首页 > 最新文献

Cognition最新文献

英文 中文
Partisan language in a polarized world: In-group language provides reputational benefits to speakers while polarizing audiences 两极化世界中的党派语言:群体内语言在分化受众的同时,也为发言者带来了声誉上的好处。
IF 2.8 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-11-21 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2024.106012
Alexander C. Walker , Jonathan A. Fugelsang , Derek J. Koehler
We examine the impact of partisan language (i.e., language that describes events in a manner that supports a political agenda), both with regard to peoples' perceptions of the speakers who use it and their evaluations of the events it is used to describe. In two experiments, we recruited 1121 Democrats and Republicans from the United States. Using a set of liberal-biased (e.g., expand voting rights) and conservative-biased (e.g., reduce election security) terms, we find that partisans judge speakers describing polarizing events using ideologically-congruent language as more trustworthy than those describing events in a non-partisan way (e.g., expand mail-in voting). However, when presented to rival partisans, ideologically-biased language promoted negative evaluations of opposing partisans, with speakers attributed out-group language being viewed as far less trustworthy than non-partisan speakers. Furthermore, presenting Democrats and Republicans with ideologically-congruent descriptions of political events polarized their attitudes towards the events described. Overall, the present investigation reveals how partisan language, while praised by co-partisans, can damage trust and amplify disagreement across political divides.
我们研究了党派语言(即以支持政治议程的方式描述事件的语言)的影响,包括人们对使用这种语言的发言者的看法以及他们对这种语言所描述的事件的评价。在两次实验中,我们从美国招募了 1121 名民主党人和共和党人。通过使用一组偏向自由派(如扩大投票权)和偏向保守派(如减少选举安全)的词语,我们发现党派人士认为使用意识形态一致的语言描述两极分化事件的演讲者比使用非党派方式描述事件(如扩大邮寄投票)的演讲者更值得信赖。然而,当向敌对的党派人士介绍时,意识形态偏见的语言会促进对敌对党派人士的负面评价,使用外群体语言的发言人被认为远不如非党派发言人值得信赖。此外,向民主党人和共和党人提供意识形态一致的政治事件描述会使他们对所描述事件的态度两极分化。总之,本调查揭示了党派语言虽然受到共同党派人士的赞扬,但却会损害信任并扩大政治分歧。
{"title":"Partisan language in a polarized world: In-group language provides reputational benefits to speakers while polarizing audiences","authors":"Alexander C. Walker ,&nbsp;Jonathan A. Fugelsang ,&nbsp;Derek J. Koehler","doi":"10.1016/j.cognition.2024.106012","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cognition.2024.106012","url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>We examine the impact of partisan language (i.e., language that describes events in a manner that supports a political agenda), both with regard to peoples' perceptions of the speakers who use it and their evaluations of the events it is used to describe. In two experiments, we recruited 1121 Democrats and Republicans from the United States. Using a set of liberal-biased (e.g., <em>expand voting rights</em>) and conservative-biased (e.g., <em>reduce election security</em>) terms, we find that partisans judge speakers describing polarizing events using ideologically-congruent language as more trustworthy than those describing events in a non-partisan way (e.g., <em>expand mail-in voting</em>). However, when presented to rival partisans, ideologically-biased language promoted negative evaluations of opposing partisans, with speakers attributed out-group language being viewed as far less trustworthy than non-partisan speakers. Furthermore, presenting Democrats and Republicans with ideologically-congruent descriptions of political events polarized their attitudes towards the events described. Overall, the present investigation reveals how partisan language, while praised by co-partisans, can damage trust and amplify disagreement across political divides.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":48455,"journal":{"name":"Cognition","volume":"254 ","pages":"Article 106012"},"PeriodicalIF":2.8,"publicationDate":"2024-11-21","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142693659","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
What's left of the leftward bias in scene viewing? Lateral asymmetries in information processing during early search guidance 场景观察中的左向偏差还剩什么?早期搜索引导过程中信息处理的两侧不对称性
IF 2.8 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-11-21 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2024.106009
Sara Spotorno , Benjamin W. Tatler
Understanding how early scene viewing is guided can reveal fundamental brain mechanisms for quickly making sense of our surroundings. Viewing is often initiated from the left side. Across two experiments, we focused on search initiation for lateralised targets within real-world scenes, investigating the role of the cerebral hemispheres in guiding the first saccade. We aimed to disentangle hemispheric contribution from the effects of reading habits and distinguish between an overall dominance of the right hemisphere for visuospatial processing and finer hemispheric specialisation for the type of target template representation (from pictorial versus verbal cues), spatial scale (global versus local), and timescale (short versus longer). We replicated the tendency to initiate search leftward in both experiments. However, we found no evidence supporting a significant impact of left-to-right reading habits, either as a purely motor or attentional bias to the left. A general visuospatial dominance of the right hemisphere could not account for the results either. In Experiment 1, we found a greater probability of directing the first saccade toward targets in the left visual field but only after a verbal target cue, with no lateral differences after a pictorial cue. This suggested a contribution of the right hemisphere specialisation in perceptually simulating words' referents. Lengthening the Inter-Stimulus Interval between the cue and the scene (from 100 to 900 ms) resulted in reduced first saccade gain in the left visual field, suggesting a decreased ability of the the right hemisphere to use the target template to guide gaze close to the target object, which primarily depends on local information processing. Experiment 2, using visual versus auditory verbal cues, replicated and extended the findings for both first saccade direction and gain. Overall, our study shows that the multidetermined functional specialisation of the cerebral hemispheres is a key driver of early scene search and must be incorporated into theories and models to advance understanding of the mechanisms that guide viewing behaviour.
了解早期场景观察的引导方式,可以揭示大脑快速感知周围环境的基本机制。观看通常是从左侧开始的。在两个实验中,我们重点研究了真实世界场景中侧向目标的搜索启动,调查了大脑半球在引导第一次囊回中的作用。我们的目的是将大脑半球的贡献与阅读习惯的影响区分开来,并区分出右半球在视觉空间处理方面的整体优势,以及大脑半球在目标模板表征类型(来自图像线索还是语言线索)、空间尺度(全局还是局部)和时间尺度(短还是长)方面更精细的特化。我们在两个实验中都复制了向左搜索的趋势。然而,我们没有发现任何证据支持从左到右的阅读习惯会产生重大影响,无论是纯粹的运动偏向还是注意力偏向左侧。右半球的视觉空间优势也无法解释实验结果。在实验 1 中,我们发现将第一个囊回指向左侧视野目标的概率更大,但仅限于语言目标提示之后,图像提示之后则没有横向差异。这表明右半球在感知模拟单词所指方面的特化做出了贡献。延长提示和场景之间的刺激间隔(从 100 毫秒到 900 毫秒)会导致左侧视野的首次囊回增益减少,这表明右半球利用目标模板引导视线接近目标物体的能力下降,而这主要依赖于局部信息处理。实验二使用了视觉与听觉言语线索,复制并扩展了首次囊回方向和增益的研究结果。总之,我们的研究表明,大脑半球的多决定功能特化是早期场景搜索的关键驱动力,必须将其纳入理论和模型中,以促进对引导注视行为的机制的理解。
{"title":"What's left of the leftward bias in scene viewing? Lateral asymmetries in information processing during early search guidance","authors":"Sara Spotorno ,&nbsp;Benjamin W. Tatler","doi":"10.1016/j.cognition.2024.106009","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cognition.2024.106009","url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>Understanding how early scene viewing is guided can reveal fundamental brain mechanisms for quickly making sense of our surroundings. Viewing is often initiated from the left side. Across two experiments, we focused on search initiation for lateralised targets within real-world scenes, investigating the role of the cerebral hemispheres in guiding the first saccade. We aimed to disentangle hemispheric contribution from the effects of reading habits and distinguish between an overall dominance of the right hemisphere for visuospatial processing and finer hemispheric specialisation for the type of target template representation (from pictorial versus verbal cues), spatial scale (global versus local), and timescale (short versus longer). We replicated the tendency to initiate search leftward in both experiments. However, we found no evidence supporting a significant impact of left-to-right reading habits, either as a purely motor or attentional bias to the left. A general visuospatial dominance of the right hemisphere could not account for the results either. In Experiment 1, we found a greater probability of directing the first saccade toward targets in the left visual field but only after a verbal target cue, with no lateral differences after a pictorial cue. This suggested a contribution of the right hemisphere specialisation in perceptually simulating words' referents. Lengthening the Inter-Stimulus Interval between the cue and the scene (from 100 to 900 ms) resulted in reduced first saccade gain in the left visual field, suggesting a decreased ability of the the right hemisphere to use the target template to guide gaze close to the target object, which primarily depends on local information processing. Experiment 2, using visual versus auditory verbal cues, replicated and extended the findings for both first saccade direction and gain. Overall, our study shows that the multidetermined functional specialisation of the cerebral hemispheres is a key driver of early scene search and must be incorporated into theories and models to advance understanding of the mechanisms that guide viewing behaviour.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":48455,"journal":{"name":"Cognition","volume":"254 ","pages":"Article 106009"},"PeriodicalIF":2.8,"publicationDate":"2024-11-21","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142693660","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Language enables the acquisition of distinct sensorimotor memories for speech 语言能让人获得与言语不同的感官运动记忆。
IF 2.8 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-11-20 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2024.106010
Daniel R. Lametti , Emma D. Wheeler , Samantha Palatinus , Imane Hocine , Douglas M. Shiller
Interactions between the context in which a sensorimotor skill is learned and the recall of that memory have been primarily studied in limb movements, but speech production requires movement, and many aspects of speech processing are influenced by task-relevant contextual information. Here, in ecologically valid speech (read sentences), we test whether English-French bilinguals can use the language of production to acquire and recall distinct motor plans for similar speech sounds spanning the production workspace. Participants experienced real-time alterations of auditory feedback while producing interleaved English and French sentences. The alterations were equal in magnitude but opposite in direction between languages. Over three experiments (n = 15 in each), we observed language-specific sensorimotor learning in speech that countered the alterations and persisted after the alterations were removed. The effects were not observed in a fourth experiment (n = 15) when the feedback alterations were tied to a non-linguistic cue. In a fifth experiment (n = 15), we provide further confirmation that the observed language-specific changes in speech production were confined to sentence production, the linguistic level at which they were learned. The results contrast with recent work and theories of second language learning that predict broad interference between L1 and L2 phonetic representations. When faced with contrasting sensorimotor demands between languages, bilinguals readily acquire and recall highly specific motor representations for speech.
感官运动技能的学习环境与该记忆的回忆之间的相互作用主要是在肢体运动中进行研究的,但语音的产生需要运动,而且语音处理的许多方面都受到任务相关环境信息的影响。在此,我们测试了在生态有效的语音(读句子)中,英法双语者是否能利用语言生产来获取和回忆跨越生产工作空间的类似语音的不同运动计划。受试者在发出交错的英语和法语句子时,会经历听觉反馈的实时变化。不同语言的听觉反馈变化幅度相同,但方向相反。在三次实验中(每次实验的人数为 15 人),我们观察到了特定语言在语音方面的感觉运动学习,这种学习抵消了改变,并在改变消除后继续存在。在第四个实验(n = 15)中,当反馈改变与非语言线索联系在一起时,没有观察到这种效果。在第五个实验(n = 15)中,我们进一步证实了所观察到的特定语言的语音生成变化仅限于句子生成,而句子生成正是学习这些变化的语言层面。这些结果与最近的研究和第二语言学习理论所预测的第一语言和第二语言语音表征之间的广泛干扰形成了鲜明对比。当面对不同语言之间截然不同的感觉运动要求时,双语者很容易获得并回忆起高度特定的语音运动表征。
{"title":"Language enables the acquisition of distinct sensorimotor memories for speech","authors":"Daniel R. Lametti ,&nbsp;Emma D. Wheeler ,&nbsp;Samantha Palatinus ,&nbsp;Imane Hocine ,&nbsp;Douglas M. Shiller","doi":"10.1016/j.cognition.2024.106010","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cognition.2024.106010","url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>Interactions between the context in which a sensorimotor skill is learned and the recall of that memory have been primarily studied in limb movements, but speech production requires movement, and many aspects of speech processing are influenced by task-relevant contextual information. Here, in ecologically valid speech (read sentences), we test whether English-French bilinguals can use the language of production to acquire and recall distinct motor plans for similar speech sounds spanning the production workspace. Participants experienced real-time alterations of auditory feedback while producing interleaved English and French sentences. The alterations were equal in magnitude but <em>opposite</em> in direction between languages. Over three experiments (<em>n</em> = 15 in each), we observed language-specific sensorimotor learning in speech that countered the alterations and persisted after the alterations were removed. The effects were not observed in a fourth experiment (<em>n</em> = 15) when the feedback alterations were tied to a non-linguistic cue. In a fifth experiment (n = 15), we provide further confirmation that the observed language-specific changes in speech production were confined to sentence production, the linguistic level at which they were learned. The results contrast with recent work and theories of second language learning that predict broad interference between L1 and L2 phonetic representations. When faced with contrasting sensorimotor demands between languages, bilinguals readily acquire and recall highly specific motor representations for speech.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":48455,"journal":{"name":"Cognition","volume":"254 ","pages":"Article 106010"},"PeriodicalIF":2.8,"publicationDate":"2024-11-20","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142689289","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Morality on the road: Should machine drivers be more utilitarian than human drivers? 道路上的道德:机器驾驶员是否应该比人类驾驶员更加功利?
IF 2.8 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-11-19 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2024.106011
Peng Liu , Yueying Chu , Siming Zhai , Tingru Zhang , Edmond Awad
Machines powered by artificial intelligence have the potential to replace or collaborate with human decision-makers in moral settings. In these roles, machines would face moral tradeoffs, such as automated vehicles (AVs) distributing inevitable risks among road users. Do people believe that machines should make moral decisions differently from humans? If so, why? To address these questions, we conducted six studies (N = 6805) to examine how people, as observers, believe human drivers and AVs should act in similar moral dilemmas and how they judge their moral decisions. In pedestrian-only dilemmas where the two agents had to sacrifice one pedestrian to save more pedestrians, participants held them to similar utilitarian norms (Study 1). In occupant dilemmas where the agents needed to weigh the in-vehicle occupant against more pedestrians, participants were less accepting of AVs sacrificing their passenger compared to human drivers sacrificing themselves (Studies 1–3) or another passenger (Studies 5–6). The difference was not driven by reduced occupant agency in AVs (Study 4) or by non-voluntary occupant sacrifice in AVs (Study 5), but rather by the perceived social relationship between AVs and their users (Study 6). Thus, even when people adopt an impartial stance as observers, they are more likely to believe that AVs should prioritize serving their users in moral dilemmas. We discuss the theoretical and practical implications for AV morality.
人工智能驱动的机器有可能在道德领域取代人类决策者或与人类决策者合作。在这些角色中,机器将面临道德权衡,例如自动驾驶汽车(AV)在道路使用者之间分配不可避免的风险。人们是否认为机器在做出道德决定时应与人类有所不同?如果是,为什么?为了解决这些问题,我们进行了六项研究(N = 6805),以考察作为观察者的人们认为人类驾驶员和自动驾驶汽车在类似的道德困境中应如何行动,以及他们如何判断自己的道德决策。在只有行人的困境中,两个驾驶员必须牺牲一名行人以拯救更多行人,参与者认为他们的功利准则相似(研究 1)。在乘员困境中,驾驶员需要权衡车内乘员和更多行人,与人类驾驶员牺牲自己(研究 1-3)或另一名乘客(研究 5-6)相比,参与者对自动驾驶汽车牺牲乘客的接受度较低。造成这种差异的原因并不是自动驾驶汽车中乘员代理权的降低(研究 4),也不是自动驾驶汽车中乘员的非自愿牺牲(研究 5),而是自动驾驶汽车与其使用者之间的社会关系(研究 6)。因此,即使人们作为观察者采取公正的立场,他们也更有可能认为,在道德困境中,自动驾驶汽车应优先为其用户服务。我们将讨论 AV 道德的理论和实践意义。
{"title":"Morality on the road: Should machine drivers be more utilitarian than human drivers?","authors":"Peng Liu ,&nbsp;Yueying Chu ,&nbsp;Siming Zhai ,&nbsp;Tingru Zhang ,&nbsp;Edmond Awad","doi":"10.1016/j.cognition.2024.106011","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cognition.2024.106011","url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>Machines powered by artificial intelligence have the potential to replace or collaborate with human decision-makers in moral settings. In these roles, machines would face moral tradeoffs, such as automated vehicles (AVs) distributing inevitable risks among road users. Do people believe that machines should make moral decisions differently from humans? If so, why? To address these questions, we conducted six studies (<em>N</em> = 6805) to examine how people, as observers, believe human drivers and AVs should act in similar moral dilemmas and how they judge their moral decisions. In pedestrian-only dilemmas where the two agents had to sacrifice one pedestrian to save more pedestrians, participants held them to similar utilitarian norms (Study 1). In occupant dilemmas where the agents needed to weigh the in-vehicle occupant against more pedestrians, participants were less accepting of AVs sacrificing their passenger compared to human drivers sacrificing themselves (Studies 1–3) or another passenger (Studies 5–6). The difference was not driven by reduced occupant agency in AVs (Study 4) or by non-voluntary occupant sacrifice in AVs (Study 5), but rather by the perceived social relationship between AVs and their users (Study 6). Thus, even when people adopt an impartial stance as observers, they are more likely to believe that AVs should prioritize serving their users in moral dilemmas. We discuss the theoretical and practical implications for AV morality.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":48455,"journal":{"name":"Cognition","volume":"254 ","pages":"Article 106011"},"PeriodicalIF":2.8,"publicationDate":"2024-11-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142676223","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Relative source credibility affects the continued influence effect: Evidence of rationality in the CIE 相对来源可信度影响持续影响效应:CIE 中的理性证据。
IF 2.8 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-11-18 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2024.106000
Carolin V. Hey, Marie Luisa Schaper, Ute J. Bayen
The Continued Influence Effect (CIE) is the phenomenon that retracted information often continues to influence judgments and inferences. The CIE is rational when the source that retracts the information (the retractor) is less credible than the source that originally presented the information (the informant; Connor Desai et al., 2020). Conversely, a CIE is not rational when the retractor is at least as credible as the informant. Thus, a rational account predicts that the CIE depends on the relative credibility of informant and retractor. In two experiments (N = 151, N = 146), informant credibility and retractor credibility were independently manipulated. Participants read a fictitious news report in which original information and a retraction were each presented by either a source with high credibility or a source with low credibility. In both experiments, when the informant was more credible than the retractor, participants showed a CIE compared to control participants who saw neither the information nor the retraction (ds > 0.82). When the informant was less credible than the retractor, participants showed no CIE, in line with a rational account. However, in Experiment 2, participants also showed a CIE when informant and retractor were equally credible (ds > 0.51). This cannot be explained by a rational account, but is consistent with error-based accounts of the CIE. Thus, a rational account alone cannot fully account for the complete pattern of results, but needs to be complemented with accounts that view the CIE as a memory-based error.
持续影响效应(CIE)是指被撤回的信息往往会继续影响人们的判断和推论。当撤回信息的来源(撤回者)的可信度低于最初提供信息的来源(提供者;Connor Desai et al.)反之,如果撤回信息者的可信度至少与提供信息者相当,那么 CIE 就不是理性的。因此,从理性的角度来看,CIE 取决于信息提供者和信息收回者的相对可信度。在两个实验中(N = 151,N = 146),告知者可信度和转述者可信度被独立操纵。参与者阅读了一篇虚构的新闻报道,在这篇报道中,原始信息和撤回信息分别由可信度高或可信度低的信息来源提供。在这两项实验中,当提供信息者比撤回信息者更可信时,与既没有看到信息也没有看到撤回信息的对照组参与者相比,参与者都表现出了 CIE(ds > 0.82)。当信息提供者的可信度低于撤回者时,参与者没有表现出 CIE,这与理性解释相符。然而,在实验 2 中,当提供信息者和撤回信息者的可信度相同时,参与者也表现出了 CIE(ds > 0.51)。这不能用理性解释来解释,而是与基于错误的 CIE 解释相一致。因此,仅凭理性解释并不能完全解释完整的结果模式,还需要将 CIE 视为基于记忆的错误的解释来补充。
{"title":"Relative source credibility affects the continued influence effect: Evidence of rationality in the CIE","authors":"Carolin V. Hey,&nbsp;Marie Luisa Schaper,&nbsp;Ute J. Bayen","doi":"10.1016/j.cognition.2024.106000","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cognition.2024.106000","url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>The <em>Continued Influence Effect</em> (CIE) is the phenomenon that retracted information often continues to influence judgments and inferences. The CIE is rational when the source that retracts the information (the <em>retractor</em>) is less credible than the source that originally presented the information (the <em>informant</em>; Connor Desai et al., 2020). Conversely, a CIE is not rational when the retractor is at least as credible as the informant. Thus, a rational account predicts that the CIE depends on the relative credibility of informant and retractor. In two experiments (<em>N</em> = 151, <em>N</em> = 146), informant credibility and retractor credibility were independently manipulated. Participants read a fictitious news report in which original information and a retraction were each presented by either a source with high credibility or a source with low credibility. In both experiments, when the informant was more credible than the retractor, participants showed a CIE compared to control participants who saw neither the information nor the retraction (<em>d</em>s &gt; 0.82). When the informant was less credible than the retractor, participants showed no CIE, in line with a rational account. However, in Experiment 2, participants also showed a CIE when informant and retractor were equally credible (<em>d</em>s &gt; 0.51). This cannot be explained by a rational account, but is consistent with error-based accounts of the CIE. Thus, a rational account alone cannot fully account for the complete pattern of results, but needs to be complemented with accounts that view the CIE as a memory-based error.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":48455,"journal":{"name":"Cognition","volume":"254 ","pages":"Article 106000"},"PeriodicalIF":2.8,"publicationDate":"2024-11-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142676486","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Decoding face identity: A reverse-correlation approach using deep learning 解码人脸身份:利用深度学习的反向相关方法。
IF 2.8 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-11-16 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2024.106008
Xue Tian , Yiying Song , Jia Liu
Face recognition is crucial for social interactions. Traditional approaches primarily rely on subjective judgment, utilizing a pre-selected set of facial features based on literature or intuition to identify critical facial features for face recognition. In this study, we adopted a reverse-correlation approach, aligning responses of a deep convolutional neural network (DCNN) with its internal representations to objectively identify facial features pivotal for face recognition. Specifically, we trained a DCNN, namely VGG-FD, to possess human-like capability in discriminating facial identities. A representational similarity analysis (RSA) was employed to characterize VGG-FD's performance metrics, which was subsequently reverse-correlated with its representations in layers capable of discriminating facial identities. Our analysis revealed a higher likelihood of face pairs being perceived as different identities when their representations significantly differed in areas such as the eyes, eyebrows, or central facial region, suggesting the significance of the eyes as facial parts and the central facial region as an integral of face configuration in face recognition. In summary, our study leveraged DCNNs to identify critical facial features for face discrimination in a hypothesis-neutral, data-driven manner, hereby advocating for the adoption of this new paradigm to explore critical facial features across various face recognition tasks.
人脸识别对于社会交往至关重要。传统方法主要依赖于主观判断,利用根据文献或直觉预先选择的一组面部特征来识别人脸识别的关键面部特征。在本研究中,我们采用了一种反向相关方法,将深度卷积神经网络(DCNN)的响应与其内部表征相一致,从而客观地识别出人脸识别中至关重要的面部特征。具体来说,我们训练了一个 DCNN,即 VGG-FD,使其在辨别人脸身份方面具有与人类类似的能力。我们采用了表征相似性分析(RSA)来描述 VGG-FD 的性能指标,随后将其与能够辨别人脸身份的层中的表征进行反向关联。我们的分析表明,如果人脸对在眼睛、眉毛或面部中央区域等区域的表征存在显著差异,那么人脸对被视为不同身份的可能性就会更高,这表明眼睛作为面部的一部分以及面部中央区域作为人脸识别中面部配置的一个组成部分具有重要意义。总之,我们的研究利用 DCNNs 以假设中立、数据驱动的方式识别了人脸识别的关键面部特征,从而倡导采用这种新范式来探索各种人脸识别任务中的关键面部特征。
{"title":"Decoding face identity: A reverse-correlation approach using deep learning","authors":"Xue Tian ,&nbsp;Yiying Song ,&nbsp;Jia Liu","doi":"10.1016/j.cognition.2024.106008","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cognition.2024.106008","url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>Face recognition is crucial for social interactions. Traditional approaches primarily rely on subjective judgment, utilizing a pre-selected set of facial features based on literature or intuition to identify critical facial features for face recognition. In this study, we adopted a reverse-correlation approach, aligning responses of a deep convolutional neural network (DCNN) with its internal representations to objectively identify facial features pivotal for face recognition. Specifically, we trained a DCNN, namely VGG-FD, to possess human-like capability in discriminating facial identities. A representational similarity analysis (RSA) was employed to characterize VGG-FD's performance metrics, which was subsequently reverse-correlated with its representations in layers capable of discriminating facial identities. Our analysis revealed a higher likelihood of face pairs being perceived as different identities when their representations significantly differed in areas such as the eyes, eyebrows, or central facial region, suggesting the significance of the eyes as facial parts and the central facial region as an integral of face configuration in face recognition. In summary, our study leveraged DCNNs to identify critical facial features for face discrimination in a hypothesis-neutral, data-driven manner, hereby advocating for the adoption of this new paradigm to explore critical facial features across various face recognition tasks.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":48455,"journal":{"name":"Cognition","volume":"254 ","pages":"Article 106008"},"PeriodicalIF":2.8,"publicationDate":"2024-11-16","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142649374","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
How does color distribution learning affect goal-directed visuomotor behavior? 颜色分布学习如何影响目标导向的视觉运动行为?
IF 2.8 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-11-15 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2024.106002
Léa Entzmann , Árni Gunnar Ásgeirsson , Árni Kristjánsson
While the visual world is rich and complex, importantly, it nevertheless contains many statistical regularities. For example, environmental feature distributions tend to remain relatively stable from one moment to the next. Recent findings have shown how observers can learn surprising details of environmental color distributions, even when the colors belong to actively ignored stimuli such as distractors in visual search. Our aim was to determine whether such learning influences orienting in the visual environment, measured with saccadic eye movements. In two visual search experiments, observers had to find an odd-one-out target. Firstly, we tested cases where observers selected targets by fixating them. Secondly, we measured saccadic eye movements when observers made judgments on the target and responded manually. Trials were structured in blocks, containing learning trials where distractors came from the same color distribution (uniform or Gaussian) while on subsequent test trials, the target was at different distances from the mean of the learning distractor distribution. For both manual and saccadic measures, performance improved throughout the learning trials and was better when the distractor colors came from a Gaussian distribution. Moreover, saccade latencies during test trials depended on the distance between the color of the current target and the distractors on learning trials, replicating results obtained with manual responses. Latencies were slowed when the target color was within the learning distractor color distribution and also revealed that observers learned the difference between uniform and Gaussian distributions. The importance of several variables in predicting saccadic and manual reaction times was studied using random forests, revealing similar rankings for both modalities, although previous distractor color had a higher impact on free eye movements. Overall, our results demonstrate learning of detailed characteristics of environmental color distributions that affects early attentional selection rather than later decisional processes.
视觉世界丰富而复杂,但重要的是,它包含许多统计规律。例如,环境特征分布在不同时刻往往保持相对稳定。最近的研究结果表明,观察者可以学习到环境颜色分布的惊人细节,即使这些颜色属于视觉搜索中被主动忽略的刺激物(如干扰物)。我们的目的是确定这种学习是否会影响视觉环境中的定向,并用眼球移动来测量。在两个视觉搜索实验中,观察者必须找到一个奇异的目标。首先,我们测试了观察者通过固定目标来选择目标的情况。其次,我们测量了观察者对目标做出判断并手动做出反应时的眼球回旋运动。试验以块为单位,其中包含学习试验,在学习试验中,干扰物来自相同的颜色分布(均匀分布或高斯分布),而在随后的测试试验中,目标物与学习干扰物分布的平均值距离不同。在徒手和囊回测量中,整个学习试验过程中的成绩都有所提高,而当干扰物的颜色来自高斯分布时,成绩会更好。此外,测试试验中的囊回延迟取决于当前目标颜色与学习试验中的干扰物颜色之间的距离,这与手动反应的结果相同。当目标颜色在学习干扰物颜色分布范围内时,延迟会减慢,这也表明观察者学会了均匀分布和高斯分布之间的区别。我们使用随机森林研究了几个变量在预测飞蚊症和手动反应时间中的重要性,结果显示两种模式的排名相似,但先前的分心颜色对自由眼球运动的影响更大。总之,我们的研究结果表明,对环境颜色分布详细特征的学习会影响早期的注意选择,而不是后期的决策过程。
{"title":"How does color distribution learning affect goal-directed visuomotor behavior?","authors":"Léa Entzmann ,&nbsp;Árni Gunnar Ásgeirsson ,&nbsp;Árni Kristjánsson","doi":"10.1016/j.cognition.2024.106002","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cognition.2024.106002","url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>While the visual world is rich and complex, importantly, it nevertheless contains many statistical regularities. For example, environmental feature distributions tend to remain relatively stable from one moment to the next. Recent findings have shown how observers can learn surprising details of environmental color distributions, even when the colors belong to actively ignored stimuli such as distractors in visual search. Our aim was to determine whether such learning influences orienting in the visual environment, measured with saccadic eye movements. In two visual search experiments, observers had to find an odd-one-out target. Firstly, we tested cases where observers selected targets by fixating them. Secondly, we measured saccadic eye movements when observers made judgments on the target and responded manually. Trials were structured in blocks, containing <em>learning trials</em> where distractors came from the same color distribution (uniform or Gaussian) while on subsequent <em>test trials</em>, the target was at different distances from the mean of the learning distractor distribution. For both manual and saccadic measures, performance improved throughout the learning trials and was better when the distractor colors came from a Gaussian distribution. Moreover, saccade latencies during test trials depended on the distance between the color of the current target and the distractors on learning trials, replicating results obtained with manual responses. Latencies were slowed when the target color was within the learning distractor color distribution and also revealed that observers learned the difference between uniform and Gaussian distributions. The importance of several variables in predicting saccadic and manual reaction times was studied using random forests, revealing similar rankings for both modalities, although previous distractor color had a higher impact on free eye movements. Overall, our results demonstrate learning of detailed characteristics of environmental color distributions that affects early attentional selection rather than later decisional processes.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":48455,"journal":{"name":"Cognition","volume":"254 ","pages":"Article 106002"},"PeriodicalIF":2.8,"publicationDate":"2024-11-15","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142639892","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Bias-free measure of distractor avoidance in visual search 视觉搜索中避免分心的无偏测量方法
IF 2.8 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-11-14 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2024.106007
Xiaojin Ma , Richard A. Abrams
Recent findings suggest that it is possible for people to proactively avoid attentional capture by salient distractors during visual search. The results have important implications for understanding the competing influences of top-down and bottom-up factors in visual attention. Nevertheless, questions remain regarding the extent to which apparently ignored distractors are processed. To assess distractor processing, previous experiments have used a probe method in which stimuli are occasionally superimposed on the search display–requiring participants to abort the search and identify the probe stimuli. It has been recently shown that such probe tasks may be vulnerable to decision-level biases, such as a participant's willingness to report stimuli on to-be-ignored items. We report here results from a new method that is not subject to this limitation. In the new method, the non-target search elements, including the salient distractors, contained features that were either congruent or incongruent with the target. Processing of the non-target elements is inferred from the effects of the compatibility of the shared features on judgments about the target. In four experiments using the technique we show that ignored salient distractors are indeed processed less fully than non-target elements that are not salient, replicating the results of earlier studies using the probe methods. Additionally, the processing of the distractors was found to be reduced at least in part at early perceptual or attentional stages, as assumed by models of attentional suppression. The study confirms the proactive avoidance of capture by salient distractors measured without decision-level biases and provides a new technique for assessing the magnitude of distractor processing.
最近的研究结果表明,人们有可能在视觉搜索过程中主动避免被突出的分心物吸引注意力。这些结果对于理解视觉注意力中自上而下和自下而上因素的相互影响具有重要意义。尽管如此,人们对明显被忽略的分心物的处理程度仍然存在疑问。为了评估分心物的处理情况,以往的实验采用了一种探测法,即在搜索显示屏上偶尔叠加一些刺激物,要求被试中止搜索并识别探测刺激物。最近的研究表明,这种探究任务可能容易受到决策层面偏差的影响,例如被试愿意报告即将被忽略的项目上的刺激物。我们在此报告一种不受此限制的新方法的结果。在新方法中,非目标搜索元素(包括突出的干扰物)包含与目标一致或不一致的特征。对非目标元素的处理是通过共享特征的兼容性对目标判断的影响来推断的。在使用该技术进行的四次实验中,我们发现被忽略的突出分心元素的处理确实不如不突出的非目标元素那么充分,这与之前使用探针法进行的研究结果相同。此外,我们还发现,至少在早期知觉或注意阶段,对分心元素的处理会部分减少,这与注意抑制模型所假设的一样。这项研究证实了在没有决策水平偏差的情况下测得的主动避免被突出分心物捕获的结果,并提供了一种评估分心物加工程度的新技术。
{"title":"Bias-free measure of distractor avoidance in visual search","authors":"Xiaojin Ma ,&nbsp;Richard A. Abrams","doi":"10.1016/j.cognition.2024.106007","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cognition.2024.106007","url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>Recent findings suggest that it is possible for people to proactively avoid attentional capture by salient distractors during visual search. The results have important implications for understanding the competing influences of top-down and bottom-up factors in visual attention. Nevertheless, questions remain regarding the extent to which apparently ignored distractors are processed. To assess distractor processing, previous experiments have used a probe method in which stimuli are occasionally superimposed on the search display–requiring participants to abort the search and identify the probe stimuli. It has been recently shown that such probe tasks may be vulnerable to decision-level biases, such as a participant's willingness to report stimuli on to-be-ignored items. We report here results from a new method that is not subject to this limitation. In the new method, the non-target search elements, including the salient distractors, contained features that were either congruent or incongruent with the target. Processing of the non-target elements is inferred from the effects of the compatibility of the shared features on judgments about the target. In four experiments using the technique we show that ignored salient distractors are indeed processed less fully than non-target elements that are not salient, replicating the results of earlier studies using the probe methods. Additionally, the processing of the distractors was found to be reduced at least in part at early perceptual or attentional stages, as assumed by models of attentional suppression. The study confirms the proactive avoidance of capture by salient distractors measured without decision-level biases and provides a new technique for assessing the magnitude of distractor processing.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":48455,"journal":{"name":"Cognition","volume":"254 ","pages":"Article 106007"},"PeriodicalIF":2.8,"publicationDate":"2024-11-14","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142630757","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Signaling (in)tolerance: Social evaluation and metaethical relativism and objectivism 发出(不)宽容的信号:社会评价与元伦理相对主义和客观主义。
IF 2.8 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-11-13 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2024.105984
David Moss , Andres Montealegre , Lance S. Bush , Lucius Caviola , David Pizarro
Prior work has established that laypeople do not consistently treat moral questions as being objectively true or as merely true relative to different perspectives. Rather, these metaethical judgments vary dramatically across moral issues and in response to different social influences. We offer a potential explanation by examining how objectivists and relativists are evaluated in different contexts. We provide evidence for a novel account of metaethical judgments as signaling tolerance or intolerance of disagreement. The social implications of signaling tolerance or intolerance in different contexts may motivate different metaethical judgments. Study 1 finds that relativists are perceived as more tolerant, empathic, having superior moral character, and as more desirable as social partners than objectivists. Study 2 replicates these findings with a within-participants design and also shows that objectivists are perceived as more morally serious than relativists. Study 3 examines evaluations of objectivists and relativists regarding concrete moral issues, finding these results vary across situations of moral agreement and disagreement. Study 4 finds that participants' metaethical stances likewise vary when responding in the way they think would make a person who agrees or disagrees with them evaluate them more positively. However, in Study 5, we find no effect on metaethical judgment of telling participants they will be evaluated by a person who agrees or disagrees with them, which suggests either a failure to induce reputational concerns or a more limited influence of reputational considerations on metaethical judgments, despite strong effects on social evaluation.
先前的研究已经证实,非专业人士在对待道德问题时,并不会一味地认为这些问题是客观真实的,或者仅仅是相对于不同观点而言是真实的。相反,在不同的道德问题和不同的社会影响下,这些元道德判断会有很大的不同。我们通过研究客观论者和相对论者在不同情境下的评价方式,提供了一种可能的解释。我们为元道德判断作为容忍或不容忍不同意见的信号的新说法提供了证据。在不同情境中表示容忍或不容忍所产生的社会影响可能会促使人们做出不同的元伦理判断。研究 1 发现,相对主义者被认为比客观主义者更宽容、更富有同情心、道德品质更高尚、更适合作为社会伙伴。研究 2 采用参与者内部设计复制了这些研究结果,并显示客观论者比相对论者被认为在道德上更严肃。研究 3 考察了客观论者和相对论者对具体道德问题的评价,发现这些结果在道德一致和不一致的情况下有所不同。研究 4 发现,当参与者以他们认为会让同意或不同意他们的人对他们做出更积极评价的方式做出反应时,他们的元道德立场同样会发生变化。然而,在研究 5 中,我们发现告诉参与者他们将被同意或不同意他们的人评价时,对他们的元伦理判断没有影响,这表明尽管对社会评价有很大的影响,但要么没有诱发声誉方面的考虑,要么声誉方面的考虑对元伦理判断的影响比较有限。
{"title":"Signaling (in)tolerance: Social evaluation and metaethical relativism and objectivism","authors":"David Moss ,&nbsp;Andres Montealegre ,&nbsp;Lance S. Bush ,&nbsp;Lucius Caviola ,&nbsp;David Pizarro","doi":"10.1016/j.cognition.2024.105984","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cognition.2024.105984","url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>Prior work has established that laypeople do not consistently treat moral questions as being objectively true or as merely true relative to different perspectives. Rather, these metaethical judgments vary dramatically across moral issues and in response to different social influences. We offer a potential explanation by examining how objectivists and relativists are evaluated in different contexts. We provide evidence for a novel account of metaethical judgments as signaling tolerance or intolerance of disagreement. The social implications of signaling tolerance or intolerance in different contexts may motivate different metaethical judgments. Study 1 finds that relativists are perceived as more tolerant, empathic, having superior moral character, and as more desirable as social partners than objectivists. Study 2 replicates these findings with a within-participants design and also shows that objectivists are perceived as more morally serious than relativists. Study 3 examines evaluations of objectivists and relativists regarding concrete moral issues, finding these results vary across situations of moral agreement and disagreement. Study 4 finds that participants' metaethical stances likewise vary when responding in the way they think would make a person who agrees or disagrees with them evaluate them more positively. However, in Study 5, we find no effect on metaethical judgment of telling participants they will be evaluated by a person who agrees or disagrees with them, which suggests either a failure to induce reputational concerns or a more limited influence of reputational considerations on metaethical judgments, despite strong effects on social evaluation.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":48455,"journal":{"name":"Cognition","volume":"254 ","pages":"Article 105984"},"PeriodicalIF":2.8,"publicationDate":"2024-11-13","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142630765","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Possible reasons for reductive seductions: A reply to Wilson et al. 还原诱惑的可能原因:对威尔逊等人的答复
IF 2.8 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-11-12 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2024.106003
Deena Skolnick Weisberg
Wilson et al. (2025) report a failed attempt to replicate the reductive allure effect: Unlike prior work, they do not find that participants judged explanations of scientific phenomena to be higher quality when they contained irrelevant reductive language. The current commentary considers three possible reasons for this failure to replicate: (1) a change in the nature of online study participants, (2) a change in the background knowledge that people bring to judgments of scientific explanations, and (3) a change in the kinds of explanations that people find satisfying.
威尔逊等人(2025 年)报告了一次复制还原诱惑效应的失败尝试:与之前的研究不同,他们没有发现当科学现象的解释包含不相关的还原性语言时,参与者会判断这些解释的质量更高。本评论认为,这种复制失败可能有三个原因:(1) 在线研究参与者的性质发生了变化;(2) 人们对科学解释的判断所带来的背景知识发生了变化;(3) 人们认为令人满意的解释类型发生了变化。
{"title":"Possible reasons for reductive seductions: A reply to Wilson et al.","authors":"Deena Skolnick Weisberg","doi":"10.1016/j.cognition.2024.106003","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cognition.2024.106003","url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>Wilson et al. (2025) report a failed attempt to replicate the reductive allure effect: Unlike prior work, they do not find that participants judged explanations of scientific phenomena to be higher quality when they contained irrelevant reductive language. The current commentary considers three possible reasons for this failure to replicate: (1) a change in the nature of online study participants, (2) a change in the background knowledge that people bring to judgments of scientific explanations, and (3) a change in the kinds of explanations that people find satisfying.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":48455,"journal":{"name":"Cognition","volume":"254 ","pages":"Article 106003"},"PeriodicalIF":2.8,"publicationDate":"2024-11-12","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142630763","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Cognition
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1