predict observed and reported parenting outcomes when offspring (G3) were aged 3 years after controlling for a range of covariates, including G2's adolescent wellbeing, early childhood socioeconomic status (collected from G1), and G2's age at child's birth. We also evaluated 2-way interactions between academic achievement and G2 parent sex, G3 child behaviour, and G2 adolescent wellbeing. Greater G2 academic achievement, net of controls, predicted more positive and less negative parenting (for mothers only), and a more positive home environment. For the home environment outcome, the effect of adolescent academic achievement was moderated by wellbeing. Adolescent academic achievement may positively influence parenting behaviour and the quality of the home environment.</p>","PeriodicalId":47820,"journal":{"name":"Infant and Child Development","volume":"33 3","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.8,"publicationDate":"2023-12-15","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/icd.2483","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138679138","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
When I took over as Editor of Infant and Child Development in 2020, I sought, among other things, to establish the journal as a welcoming outlet for opinions, perspectives and commentaries, as there are few outlets for such work in developmental science. I also wanted to be sure that those opinions, perspectives and commentaries were not just coming from the ‘usual suspects’, those who already held considerable status and access to power in the field.
My original intention was that the special issue would be published as the first issue of the year. As many people have noted, the 2021–2022 academic year was particularly challenging as many institutions began to ‘return to normal’ amidst the pandemic. At Infant and Child Development we seek to promote a healthy research and work culture, and thus we were quite flexible with deadlines. The result: the special issue is published as the last issue of the year rather than the first. Nevertheless, it is once again a fantastic collection of articles. Please take the time to read them.
A final note: there were three additional articles that were meant to be published as part of this special issue, but due to a production error were published in the previous issue. These papers, by Fong et al. (2023), Hendry and Scerif (2023) and Veldkamp and Kemner (2023), are just as good as all of the others, and are part of this special issue, at heart.
当我在2020年接任《婴幼儿发展》杂志的编辑时,除其他事项外,我力求将该杂志打造成一个受欢迎的发表意见、观点和评论的渠道,因为在发展科学领域,这类工作的渠道很少。我还想确保这些观点、观点和评论不是来自“通常的嫌疑人”,那些已经在该领域拥有相当地位和权力的人。我的初衷是把这期特刊作为今年的第一期出版。正如许多人所指出的那样,2021-2022学年尤其具有挑战性,因为许多机构在疫情期间开始“恢复正常”。在婴儿和儿童发展部,我们寻求促进健康的研究和工作文化,因此我们在截止日期方面相当灵活。其结果是:特刊作为当年的最后一期而不是第一期出版。尽管如此,它仍然是一本精彩的文章集。请花时间阅读。最后说明:还有三篇文章本来是要作为这期特刊的一部分发表的,但由于生产错误,在上一期上发表了。Fong et al. (2023), Hendry and Scerif (2023), Veldkamp and Kemner(2023)的这些论文和其他所有的论文一样好,本质上是本期特刊的一部分。
{"title":"Current perspectives in developmental science: Introduction to the 2023 special issue","authors":"Moin Syed","doi":"10.1002/icd.2479","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1002/icd.2479","url":null,"abstract":"<p>When I took over as Editor of <i>Infant and Child Development</i> in 2020, I sought, among other things, to establish the journal as a welcoming outlet for opinions, perspectives and commentaries, as there are few outlets for such work in developmental science. I also wanted to be sure that those opinions, perspectives and commentaries were not just coming from the ‘usual suspects’, those who already held considerable status and access to power in the field.</p><p>My original intention was that the special issue would be published as the first issue of the year. As many people have noted, the 2021–2022 academic year was particularly challenging as many institutions began to ‘return to normal’ amidst the pandemic. At <i>Infant and Child Development</i> we seek to promote a healthy research and work culture, and thus we were quite flexible with deadlines. The result: the special issue is published as the <i>last</i> issue of the year rather than the first. Nevertheless, it is once again a fantastic collection of articles. Please take the time to read them.</p><p>A final note: there were three additional articles that were meant to be published as part of this special issue, but due to a production error were published in the previous issue. These papers, by Fong et al. (<span>2023</span>), Hendry and Scerif (<span>2023</span>) and Veldkamp and Kemner (<span>2023</span>), are just as good as all of the others, and are part of this special issue, at heart.</p>","PeriodicalId":47820,"journal":{"name":"Infant and Child Development","volume":"32 6","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2023-12-15","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/icd.2479","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138678859","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
LaTasha R. Holden, Gabriel J. Tanenbaum, Austin E. Ashley
Students racialized as Black often experience forms of marginalization and encounter unique obstacles in their educational paths. As a result, more inclusive and tailored forms of support should be developed. Working to better support Black students should be initiated through two complementary sides—through that of the instructor and the scientist. First, we consider the importance of identity-safety and detail identity-based barriers Black students experience in their learning environments. We then define the notion of cognitive universals and discuss how it limits the effectiveness of science and its translation to more inclusive practices. We argue that to strengthen tailored forms of student support, we must move beyond focusing only on cognitive universals and more carefully consider heterogeneity of effects. To support our argument, we present evidence-based strategies from cognitive and learning science and educational interventions research to combat identity-based barriers and boost learning gains. Overall, we urge instructors and scientists to consider a variety of techniques to create more identity-safe and inclusive learning environments for Black students.
{"title":"Toward inclusive and identity safe learning for supporting racialized student achievement","authors":"LaTasha R. Holden, Gabriel J. Tanenbaum, Austin E. Ashley","doi":"10.1002/icd.2477","DOIUrl":"10.1002/icd.2477","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Students racialized as Black often experience forms of marginalization and encounter unique obstacles in their educational paths. As a result, more inclusive and tailored forms of support should be developed. Working to better support Black students should be initiated through two complementary sides—through that of the instructor and the scientist. First, we consider the importance of identity-safety and detail identity-based barriers Black students experience in their learning environments. We then define the notion of cognitive universals and discuss how it limits the effectiveness of science and its translation to more inclusive practices. We argue that to strengthen tailored forms of student support, we must move beyond focusing only on cognitive universals and more carefully consider heterogeneity of effects. To support our argument, we present evidence-based strategies from cognitive and learning science and educational interventions research to combat identity-based barriers and boost learning gains. Overall, we urge instructors and scientists to consider a variety of techniques to create more identity-safe and inclusive learning environments for Black students.</p>","PeriodicalId":47820,"journal":{"name":"Infant and Child Development","volume":"32 6","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2023-11-27","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/icd.2477","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138455822","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}