首页 > 最新文献

European Journal of Psychological Assessment最新文献

英文 中文
Who Is Satisfied With Effort? 谁对努力感到满意?
IF 2.5 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, APPLIED Pub Date : 2022-11-01 DOI: 10.1027/1015-5759/a000742
Georgia Clay, C. Dumitrescu, Janina Habenicht, Isabel Kmiecik, Marzia Musetti, I. Domachowska
Abstract. The effort required to obtain certain rewards may influence the level of satisfaction with the following reward. Since people differ in beliefs about the availability of willpower resources required to pursue effortful actions, we investigated how willpower beliefs affect the perception of effort and satisfaction with reward. We hypothesized that people with limited willpower beliefs (i.e., believing that exerting effort leads to depletion of their inner resources) will perceive cognitive tasks as more effortful and will be less satisfied with the subsequent reward than those with non-limited beliefs (i.e., believing that exerting effort is invigorating rather than depleting). We tested this hypothesis by manipulating effort with different difficulty levels of the N-back task and measuring participants’ perception of effort expenditure and subjective satisfaction with a reward depending on their willpower beliefs. In accordance with the predictions, we found that those with limited willpower beliefs perceived the task as more effortful than those with non-limited willpower beliefs. Furthermore, when asked to subjectively rate their satisfaction with the reward gained for the task, limited believers rated their satisfaction lower than non-limited believers. These findings suggest that people take their willpower capacities into effort-satisfaction calculations. Results are discussed within the context of other models of effort, and practical implications of the findings are suggested.
摘要获得某种奖励所需的努力可能会影响对下一个奖励的满意程度。由于人们对努力行动所需的意志力资源的可得性有不同的看法,我们研究了意志力信念如何影响努力和回报满意度的感知。我们假设拥有有限意志力信念的人(即,相信付出努力会耗尽他们的内在资源)会认为认知任务更费力,并且对随后的奖励更不满意,而不是那些没有有限信念的人(即,相信付出努力会使人精力充沛而不是消耗精力)。我们通过操纵不同难度的N-back任务的努力程度来验证这一假设,并测量了参与者对努力支出的感知和对奖励的主观满意度,这取决于他们的意志力信念。与预测一致,我们发现那些意志力有限的人比那些意志力不有限的人更容易完成任务。此外,当被要求主观评价他们对任务所获得的奖励的满意度时,有限信徒的满意度低于非有限信徒。这些发现表明,人们将自己的意志力能力纳入了努力满意度的计算中。在其他努力模型的背景下讨论了结果,并提出了研究结果的实际意义。
{"title":"Who Is Satisfied With Effort?","authors":"Georgia Clay, C. Dumitrescu, Janina Habenicht, Isabel Kmiecik, Marzia Musetti, I. Domachowska","doi":"10.1027/1015-5759/a000742","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1027/1015-5759/a000742","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract. The effort required to obtain certain rewards may influence the level of satisfaction with the following reward. Since people differ in beliefs about the availability of willpower resources required to pursue effortful actions, we investigated how willpower beliefs affect the perception of effort and satisfaction with reward. We hypothesized that people with limited willpower beliefs (i.e., believing that exerting effort leads to depletion of their inner resources) will perceive cognitive tasks as more effortful and will be less satisfied with the subsequent reward than those with non-limited beliefs (i.e., believing that exerting effort is invigorating rather than depleting). We tested this hypothesis by manipulating effort with different difficulty levels of the N-back task and measuring participants’ perception of effort expenditure and subjective satisfaction with a reward depending on their willpower beliefs. In accordance with the predictions, we found that those with limited willpower beliefs perceived the task as more effortful than those with non-limited willpower beliefs. Furthermore, when asked to subjectively rate their satisfaction with the reward gained for the task, limited believers rated their satisfaction lower than non-limited believers. These findings suggest that people take their willpower capacities into effort-satisfaction calculations. Results are discussed within the context of other models of effort, and practical implications of the findings are suggested.","PeriodicalId":48018,"journal":{"name":"European Journal of Psychological Assessment","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2022-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"44298066","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Measuring Growth Mindset 衡量成长心态
IF 2.5 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, APPLIED Pub Date : 2022-09-29 DOI: 10.1027/1015-5759/a000735
Beatrice Rammstedt, D. Grüning, Clemens M. Lechner
Abstract. A growth mindset is a belief that personal characteristics, specifically intellectual ability, are malleable and can be developed by investing time and effort. Numerous studies have investigated the associations between a growth mindset and academic achievement, and large intervention programs have been established to train adolescents to develop a stronger growth mindset. However, methodological research on the adequacy of the measures used to assess a growth mindset is scarce. In our study, we conducted one of the first comprehensive assessments of the psychometric properties of Dweck’s widely used three-item Growth Mindset Scale in two samples (adolescents aged 14–19 years and adults aged 20–64 years). We test the comparability (i.e., measurement invariance) of the scale across these age groups. Furthermore, using the same two samples, we identified and validated a single-item measure to assess growth mindset in settings with severe time constraints. Results reveal that both the three-item and the single-item scales have acceptable psychometric properties regarding reliability, comparability, and validity. However, the results did not support some of the central tenets of mindset theory, such as that a growth mindset is positively linked to goal regulation and achievement, calling for future research on the criterion validity of a growth mindset.
摘要成长心态是一种信念,即个人特征,特别是智力,是可塑的,可以通过投入时间和精力来发展。许多研究调查了成长心态与学业成绩之间的关系,并建立了大型干预计划来训练青少年培养更强的成长心态。然而,关于用于评估增长心态的措施是否充分的方法研究很少。在我们的研究中,我们在两个样本(14-19岁的青少年和20-64岁的成年人)中对Dweck广泛使用的三项成长心态量表的心理测量特性进行了首次全面评估。我们测试了这些年龄组的量表的可比性(即测量不变性)。此外,使用相同的两个样本,我们确定并验证了一个单项指标,以评估在时间限制严重的环境中的成长心态。结果表明,三项量表和单项量表在信度、可比性和有效性方面都具有可接受的心理测量特性。然而,研究结果并不支持心态理论的一些核心原则,例如成长心态与目标调节和成就呈正相关,这呼吁未来对成长心态的标准有效性进行研究。
{"title":"Measuring Growth Mindset","authors":"Beatrice Rammstedt, D. Grüning, Clemens M. Lechner","doi":"10.1027/1015-5759/a000735","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1027/1015-5759/a000735","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract. A growth mindset is a belief that personal characteristics, specifically intellectual ability, are malleable and can be developed by investing time and effort. Numerous studies have investigated the associations between a growth mindset and academic achievement, and large intervention programs have been established to train adolescents to develop a stronger growth mindset. However, methodological research on the adequacy of the measures used to assess a growth mindset is scarce. In our study, we conducted one of the first comprehensive assessments of the psychometric properties of Dweck’s widely used three-item Growth Mindset Scale in two samples (adolescents aged 14–19 years and adults aged 20–64 years). We test the comparability (i.e., measurement invariance) of the scale across these age groups. Furthermore, using the same two samples, we identified and validated a single-item measure to assess growth mindset in settings with severe time constraints. Results reveal that both the three-item and the single-item scales have acceptable psychometric properties regarding reliability, comparability, and validity. However, the results did not support some of the central tenets of mindset theory, such as that a growth mindset is positively linked to goal regulation and achievement, calling for future research on the criterion validity of a growth mindset.","PeriodicalId":48018,"journal":{"name":"European Journal of Psychological Assessment","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2022-09-29","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"49264770","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 3
Validation of the Dutch Version of the Plymouth Sensory Imagery Questionnaire 荷兰版普利茅斯感觉意象问卷的验证
IF 2.5 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, APPLIED Pub Date : 2022-09-29 DOI: 10.1027/1015-5759/a000729
Mandy Woelk, M. Hagenaars, J. Krans
Abstract. Mental imagery plays an important role in the onset and maintenance of psychological disorders as well as their treatment. Therefore, a reliable and valid measure of mental imagery is essential. Andrade and colleagues (2014) developed the Plymouth Sensory Imagery Questionnaire (PsiQ), which contains 35 items (long version) or 21 items (shortened version) measuring the vividness of mental imagery in seven different modalities: vision, sound, smell, taste, touch, bodily sensation, and emotion. Andrade et al. reported a seven-factor structure corresponding to the different modalities for both versions rather than a one-factor model measuring general mental imagery. The current paper reports on the translation and validation of the Dutch version of the PsiQ (PsiQ-NL-35 and PsiQ-NL-21). In two independent samples (student and mixed), the PsiQ-NL-35 showed excellent internal consistency, adequate model fit for the seven-factor model, and a poor fit for the one-factor model. Test-retest reliability (Study 1, student sample) was good. Construct validity (Study 2, mixed sample) was adequate. The PsiQ-NL-21 also showed excellent internal consistency, good test-retest reliability, adequate seven-factor model fit, and adequate construct validity. Measurement invariance between the Dutch and the English version was found, implying that both versions measure the same construct.
摘要心理意象在心理障碍的发生和维持及其治疗中起着重要作用。因此,一个可靠和有效的心理意象测量是必不可少的。Andrade及其同事(2014)开发了普利茅斯感觉意象问卷(PsiQ),该问卷包含35个项目(长版本)或21个项目(短版本),测量了七种不同形式的心理意象的生动性:视觉、声音、嗅觉、味觉、触觉、身体感觉和情感。Andrade等人报告了一个七因素结构,对应于两种版本的不同模式,而不是测量一般心理意象的单因素模型。本文报道了荷兰语版本PsiQ (PsiQ- nl -35和PsiQ- nl -21)的翻译和验证。在两个独立样本(学生和混合)中,PsiQ-NL-35表现出良好的内部一致性,对七因素模型的模型拟合足够,对单因素模型的拟合较差。重测信度(研究1,学生样本)良好。结构效度(研究2,混合样本)足够。PsiQ-NL-21具有良好的内部一致性、良好的重测信度、良好的七因素模型拟合和良好的结构效度。荷兰语和英语版本之间的测量不变性被发现,这意味着两个版本测量相同的结构。
{"title":"Validation of the Dutch Version of the Plymouth Sensory Imagery Questionnaire","authors":"Mandy Woelk, M. Hagenaars, J. Krans","doi":"10.1027/1015-5759/a000729","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1027/1015-5759/a000729","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract. Mental imagery plays an important role in the onset and maintenance of psychological disorders as well as their treatment. Therefore, a reliable and valid measure of mental imagery is essential. Andrade and colleagues (2014) developed the Plymouth Sensory Imagery Questionnaire (PsiQ), which contains 35 items (long version) or 21 items (shortened version) measuring the vividness of mental imagery in seven different modalities: vision, sound, smell, taste, touch, bodily sensation, and emotion. Andrade et al. reported a seven-factor structure corresponding to the different modalities for both versions rather than a one-factor model measuring general mental imagery. The current paper reports on the translation and validation of the Dutch version of the PsiQ (PsiQ-NL-35 and PsiQ-NL-21). In two independent samples (student and mixed), the PsiQ-NL-35 showed excellent internal consistency, adequate model fit for the seven-factor model, and a poor fit for the one-factor model. Test-retest reliability (Study 1, student sample) was good. Construct validity (Study 2, mixed sample) was adequate. The PsiQ-NL-21 also showed excellent internal consistency, good test-retest reliability, adequate seven-factor model fit, and adequate construct validity. Measurement invariance between the Dutch and the English version was found, implying that both versions measure the same construct.","PeriodicalId":48018,"journal":{"name":"European Journal of Psychological Assessment","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2022-09-29","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"42846769","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
The Impact of Different Methods to Correct for Response Styles on the External Validity of Self-Reports 不同答题方式对自我报告外部效度的影响
IF 2.5 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, APPLIED Pub Date : 2022-09-29 DOI: 10.1027/1015-5759/a000731
A. Scharl, Timo Gnambs
Abstract. Response styles (RSs) such as acquiescence represent systematic respondent behaviors in self-report questionnaires beyond the actual item content. They distort trait estimates and contribute to measurement bias in questionnaire-based research. Although various approaches were proposed to correct the influence of RSs, little is known about their relative performance. Because different correction methods formalize the latent traits differently, it is unclear how model choice affects the external validity of the corrected measures. Therefore, the present study on N = 1,000 Dutch respondents investigated the impact of correcting responses to measures of self-esteem and the need for cognition using structural equation models with structured residuals, multidimensional generalized partial credit models, and multinomial processing trees. The study considered three RSs: extreme, midpoint, and acquiescence RS. The results showed homogeneous correlation patterns among the modeled latent and external variables, especially if they were not themselves subject to RSs. In that case, the IRT-based models, including an uncorrected model, still yielded consistent results. Nevertheless, the strength of the effect sizes showed variation.
摘要默认等回应风格代表了自我报告问卷中超出实际项目内容的系统性被调查者行为。它们扭曲了对特质的估计,并在基于问卷的研究中造成了测量偏差。虽然提出了各种方法来纠正RSs的影响,但对它们的相对性能知之甚少。由于不同的校正方法对潜在特征的形式化不同,模型选择如何影响校正措施的外部效度尚不清楚。因此,本研究采用结构化残差结构方程模型、多维广义部分信用模型和多项处理树来研究纠正反应对自尊和认知需求的影响。该研究考虑了三种RSs:极端RSs、中点RSs和默认RSs。结果显示,建模的潜在变量和外部变量之间存在均匀的相关模式,特别是当它们本身不受RSs影响时。在这种情况下,基于红外光谱的模型,包括一个未修正的模型,仍然产生一致的结果。然而,效应量的强度表现出变化。
{"title":"The Impact of Different Methods to Correct for Response Styles on the External Validity of Self-Reports","authors":"A. Scharl, Timo Gnambs","doi":"10.1027/1015-5759/a000731","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1027/1015-5759/a000731","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract. Response styles (RSs) such as acquiescence represent systematic respondent behaviors in self-report questionnaires beyond the actual item content. They distort trait estimates and contribute to measurement bias in questionnaire-based research. Although various approaches were proposed to correct the influence of RSs, little is known about their relative performance. Because different correction methods formalize the latent traits differently, it is unclear how model choice affects the external validity of the corrected measures. Therefore, the present study on N = 1,000 Dutch respondents investigated the impact of correcting responses to measures of self-esteem and the need for cognition using structural equation models with structured residuals, multidimensional generalized partial credit models, and multinomial processing trees. The study considered three RSs: extreme, midpoint, and acquiescence RS. The results showed homogeneous correlation patterns among the modeled latent and external variables, especially if they were not themselves subject to RSs. In that case, the IRT-based models, including an uncorrected model, still yielded consistent results. Nevertheless, the strength of the effect sizes showed variation.","PeriodicalId":48018,"journal":{"name":"European Journal of Psychological Assessment","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2022-09-29","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41438191","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Receptivity to Instructional Feedback 对教学反馈的接受能力
IF 2.5 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, APPLIED Pub Date : 2022-09-29 DOI: 10.1027/1015-5759/a000733
A. Lipnevich, Carolina Lopera-Oquendo
Abstract. The purpose of this study was to report validity evidence for the instrument intended to measure receptivity to instructional feedback in a sample of secondary school students from Singapore ( N = 314). We tested a nested hierarchy of hypotheses for addressing the cross-group (i.e., gender) invariance and compared means on the receptivity to feedback subscales between gender groups. We also examined whether receptivity to feedback predicted student grades. The four-factor hypothesized model comprising experiential attitudes, instrumental attitudes, cognitive engagement, and behavioral engagement with feedback had a good model fit. Multi-group confirmatory factor analysis supported configural, metric, partial scalar, partial strict as well as variance and covariance invariance across gender groups. After controlling for gender, cognitive engagement, and experiential attitudes predicted increments in grades, suggesting evidence for discriminant validity among the receptivity factors as well as their relevance for the prediction of meaningful educational outcomes.
摘要本研究的目的是报告新加坡中学生样本(N = 314)中用于测量教学反馈接受度的工具的效度证据。我们测试了解决跨群体(即性别)不变性的假设嵌套层次结构,并比较了性别群体对反馈子量表的接受程度。我们还研究了对反馈的接受程度是否能预测学生的成绩。由经验态度、工具态度、认知参与和反馈行为参与组成的四因素假设模型具有良好的模型拟合性。多组验证性因子分析支持构型、度量、部分标量、部分严格以及跨性别群体的方差和协方差不变性。在控制了性别之后,认知参与和经验态度预测了成绩的增加,这表明在接受性因素之间存在区别效度的证据,以及它们与预测有意义的教育成果的相关性。
{"title":"Receptivity to Instructional Feedback","authors":"A. Lipnevich, Carolina Lopera-Oquendo","doi":"10.1027/1015-5759/a000733","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1027/1015-5759/a000733","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract. The purpose of this study was to report validity evidence for the instrument intended to measure receptivity to instructional feedback in a sample of secondary school students from Singapore ( N = 314). We tested a nested hierarchy of hypotheses for addressing the cross-group (i.e., gender) invariance and compared means on the receptivity to feedback subscales between gender groups. We also examined whether receptivity to feedback predicted student grades. The four-factor hypothesized model comprising experiential attitudes, instrumental attitudes, cognitive engagement, and behavioral engagement with feedback had a good model fit. Multi-group confirmatory factor analysis supported configural, metric, partial scalar, partial strict as well as variance and covariance invariance across gender groups. After controlling for gender, cognitive engagement, and experiential attitudes predicted increments in grades, suggesting evidence for discriminant validity among the receptivity factors as well as their relevance for the prediction of meaningful educational outcomes.","PeriodicalId":48018,"journal":{"name":"European Journal of Psychological Assessment","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2022-09-29","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"46036333","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Emotional Intelligence as a Personality State 情商是一种人格状态
IF 2.5 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, APPLIED Pub Date : 2022-09-29 DOI: 10.1027/1015-5759/a000734
Leonidas A. Zampetakis, E. M. Mitropoulou
Abstract. Contemporary research has begun to explore the notion that emotional intelligence (EI) has an important state component in addition to the trait component, as represented in the whole trait theory. This implies that state EI (or enacted EI) has similar cognitive, affective, and motivational contents as its corresponding trait. The question, however, of whether a trait EI construct means the same across the individual (trait) and state levels of analysis has not been empirically investigated. To address this gap, the present study examines the assessment of enacted EI, using the full version of the Wong and Law Emotional Intelligence Scale (WLEIS) on both between-person and within-person levels of analysis. Participants were 493 Greek employees who completed the WLEIS for 5 consecutive workdays. Multilevel confirmatory factor analyses confirmed that the original four-factor multilevel model appeared to best fit the data. Multilevel measurement invariance analysis supported the equivalence of the measure across different levels of analysis. In conclusion, the WLEIS is a configural cluster construct, believed to be a valuable and reliable tool for assessing enacted EI within the workplace. Implications for future research on enacted EI are discussed.
摘要当代研究已经开始探索情绪智力(EI)除了特质成分外,还有一个重要的状态成分,这在整个特质理论中都有体现。这意味着状态EI(或制定EI)与其相应的特征具有相似的认知、情感和动机内容。然而,一个特质的EI结构在个体(特质)和状态水平的分析中是否意味着相同的问题尚未得到实证调查。为了解决这一差距,本研究使用完整版的Wong和Law情绪智力量表(WLEIS)在人与人之间和人与人之间的分析水平上对制定的EI进行了评估。参与者是493名希腊员工,他们完成了连续5个工作日的WLEIS。多层验证性因子分析证实,原来的四因素多层模型似乎最适合的数据。多水平测量不变性分析支持测量在不同分析水平上的等价性。总之,WLEIS是一个配置集群结构,被认为是评估工作场所制定的EI的有价值和可靠的工具。最后讨论了未来研究的启示。
{"title":"Emotional Intelligence as a Personality State","authors":"Leonidas A. Zampetakis, E. M. Mitropoulou","doi":"10.1027/1015-5759/a000734","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1027/1015-5759/a000734","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract. Contemporary research has begun to explore the notion that emotional intelligence (EI) has an important state component in addition to the trait component, as represented in the whole trait theory. This implies that state EI (or enacted EI) has similar cognitive, affective, and motivational contents as its corresponding trait. The question, however, of whether a trait EI construct means the same across the individual (trait) and state levels of analysis has not been empirically investigated. To address this gap, the present study examines the assessment of enacted EI, using the full version of the Wong and Law Emotional Intelligence Scale (WLEIS) on both between-person and within-person levels of analysis. Participants were 493 Greek employees who completed the WLEIS for 5 consecutive workdays. Multilevel confirmatory factor analyses confirmed that the original four-factor multilevel model appeared to best fit the data. Multilevel measurement invariance analysis supported the equivalence of the measure across different levels of analysis. In conclusion, the WLEIS is a configural cluster construct, believed to be a valuable and reliable tool for assessing enacted EI within the workplace. Implications for future research on enacted EI are discussed.","PeriodicalId":48018,"journal":{"name":"European Journal of Psychological Assessment","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2022-09-29","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41485099","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Delaying Academic Tasks and Feeling Bad About It 拖延学术任务并为此感到难过
IF 2.5 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, APPLIED Pub Date : 2022-09-13 DOI: 10.1027/1015-5759/a000728
Julia Bobe, Theresa Schnettler, Anne Scheunemann, Stefan Fries, L. Bäulke, Daniel O. Thies, M. Dresel, D. Leutner, Joachim Wirth, Katrin B. Klingsieck, C. Grunschel
Abstract. Procrastination is the irrational delay of an intended task and is common among students. A delay can only be defined as procrastination when it is voluntary, the action was intended but not implemented, and the delay is accompanied by subjective discomfort. Established scales of procrastination cover mainly behavioral aspects but have neglected the emotional aspect. This inaccuracy concerning the construct validity might entail misconceptions of procrastination. Accordingly, we developed and validated the Behavioral and Emotional Academic Procrastination Scale (BEPS), which covers all aspects of the definition of procrastination. The 6-item scale measuring self-reported academic procrastination was tested in three studies. Study 1 ( N = 239) evaluated the psychometric qualities of the BEPS, indicating good item characteristics and internal consistency. Study 2 ( N = 1,441) used confirmatory factor analysis and revealed two correlated factors: one covering the behavioral aspect and the other reflecting the emotional aspect. Measurement invariance was shown through longitudinal and multigroup confirmatory factor analyses. Study 3 ( N = 234) provided evidence for the scale’s convergent validity through correlations with established procrastination scales, self-efficacy, and neuroticism. The BEPS thus economically operationalizes all characteristics of academic procrastination and appears to be a reliable and valid self-report measure.
摘要拖延症是指不合理地拖延预定的任务,在学生中很常见。延迟只能被定义为拖延,当它是自愿的,行动是有意的,但没有实施,延迟伴随着主观的不适。现有的拖延量表主要涵盖行为方面,而忽略了情绪方面。这种关于构念效度的不准确可能导致对拖延症的误解。因此,我们开发并验证了行为与情感学业拖延量表(BEPS),该量表涵盖了拖延症定义的各个方面。在三项研究中测试了自我报告的学业拖延症的六项量表。研究1 (N = 239)评估了BEPS的心理测量质量,表明项目特征和内部一致性良好。研究2 (N = 1441)采用验证性因子分析,揭示了两个相关因素:一个覆盖行为方面,另一个反映情绪方面。通过纵向和多组验证性因子分析显示测量不变性。研究3 (N = 234)通过与现有拖延量表、自我效能和神经质的相关性,为该量表的收敛效度提供了证据。因此,BEPS经济地操作了学业拖延的所有特征,似乎是一个可靠和有效的自我报告测量。
{"title":"Delaying Academic Tasks and Feeling Bad About It","authors":"Julia Bobe, Theresa Schnettler, Anne Scheunemann, Stefan Fries, L. Bäulke, Daniel O. Thies, M. Dresel, D. Leutner, Joachim Wirth, Katrin B. Klingsieck, C. Grunschel","doi":"10.1027/1015-5759/a000728","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1027/1015-5759/a000728","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract. Procrastination is the irrational delay of an intended task and is common among students. A delay can only be defined as procrastination when it is voluntary, the action was intended but not implemented, and the delay is accompanied by subjective discomfort. Established scales of procrastination cover mainly behavioral aspects but have neglected the emotional aspect. This inaccuracy concerning the construct validity might entail misconceptions of procrastination. Accordingly, we developed and validated the Behavioral and Emotional Academic Procrastination Scale (BEPS), which covers all aspects of the definition of procrastination. The 6-item scale measuring self-reported academic procrastination was tested in three studies. Study 1 ( N = 239) evaluated the psychometric qualities of the BEPS, indicating good item characteristics and internal consistency. Study 2 ( N = 1,441) used confirmatory factor analysis and revealed two correlated factors: one covering the behavioral aspect and the other reflecting the emotional aspect. Measurement invariance was shown through longitudinal and multigroup confirmatory factor analyses. Study 3 ( N = 234) provided evidence for the scale’s convergent validity through correlations with established procrastination scales, self-efficacy, and neuroticism. The BEPS thus economically operationalizes all characteristics of academic procrastination and appears to be a reliable and valid self-report measure.","PeriodicalId":48018,"journal":{"name":"European Journal of Psychological Assessment","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2022-09-13","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"44444759","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Evaluating Gender Differences in Problematic Smartphone Use 评估有问题的智能手机使用中的性别差异
IF 2.5 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, APPLIED Pub Date : 2022-09-13 DOI: 10.1027/1015-5759/a000730
Laura Salerno, Analyn Alquitran, Noor Alibrahim, G. Lo Coco, M. Di Blasi, C. Giordano
Abstract. The Smartphone Addiction Inventory (SPAI) is widely used to measure problematic smartphone use (PSU). Although the SPAI has been translated and validated in different countries, its measurement invariance across gender has received little research attention. This study aimed to examine whether men and women interpreted the Italian version of the SPAI (SPAI-I) similarly and, consequently, whether the observed gender differences in SPAI scores, which have been shown in previous studies, could be due to true differences, rather than to differences in measurement. Six hundred nineteen Italian young adults ( Mage = 22.02 ± 2.63; 55.7% women) took part in the study and completed the SPAI-I. Multigroup CFA was applied to test the measurement invariance across gender, and the item parameter invariance was investigated with the item-response theory (IRT) differential item functioning (DIF) method for multidimensional models. Evidence of measurement invariance across gender was found. Only one item (i.e., item 14, “The idea of using smartphone comes as the first thought on mind when waking up each morning”) out of 24 items of the SPAI-I showed DIF with a large effect size. Gender-related differences found with the SPAI-I reflect true differences in smartphone overuse rather than specific characteristics of the measure.
摘要智能手机成瘾量表(SPAI)被广泛用于衡量有问题的智能手机使用情况(PSU)。尽管SPAI已在不同国家得到翻译和验证,但其跨性别的测量不变性很少受到研究关注。本研究旨在检验男性和女性对意大利版SPAI(SPAI-I)的解释是否相似,因此,先前研究中显示的SPAI评分中观察到的性别差异是否可能是由于真正的差异,而不是测量上的差异。619名意大利年轻人(Mage=22.02±2.63;55.7%的女性)参加了这项研究并完成了SPAI-I。应用多组CFA测试跨性别的测量不变性,并用项目反应理论(IRT)微分项目功能(DIF)方法研究多维模型的项目参数不变性。发现了跨性别测量不变性的证据。在SPAI-i的24个项目中,只有一个项目(即第14项,“每天早上醒来时第一个想到的是使用智能手机”)显示了具有较大效果大小的DIF。SPAI-I发现的与性别相关的差异反映了智能手机过度使用的真实差异,而不是该测量的具体特征。
{"title":"Evaluating Gender Differences in Problematic Smartphone Use","authors":"Laura Salerno, Analyn Alquitran, Noor Alibrahim, G. Lo Coco, M. Di Blasi, C. Giordano","doi":"10.1027/1015-5759/a000730","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1027/1015-5759/a000730","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract. The Smartphone Addiction Inventory (SPAI) is widely used to measure problematic smartphone use (PSU). Although the SPAI has been translated and validated in different countries, its measurement invariance across gender has received little research attention. This study aimed to examine whether men and women interpreted the Italian version of the SPAI (SPAI-I) similarly and, consequently, whether the observed gender differences in SPAI scores, which have been shown in previous studies, could be due to true differences, rather than to differences in measurement. Six hundred nineteen Italian young adults ( Mage = 22.02 ± 2.63; 55.7% women) took part in the study and completed the SPAI-I. Multigroup CFA was applied to test the measurement invariance across gender, and the item parameter invariance was investigated with the item-response theory (IRT) differential item functioning (DIF) method for multidimensional models. Evidence of measurement invariance across gender was found. Only one item (i.e., item 14, “The idea of using smartphone comes as the first thought on mind when waking up each morning”) out of 24 items of the SPAI-I showed DIF with a large effect size. Gender-related differences found with the SPAI-I reflect true differences in smartphone overuse rather than specific characteristics of the measure.","PeriodicalId":48018,"journal":{"name":"European Journal of Psychological Assessment","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2022-09-13","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41683442","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Meta-Analysis of Factor Analyses of the General Health Questionnaire – Short Forms GHQ-28 and GHQ-30 一般健康问卷- GHQ-28和GHQ-30简表因子分析的元分析
IF 2.5 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, APPLIED Pub Date : 2022-09-13 DOI: 10.1027/1015-5759/a000727
Alan B. Shafer
Abstract. Two meta-analyses of exploratory factor analyses of the General Health Questionnaire short forms, GHQ-28 ( N = 26,848, k = 40) and GHQ-30 ( N = 43,151  k = 25), were conducted to determine the consistent factors found in each test and any common factors across them. Five databases (PsycINFO, PubMed, BASE, Semantic, and Google Scholar) were searched in 2021. Reproduced correlations derived from the original studies’ factor matrices and aggregated across studies were factor analyzed for the meta-analyses. For the GHQ-28, the standard four subscales of somatic, anxiety, social dysfunction, and depression were clearly identified and strongly supported by a four-factor structure. For the GHQ-30, a four-factor solution identified factors of anxiety, depression, social dysfunction, and social satisfaction, the first three factors shared a number of items with the same scales found in the GHQ-28. These shared factors appear similar across tests and should help bridge research using the GHQ-30 and the GHQ-28. Confirmatory factor analyses supported the four-factor models in both tests. The four standard subscales of GHQ-28 were strongly supported and can be recommended. The three similar factors in the GHQ-30, as well as the social satisfaction factor, appear reasonable to use.
摘要对GHQ-28 (N = 26,848, k = 40)和GHQ-30 (N = 43,151, k = 25)的探索性因子分析进行了两项荟萃分析,以确定每个测试中发现的一致性因素以及它们之间的共同因素。五个数据库(PsycINFO、PubMed、BASE、Semantic和谷歌Scholar)在2021年被检索。从原始研究的因素矩阵中得出的再现相关性和跨研究汇总的相关性在meta分析中进行了因素分析。在GHQ-28中,躯体、焦虑、社交功能障碍和抑郁四个标准分量表被清晰地识别出来,并得到四因素结构的有力支持。对于GHQ-30,一个四因素解决方案确定了焦虑,抑郁,社交功能障碍和社交满意度的因素,前三个因素与GHQ-28中发现的相同量表共享许多项目。这些共同的因素在测试中似乎相似,应该有助于使用GHQ-30和GHQ-28进行研究。验证性因子分析支持两项测试的四因素模型。GHQ-28的四个标准分量表得到了强有力的支持和推荐。GHQ-30中的三个相似因子,以及社会满意度因子,似乎是合理的。
{"title":"Meta-Analysis of Factor Analyses of the General Health Questionnaire – Short Forms GHQ-28 and GHQ-30","authors":"Alan B. Shafer","doi":"10.1027/1015-5759/a000727","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1027/1015-5759/a000727","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract. Two meta-analyses of exploratory factor analyses of the General Health Questionnaire short forms, GHQ-28 ( N = 26,848, k = 40) and GHQ-30 ( N = 43,151  k = 25), were conducted to determine the consistent factors found in each test and any common factors across them. Five databases (PsycINFO, PubMed, BASE, Semantic, and Google Scholar) were searched in 2021. Reproduced correlations derived from the original studies’ factor matrices and aggregated across studies were factor analyzed for the meta-analyses. For the GHQ-28, the standard four subscales of somatic, anxiety, social dysfunction, and depression were clearly identified and strongly supported by a four-factor structure. For the GHQ-30, a four-factor solution identified factors of anxiety, depression, social dysfunction, and social satisfaction, the first three factors shared a number of items with the same scales found in the GHQ-28. These shared factors appear similar across tests and should help bridge research using the GHQ-30 and the GHQ-28. Confirmatory factor analyses supported the four-factor models in both tests. The four standard subscales of GHQ-28 were strongly supported and can be recommended. The three similar factors in the GHQ-30, as well as the social satisfaction factor, appear reasonable to use.","PeriodicalId":48018,"journal":{"name":"European Journal of Psychological Assessment","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2022-09-13","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"46321258","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Can Psychological Assessment Contribute to a Better World? 心理评估能为更美好的世界做出贡献吗?
IF 2.5 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, APPLIED Pub Date : 2022-09-01 DOI: 10.1027/1015-5759/a000739
D. Gallardo-Pujol, M. Ziegler, D. Iliescu
{"title":"Can Psychological Assessment Contribute to a Better World?","authors":"D. Gallardo-Pujol, M. Ziegler, D. Iliescu","doi":"10.1027/1015-5759/a000739","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1027/1015-5759/a000739","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":48018,"journal":{"name":"European Journal of Psychological Assessment","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2022-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"46432901","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
期刊
European Journal of Psychological Assessment
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1