首页 > 最新文献

Cognition最新文献

英文 中文
Blocking of associative learning by explicit descriptions 显性描述对联想学习的阻碍。
IF 2.8 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-12-04 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2024.106015
Tom Kelly , Elliot A. Ludvig
People given written descriptions often learn and decide differently from those learning from experience, even in formally identical tasks. This paper presents two experiments detailing how telling participants about the value of one stimulus impacts a keystone learning effect – blocking. The paper investigates if descriptions can be used to effectively block future trial-by-trial learning. Participants were presented with coloured shape stimuli and asked if those shapes caused reward. Experiment 1 found both standard, trial-by-trial experienced blocking and the novel effect of described blocking of future trial-by-trial learning. Experiment 2 investigated the conditions that promote described blocking by manipulating the training that occurred prior to exposure to the description. In the Pre-training Present group, participants exposed to a training set of compound and elemental stimuli produced more pronounced blocking than the Pre-training Absent group, which had no such training. These results show that explicit descriptions about causal relations can block learning from subsequent experience, providing a new extension of associative learning toward the verbal domain.
即使在形式上相同的任务中,被书面描述的人的学习和决定往往与那些从经验中学习的人不同。本文提出了两个实验,详细说明了告诉参与者一个刺激的价值如何影响一个关键的学习效应-阻碍。本文探讨了描述是否可以有效地阻止未来的逐次学习。研究人员向参与者展示了彩色形状的刺激,并询问这些形状是否会引起奖励。实验1发现了标准的、逐次经历的阻碍和描述的阻碍对未来逐次学习的新影响。实验2研究了通过操纵描述前的训练来促进描述阻塞的条件。在训练前在场组中,暴露于复合和基本刺激训练集的参与者比没有进行此类训练的训练前缺席组产生了更明显的阻滞。这些结果表明,对因果关系的明确描述可以阻碍对后续经验的学习,为联想学习向言语领域的扩展提供了新的途径。
{"title":"Blocking of associative learning by explicit descriptions","authors":"Tom Kelly ,&nbsp;Elliot A. Ludvig","doi":"10.1016/j.cognition.2024.106015","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cognition.2024.106015","url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>People given written descriptions often learn and decide differently from those learning from experience, even in formally identical tasks. This paper presents two experiments detailing how telling participants about the value of one stimulus impacts a keystone learning effect – blocking. The paper investigates if descriptions can be used to effectively block future trial-by-trial learning. Participants were presented with coloured shape stimuli and asked if those shapes caused reward. Experiment 1 found both standard, trial-by-trial experienced blocking and the novel effect of described blocking of future trial-by-trial learning. Experiment 2 investigated the conditions that promote described blocking by manipulating the training that occurred prior to exposure to the description. In the Pre-training Present group, participants exposed to a training set of compound and elemental stimuli produced more pronounced blocking than the Pre-training Absent group, which had no such training. These results show that explicit descriptions about causal relations can block learning from subsequent experience, providing a new extension of associative learning toward the verbal domain.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":48455,"journal":{"name":"Cognition","volume":"256 ","pages":"Article 106015"},"PeriodicalIF":2.8,"publicationDate":"2024-12-04","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142786606","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Number adaptation: Reply 号码适配:回复
IF 2.8 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-11-30 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2024.105870
David Burr, Giovanni Anobile, Roberto Arrighi
Adaptation is a ubiquitous property of perceptual systems, increasing sensitivity to change and allowing them to operate over a large dynamic range. The number sense, like most other perceptual systems, is adaptable. Yousif et al. (2024) challenge the existence of number adaptation, offering an alternate explanation that they term the “old news hypothesis”. Here we consider the major evidence advanced for their theory and show that, while their predicted effects may reach statistical significance, they are far too small to begin to explain the robust phenomenon of adaptation. We also highlight a series of studies using fMRI, EEG, pupillometry and psychophysical techniques that support the existence of adaption, and are inconsistent with “old news”. We conclude that number adaptation, while not fully understood, does indeed exist, and remains an invaluable concept for understanding the number sense.
适应性是感知系统的一种普遍属性,它增加了对变化的敏感性,并允许它们在很大的动态范围内运行。与大多数其他感知系统一样,数字感具有适应性。Yousif等人(2024)对数字适应的存在提出了挑战,提出了另一种解释,他们称之为“旧新闻假说”。在这里,我们考虑了为他们的理论提出的主要证据,并表明,虽然他们预测的影响可能达到统计显著性,但它们太小,不足以开始解释强大的适应现象。我们还强调了一系列使用功能磁共振成像、脑电图、瞳孔测量和心理物理技术的研究,这些研究支持适应性的存在,并且与“旧新闻”不一致。我们的结论是,数字适应虽然没有被完全理解,但确实存在,并且仍然是理解数字感的宝贵概念。
{"title":"Number adaptation: Reply","authors":"David Burr,&nbsp;Giovanni Anobile,&nbsp;Roberto Arrighi","doi":"10.1016/j.cognition.2024.105870","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cognition.2024.105870","url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>Adaptation is a ubiquitous property of perceptual systems, increasing sensitivity to change and allowing them to operate over a large dynamic range. The <em>number sense</em>, like most other perceptual systems, is adaptable. Yousif et al. (2024) challenge the existence of number adaptation, offering an alternate explanation that they term the “old news hypothesis”. Here we consider the major evidence advanced for their theory and show that, while their predicted effects may reach statistical significance, they are far too small to begin to explain the robust phenomenon of adaptation. We also highlight a series of studies using fMRI, EEG, pupillometry and psychophysical techniques that support the existence of adaption, and are inconsistent with “old news”. We conclude that number adaptation, while not fully understood, does indeed exist, and remains an invaluable concept for understanding the <em>number sense</em>.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":48455,"journal":{"name":"Cognition","volume":"254 ","pages":"Article 105870"},"PeriodicalIF":2.8,"publicationDate":"2024-11-30","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142744569","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Exploring the hierarchical structure of human plans via program generation 通过程序生成探索人类计划的层次结构
IF 2.8 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-11-30 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2024.105990
Carlos G. Correa , Sophia Sanborn , Mark K. Ho , Frederick Callaway , Nathaniel D. Daw , Thomas L. Griffiths
Human behavior is often assumed to be hierarchically structured, made up of abstract actions that can be decomposed into concrete actions. However, behavior is typically measured as a sequence of actions, which makes it difficult to infer its hierarchical structure. In this paper, we explore how people form hierarchically structured plans, using an experimental paradigm with observable hierarchical representations: participants create programs that produce sequences of actions in a language with explicit hierarchical structure. This task lets us test two well-established principles of human behavior: utility maximization (i.e. using fewer actions) and minimum description length (MDL; i.e. having a shorter program). We find that humans are sensitive to both metrics, but that both accounts fail to predict a qualitative feature of human-created programs, namely that people prefer programs with reuse over and above the predictions of MDL. We formalize this preference for reuse by extending the MDL account into a generative model over programs, modeling hierarchy choice as the induction of a grammar over actions. Our account can explain the preference for reuse and provides better predictions of human behavior, going beyond simple accounts of compressibility to highlight a principle that guides hierarchical planning.
人类行为通常被认为是分层结构的,由可以分解为具体行为的抽象行为组成。然而,行为通常是作为一系列动作来衡量的,这使得很难推断其层次结构。在本文中,我们探索了人们如何形成分层结构的计划,使用一个具有可观察到的分层表示的实验范式:参与者创建程序,用具有明确分层结构的语言产生动作序列。这个任务让我们测试了两个公认的人类行为原则:效用最大化(即使用更少的动作)和最小描述长度(MDL;例如,缩短课程时间)。我们发现人类对这两个指标都很敏感,但是这两个描述都无法预测人类创建的程序的一个定性特征,即人们更喜欢具有重用性的程序,而不是MDL的预测。我们通过将MDL帐户扩展到程序上的生成模型来形式化这种重用偏好,将层次选择建模为对操作的语法归纳。我们的解释可以解释对重用的偏好,并提供更好的人类行为预测,超越了简单的可压缩性解释,强调了指导分层规划的原则。
{"title":"Exploring the hierarchical structure of human plans via program generation","authors":"Carlos G. Correa ,&nbsp;Sophia Sanborn ,&nbsp;Mark K. Ho ,&nbsp;Frederick Callaway ,&nbsp;Nathaniel D. Daw ,&nbsp;Thomas L. Griffiths","doi":"10.1016/j.cognition.2024.105990","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cognition.2024.105990","url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>Human behavior is often assumed to be hierarchically structured, made up of abstract actions that can be decomposed into concrete actions. However, behavior is typically measured as a sequence of actions, which makes it difficult to infer its hierarchical structure. In this paper, we explore how people form hierarchically structured plans, using an experimental paradigm with observable hierarchical representations: participants create programs that produce sequences of actions in a language with explicit hierarchical structure. This task lets us test two well-established principles of human behavior: utility maximization (i.e. using fewer actions) and minimum description length (MDL; i.e. having a shorter program). We find that humans are sensitive to both metrics, but that both accounts fail to predict a qualitative feature of human-created programs, namely that people prefer programs with <em>reuse</em> over and above the predictions of MDL. We formalize this preference for reuse by extending the MDL account into a generative model over programs, modeling hierarchy choice as the induction of a grammar over actions. Our account can explain the preference for reuse and provides better predictions of human behavior, going beyond simple accounts of compressibility to highlight a principle that guides hierarchical planning.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":48455,"journal":{"name":"Cognition","volume":"255 ","pages":"Article 105990"},"PeriodicalIF":2.8,"publicationDate":"2024-11-30","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142744998","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Why might there be lexical-prelexical feedback in speech recognition? 为什么在语音识别中会有词汇-词汇前反馈?
IF 2.8 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-11-30 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2024.106025
Dennis Norris , James M. McQueen
In reply to Magnuson, Crinnion, Luthra, Gaston, and Grubb (2023), we challenge their conclusion that on-line activation feedback improves word recognition. This type of feedback is instantiated in the TRACE model (McClelland & Elman, 1986) as top-down spread of activation from lexical to phoneme nodes. We give two main reasons why Magnuson et al.'s demonstration that activation feedback speeds up word recognition in TRACE is not informative about whether activation feedback helps humans recognize words. First, the same speed-up could be achieved by changing other parameters in TRACE. Second, more fundamentally, there is room for improvement in TRACE's performance only because the model, unlike Bayesian models, is suboptimal. We also challenge Magnuson et al.'s claim that the available empirical data support activation feedback. The data they base this claim on are open to alternative explanations and there are data against activation feedback that they do not discuss. We argue, therefore, that there are no computational or empirical grounds to conclude that activation feedback benefits human spoken-word recognition and indeed no theoretical grounds why activation feedback would exist. Other types of feedback, for example feedback to support perceptual learning, likely do exist, precisely because they can help listeners recognize words.
作为对Magnuson、crinion、Luthra、Gaston和Grubb(2023)的回应,我们对他们关于在线激活反馈提高单词识别的结论提出了质疑。这种类型的反馈在TRACE模型(McClelland &;Elman, 1986),从词汇节点到音素节点的自上而下的激活传播。Magnuson等人关于激活反馈在TRACE中加速单词识别的论证并不能说明激活反馈是否有助于人类识别单词,我们给出了两个主要原因。首先,可以通过更改TRACE中的其他参数来实现相同的加速。其次,更根本的是,TRACE的性能还有改进的空间,因为与贝叶斯模型不同,该模型不是最优的。我们也质疑Magnuson等人的说法,即现有的经验数据支持激活反馈。他们提出这一主张所依据的数据对其他解释是开放的,也有一些反对激活反馈的数据,他们没有讨论。因此,我们认为,没有计算或经验依据来得出激活反馈有利于人类口语识别的结论,实际上也没有理论依据来解释激活反馈为什么会存在。其他类型的反馈,例如支持感知学习的反馈,可能确实存在,正是因为它们可以帮助听者识别单词。
{"title":"Why might there be lexical-prelexical feedback in speech recognition?","authors":"Dennis Norris ,&nbsp;James M. McQueen","doi":"10.1016/j.cognition.2024.106025","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cognition.2024.106025","url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>In reply to Magnuson, Crinnion, Luthra, Gaston, and Grubb (2023), we challenge their conclusion that on-line activation feedback improves word recognition. This type of feedback is instantiated in the TRACE model (McClelland &amp; Elman, 1986) as top-down spread of activation from lexical to phoneme nodes. We give two main reasons why Magnuson et al.'s demonstration that activation feedback speeds up word recognition in TRACE is not informative about whether activation feedback helps humans recognize words. First, the same speed-up could be achieved by changing other parameters in TRACE. Second, more fundamentally, there is room for improvement in TRACE's performance only because the model, unlike Bayesian models, is suboptimal. We also challenge Magnuson et al.'s claim that the available empirical data support activation feedback. The data they base this claim on are open to alternative explanations and there are data against activation feedback that they do not discuss. We argue, therefore, that there are no computational or empirical grounds to conclude that activation feedback benefits human spoken-word recognition and indeed no theoretical grounds why activation feedback would exist. Other types of feedback, for example feedback to support perceptual learning, likely do exist, precisely because they can help listeners recognize words.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":48455,"journal":{"name":"Cognition","volume":"255 ","pages":"Article 106025"},"PeriodicalIF":2.8,"publicationDate":"2024-11-30","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142757346","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Refreshing the conversation about adaptation and perceived numerosity: A reply to Yousif, Clarke and Brannon 更新关于适应和感知数量的对话:对Yousif, Clarke和Brannon的回复
IF 2.8 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-11-30 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2024.105883
Frank H. Durgin
Yousif et al. (2024) have raised a number of pertinent objections to the idea that number adaptation is a straightforward account of the readily-observable aftereffects that affect perceived numerosity. Their criticisms appear well-motivated, but their particular version of the old-news proposal, involving specific dots, may be insufficiently abstract given that adaptation accumulates. Two new experiments are presented that are meant to buttress their critique by (1) confirming their predictions concerning neutral adaptation, and (2) re-evaluating related claims concerning number vs. density comparisons that have been widely accepted. Present behavioral evidence dissociating effects of adapter size, adapter number and adapter density, supports the idea that density adaptation is implicated as a primary source of most phenomenologically-compelling aftereffects of perceived numerosity.
Experiment 2 was preregistered on AsPredicted.org. The pre-registration is available at the following link: https://aspredicted.org/PC7_2ZB
The full raw data sets for the two experiments reported her are available on OSF at the following links:
Experiment 1: https://osf.io/b9qwy/?view_only=73beb62d9c2046c3aa08cdeb96cd5cca
Experiment 2: https://osf.io/6ax5j/?view_only=723ceb0b44da47dba99e56db12db02a9
Yousif等人(2024)提出了许多相关的反对意见,反对数字适应是对影响感知数量的可观察后果的直接解释。他们的批评似乎是有动机的,但他们对旧新闻提议的特殊版本,涉及具体的点,可能不够抽象,因为适应是累积的。提出了两个新的实验,旨在通过(1)证实他们关于中性适应的预测,以及(2)重新评估已被广泛接受的关于数量与密度比较的相关主张来支持他们的批评。目前的行为证据表明,适配器大小、适配器数量和适配器密度的分离效应,支持了密度适应是感知数量的大多数现象学上引人注目的后果的主要来源的观点。实验2在AsPredicted.org上进行了预注册。预注册可在以下链接获得:https://aspredicted.org/PC7_2ZBThe她报告的两个实验的完整原始数据集可在OSF上通过以下链接获得:实验1:https://osf.io/b9qwy/?view_only=73beb62d9c2046c3aa08cdeb96cd5ccaExperiment 2: https://osf.io/6ax5j/?view_only=723ceb0b44da47dba99e56db12db02a9
{"title":"Refreshing the conversation about adaptation and perceived numerosity: A reply to Yousif, Clarke and Brannon","authors":"Frank H. Durgin","doi":"10.1016/j.cognition.2024.105883","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cognition.2024.105883","url":null,"abstract":"<div><div><span><span>Yousif et al. (2024)</span></span> have raised a number of pertinent objections to the idea that number adaptation is a straightforward account of the readily-observable aftereffects that affect perceived numerosity. Their criticisms appear well-motivated, but their particular version of the old-news proposal, involving specific dots, may be insufficiently abstract given that adaptation accumulates. Two new experiments are presented that are meant to buttress their critique by (1) confirming their predictions concerning neutral adaptation, and (2) re-evaluating related claims concerning number vs. density comparisons that have been widely accepted. Present behavioral evidence dissociating effects of adapter size, adapter number and adapter density, supports the idea that density adaptation is implicated as a primary source of most phenomenologically-compelling aftereffects of perceived numerosity.</div><div>Experiment 2 was preregistered on <span><span>AsPredicted.org</span><svg><path></path></svg></span>. The pre-registration is available at the following link: <span><span>https://aspredicted.org/PC7_2ZB</span><svg><path></path></svg></span></div><div>The full raw data sets for the two experiments reported her are available on OSF at the following links:</div><div>Experiment 1: <span><span>https://osf.io/b9qwy/?view_only=73beb62d9c2046c3aa08cdeb96cd5cca</span><svg><path></path></svg></span></div><div>Experiment 2: <span><span>https://osf.io/6ax5j/?view_only=723ceb0b44da47dba99e56db12db02a9</span><svg><path></path></svg></span></div></div>","PeriodicalId":48455,"journal":{"name":"Cognition","volume":"254 ","pages":"Article 105883"},"PeriodicalIF":2.8,"publicationDate":"2024-11-30","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142744570","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The holistic forgetting of events and the (sometimes) fragmented forgetting of objects 对事件的整体遗忘和(有时)对对象的碎片遗忘
IF 2.8 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-11-29 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2024.106017
Nora Andermane , Arianna Moccia , Chong Zhai , Lisa M. Henderson , Aidan J. Horner
Episodic events are typically retrieved and forgotten holistically. If you recall one element (e.g., a person), you are more likely to recall other elements from the same event (e.g., the location), a pattern that is retained over time in the presence of forgetting. In contrast, representations of individual items, such as objects, may be less coherently bound, such that object features are forgotten at different rates and retrieval dependency decreases across delay. To test the theoretical prediction that forgetting qualitatively differs across levels in a representational hierarchy, we investigated the potential dissociation between event and item memory across five experiments. Participants encoded three-element events comprising images of famous people, locations, and objects. We measured retrieval accuracy and the dependency between the retrieval of event associations and object features, immediately after encoding and after various delays (5 h to 3 days). Across experiments, retrieval accuracy decreased for both events and objects over time, revealing forgetting. Retrieval dependency for event elements (i.e., people, locations, and objects) did not change over time, suggesting the holistic forgetting of events. Retrieval dependency for object features (i.e., state and colour) was more variable. Depending on encoding and delay conditions across the experiments, we observed both fragmentation and holistic forgetting of object features. Our results suggest that event representations remain coherent over time, whereas object representations can, but do not always, fragment. This provides support for our representational hierarchy framework of forgetting, however there are (still to be determined) boundary conditions in relation to the fragmentation of object representations.
情景性事件通常被整体地检索和遗忘。如果你回忆起一个要素(例如,一个人),你就更有可能回忆起同一事件的其他要素(例如,地点),这种模式在遗忘的情况下会随着时间的推移而保留。相反,单个项目(如对象)的表示可能不太连贯,因此对象特征会以不同的速率被遗忘,检索依赖关系会随着延迟而降低。为了验证遗忘在表征层次中不同层次的质量差异这一理论预测,我们通过五个实验研究了事件记忆和项目记忆之间的潜在分离。参与者对三要素事件进行编码,包括名人、地点和物体的图像。我们测量了检索精度和事件关联检索与对象特征之间的依赖关系,分别在编码后立即和经过各种延迟(5小时至3天)。在实验中,随着时间的推移,对事件和物体的检索准确度都在下降,这表明人们正在遗忘。事件元素(即人、地点和对象)的检索依赖关系没有随着时间的推移而改变,这表明事件的整体遗忘。对象特征(即状态和颜色)的检索依赖关系更加多变。根据实验的编码和延迟条件,我们观察到物体特征的碎片化和整体遗忘。我们的研究结果表明,随着时间的推移,事件表征保持连贯,而对象表征可以,但并不总是,碎片化。这为我们的遗忘表征层次框架提供了支持,然而,与对象表征的碎片化有关的边界条件(仍有待确定)。
{"title":"The holistic forgetting of events and the (sometimes) fragmented forgetting of objects","authors":"Nora Andermane ,&nbsp;Arianna Moccia ,&nbsp;Chong Zhai ,&nbsp;Lisa M. Henderson ,&nbsp;Aidan J. Horner","doi":"10.1016/j.cognition.2024.106017","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cognition.2024.106017","url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>Episodic events are typically retrieved and forgotten holistically. If you recall one element (e.g., a person), you are more likely to recall other elements from the same event (e.g., the location), a pattern that is retained over time in the presence of forgetting. In contrast, representations of individual items, such as objects, may be less coherently bound, such that object features are forgotten at different rates and retrieval dependency decreases across delay. To test the theoretical prediction that forgetting qualitatively differs across levels in a representational hierarchy, we investigated the potential dissociation between event and item memory across five experiments. Participants encoded three-element events comprising images of famous people, locations, and objects. We measured retrieval accuracy and the dependency between the retrieval of event associations and object features, immediately after encoding and after various delays (5 h to 3 days). Across experiments, retrieval accuracy decreased for both events and objects over time, revealing forgetting. Retrieval dependency for event elements (i.e., people, locations, and objects) did not change over time, suggesting the holistic forgetting of events. Retrieval dependency for object features (i.e., state and colour) was more variable. Depending on encoding and delay conditions across the experiments, we observed both fragmentation and holistic forgetting of object features. Our results suggest that event representations remain coherent over time, whereas object representations can, but do not always, fragment. This provides support for our representational hierarchy framework of forgetting, however there are (still to be determined) boundary conditions in relation to the fragmentation of object representations.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":48455,"journal":{"name":"Cognition","volume":"255 ","pages":"Article 106017"},"PeriodicalIF":2.8,"publicationDate":"2024-11-29","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142744997","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Prediction-based false memory: Unconfirmed prediction can result in robust false memories 基于预测的错误记忆:未经证实的预测可能导致强大的错误记忆
IF 2.8 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-11-28 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2024.106013
Olya Bulatova , Keisuke Fukuda
A growing body of literature suggests a powerful role of predictions on memory through prediction violation and prediction confirmation. Violation appears to enhance memory for the event violating the prediction, meanwhile, confirmation boosts memory for the predicted event instead. Crucially, however, the effect of prediction by itself has not been identified as it has typically been studied with its violation or confirmation. Here, we demonstrate the power of explicit predictions on memory by isolating it from its direct violation and confirmation. In a series of experiments, participants were presented with a real-world object along with three characters and they predicted which character the object belonged to. Upon prediction, participants received either visual confirmation (predicted character showing the item), visual rebuttal (another character showing the item) or no feedback (none of the characters showing the item) with regard to their prediction. When their memory was tested, participants were more likely to falsely remember that their predicted character showed them the item than the other characters did, even when no feedback was provided. This false memory was not eliminated by visual rebuttal and it was not weakened when participants had a strong item memory. Experiments 2–4 eliminated action (selecting a predicted character) as an alternative explanation and demonstrated that this prediction-based false memory could be modulated through indirect prediction confirmation and rebuttal. Taken together, our findings show that explicit predictions can be sufficient to induce false memory of predicted events that are robust enough to withstand its direct rebuttal.
越来越多的文献表明,通过预测违背和预测证实,预测在记忆中发挥着强大的作用。违背似乎增强了对违反预测的事件的记忆,同时,确认反而增强了对预测事件的记忆。然而,至关重要的是,预测本身的影响还没有被确定,因为它通常是与它的违反或证实一起研究的。在这里,我们通过将其从直接违反和确认中分离出来,证明了明确预测对记忆的作用。在一系列实验中,研究人员向参与者展示了一个现实世界中的物体和三个角色,让他们预测这个物体属于哪个角色。在预测之后,参与者要么收到视觉上的确认(预测的字符显示该项目),要么收到视觉上的反驳(另一个字符显示该项目),要么没有收到关于他们预测的反馈(没有字符显示该项目)。当他们的记忆被测试时,即使没有提供任何反馈,参与者也更有可能错误地记住他们预测的角色向他们展示了这个项目,而不是其他角色。这种错误记忆并没有被视觉反驳消除,当被试具有较强的项目记忆时,这种错误记忆也没有被削弱。实验2-4排除了行动(选择一个被预测的字符)作为另一种解释,并证明这种基于预测的错误记忆可以通过间接的预测确认和反驳来调节。综上所述,我们的研究结果表明,明确的预测足以诱发对预测事件的错误记忆,而这些错误记忆足够强大,足以承受其直接反驳。
{"title":"Prediction-based false memory: Unconfirmed prediction can result in robust false memories","authors":"Olya Bulatova ,&nbsp;Keisuke Fukuda","doi":"10.1016/j.cognition.2024.106013","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cognition.2024.106013","url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>A growing body of literature suggests a powerful role of predictions on memory through prediction violation and prediction confirmation. Violation appears to enhance memory for the event violating the prediction, meanwhile, confirmation boosts memory for the predicted event instead. Crucially, however, the effect of prediction by itself has not been identified as it has typically been studied with its violation or confirmation. Here, we demonstrate the power of explicit predictions on memory by isolating it from its direct violation and confirmation. In a series of experiments, participants were presented with a real-world object along with three characters and they predicted which character the object belonged to. Upon prediction, participants received either visual confirmation (predicted character showing the item), visual rebuttal (another character showing the item) or no feedback (none of the characters showing the item) with regard to their prediction. When their memory was tested, participants were more likely to falsely remember that their predicted character showed them the item than the other characters did, even when no feedback was provided. This false memory was not eliminated by visual rebuttal and it was not weakened when participants had a strong item memory. Experiments 2–4 eliminated action (selecting a predicted character) as an alternative explanation and demonstrated that this prediction-based false memory could be modulated through indirect prediction confirmation and rebuttal. Taken together, our findings show that explicit predictions can be sufficient to induce false memory of predicted events that are robust enough to withstand its direct rebuttal.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":48455,"journal":{"name":"Cognition","volume":"255 ","pages":"Article 106013"},"PeriodicalIF":2.8,"publicationDate":"2024-11-28","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142744996","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
How wise is the crowd: Can we infer people are accurate and competent merely because they agree with each other? 群众的智慧有多大:我们是否可以仅仅因为人们意见一致就推断他们是准确和称职的?
IF 2.8 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-11-26 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2024.106005
Jan Pfänder, Benoît De Courson, Hugo Mercier
Are people who agree on something more likely to be right and competent? Evidence suggests that people tend to make this inference. However, standard wisdom of crowds approaches only provide limited normative grounds. Using simulations and analytical arguments, we argue that when individuals make independent and unbiased estimates, under a wide range of parameters, individuals whose answers converge with each other tend to have more accurate answers and to be more competent. In 6 experiments (UK participants, total N = 1197), we show that participants infer that informants who agree have more accurate answers and are more competent, even when they have no priors, and that these inferences are weakened when the informants were systematically biased. In conclusion, we speculate that inferences from convergence to accuracy and competence might help explain why people deem scientists competent, even if they have little understanding of science.
在某些事情上意见一致的人更有可能是正确和称职的吗?有证据表明,人们倾向于做出这样的推断。然而,标准的群众智慧方法只能提供有限的规范性依据。通过模拟和分析论证,我们认为,当个体在各种参数下做出独立且无偏见的估计时,答案相互趋同的个体往往会得到更准确的答案,也更有能力。在 6 个实验中(英国参与者,总人数 = 1197),我们表明,即使参与者没有先验,他们也会推断出意见一致的信息提供者的答案更准确、能力更强,而当信息提供者存在系统性偏见时,这些推断就会被削弱。总之,我们推测,对准确性和能力的趋同推断可能有助于解释为什么人们认为科学家是称职的,即使他们对科学知之甚少。
{"title":"How wise is the crowd: Can we infer people are accurate and competent merely because they agree with each other?","authors":"Jan Pfänder,&nbsp;Benoît De Courson,&nbsp;Hugo Mercier","doi":"10.1016/j.cognition.2024.106005","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cognition.2024.106005","url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>Are people who agree on something more likely to be right and competent? Evidence suggests that people tend to make this inference. However, standard wisdom of crowds approaches only provide limited normative grounds. Using simulations and analytical arguments, we argue that when individuals make independent and unbiased estimates, under a wide range of parameters, individuals whose answers converge with each other tend to have more accurate answers and to be more competent. In 6 experiments (UK participants, total N = 1197), we show that participants infer that informants who agree have more accurate answers and are more competent, even when they have no priors, and that these inferences are weakened when the informants were systematically biased. In conclusion, we speculate that inferences from convergence to accuracy and competence might help explain why people deem scientists competent, even if they have little understanding of science.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":48455,"journal":{"name":"Cognition","volume":"255 ","pages":"Article 106005"},"PeriodicalIF":2.8,"publicationDate":"2024-11-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142722554","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Attention-aware semantic relevance predicting Chinese sentence reading 注意力感知语义相关性预测中文句子阅读
IF 2.8 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-11-26 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2024.105991
Kun Sun , Haitao Liu
In recent years, several influential computational models and metrics have been proposed to predict how humans comprehend and process sentence. One particularly promising approach is contextual semantic similarity. Inspired by the attention algorithm in Transformer and human memory mechanisms, this study proposes an “attention-aware” approach for computing contextual semantic relevance. This new approach takes into account the different contributions of contextual parts and the expectation effect, allowing it to incorporate contextual information fully. The attention-aware approach also facilitates the simulation of existing reading models and their evaluation. The resulting “attention-aware” metrics of semantic relevance can more accurately predict fixation durations in Chinese reading tasks recorded in an eye-tracking corpus than those calculated by existing approaches. The study’s findings further provide strong support for the presence of semantic preview benefits in Chinese naturalistic reading. Furthermore, the attention-aware metrics of semantic relevance, being memory-based, possess high interpretability from both linguistic and cognitive standpoints, making them a valuable computational tool for modeling eye-movements in reading and further gaining insight into the process of language comprehension. Our approach emphasizes the potential of these metrics to advance our understanding of how humans comprehend and process language.
近年来,一些有影响力的计算模型和衡量标准被提出来预测人类如何理解和处理句子。其中,语境语义相似性是一种特别有前途的方法。受 Transformer 中注意力算法和人类记忆机制的启发,本研究提出了一种计算上下文语义相关性的 "注意力感知 "方法。这种新方法考虑到了上下文部分的不同贡献和期望效应,从而能够充分纳入上下文信息。注意力感知方法还有助于模拟现有的阅读模型并对其进行评估。与现有方法相比,"注意力感知 "方法得出的语义相关性指标能更准确地预测眼动语料库中记录的中文阅读任务中的定格持续时间。研究结果进一步为中文自然阅读中语义预览效益的存在提供了有力支持。此外,语义相关性的注意力感知指标基于记忆,从语言学和认知学的角度来看都具有很高的可解释性,使其成为阅读中眼动建模的重要计算工具,并能进一步深入了解语言理解的过程。我们的方法强调了这些指标在促进我们理解人类如何理解和处理语言方面的潜力。
{"title":"Attention-aware semantic relevance predicting Chinese sentence reading","authors":"Kun Sun ,&nbsp;Haitao Liu","doi":"10.1016/j.cognition.2024.105991","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cognition.2024.105991","url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>In recent years, several influential computational models and metrics have been proposed to predict how humans comprehend and process sentence. One particularly promising approach is contextual semantic similarity. Inspired by the attention algorithm in Transformer and human memory mechanisms, this study proposes an “attention-aware” approach for computing contextual semantic relevance. This new approach takes into account the different contributions of contextual parts and the expectation effect, allowing it to incorporate contextual information fully. The attention-aware approach also facilitates the simulation of existing reading models and their evaluation. The resulting “attention-aware” metrics of semantic relevance can more accurately predict fixation durations in Chinese reading tasks recorded in an eye-tracking corpus than those calculated by existing approaches. The study’s findings further provide strong support for the presence of semantic preview benefits in Chinese naturalistic reading. Furthermore, the attention-aware metrics of semantic relevance, being memory-based, possess high interpretability from both linguistic and cognitive standpoints, making them a valuable computational tool for modeling eye-movements in reading and further gaining insight into the process of language comprehension. Our approach emphasizes the potential of these metrics to advance our understanding of how humans comprehend and process language.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":48455,"journal":{"name":"Cognition","volume":"255 ","pages":"Article 105991"},"PeriodicalIF":2.8,"publicationDate":"2024-11-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142722553","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The role of exceptions in children's and adults' judgments about generic statements 例外情况在儿童和成人对一般陈述的判断中的作用。
IF 2.8 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-11-23 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2024.106016
Ella Simmons, Susan A. Gelman
Generic statements (e.g., “Ducks lay eggs”) provide generalizations about kinds that can be judged as true, even in the face of exceptions. Although past research has focused on the positive evidence that justifies a generic, little work has explored the role of evidence that does not match the generic claim (e.g., ducks that do not lay eggs). The current studies aim to understand how different types of exceptions may differentially undermine generic claims. In Studies 1 and 2, adults (n = 560) and children ages 5–11 (n = 141) were asked to judge the truth of generic statements about fictitious animal kinds (e.g., Wugs have blue horns). Accompanying each statement was a set of 6 kind members, some of which displayed the target property (e.g., blue horns), and others of which displayed either an alternative property (e.g., red horns), or an absence of the property (e.g., no horns). Study 1 found that adults were less likely to endorse generic statements when non-matching examples displayed an alternative property than when they displayed an absence of the property. Study 2 indicated that children as well as adults were less likely to endorse generic statements when presented with alternative evidence, regardless of the salience of the alternative. Study 3 replicated these findings with a more sensitive task in which adults (n = 120) and children (n = 97) were asked to choose between sets with either alternative or absence evidence. These studies provide the first evidence that children and adults attend to non-matching evidence when making judgments about generic statements, interpret alternative evidence to be stronger counterevidence than absence evidence, and do not use the salience of alternative properties to determine the strength of alternative evidence. We discuss the implications of this work for problematic generic claims in language and thought.
一般陈述(如 "鸭子会下蛋")提供了关于种类的概括,即使面对例外情况,也可以判断为真实的。尽管过去的研究主要集中在证明通说正确的正面证据上,但很少有研究探讨与通说不符的证据(如鸭子不下蛋)的作用。目前的研究旨在了解不同类型的例外情况会如何不同程度地削弱通称。在研究 1 和研究 2 中,研究人员要求成人(n = 560)和 5-11 岁儿童(n = 141)判断关于虚构动物种类的一般性陈述(例如,乌龟有蓝色的角)的真实性。每个陈述都附有一组 6 个种类的成员,其中一些显示目标属性(如蓝色角),另一些显示替代属性(如红色角)或无属性(如无角)。研究 1 发现,当非匹配示例显示替代属性时,成人认可通用语句的可能性低于显示无属性时。研究 2 表明,当出现替代证据时,无论替代证据的显著性如何,儿童和成人都不太可能认可一般陈述。研究 3 通过一项更敏感的任务重复了这些发现,在这项任务中,成人(n = 120)和儿童(n = 97)被要求在有替代证据或无替代证据的集合中做出选择。这些研究首次证明,儿童和成人在对一般陈述进行判断时会注意非匹配证据,将替代证据解释为比缺失证据更有力的反证,并且不会使用替代属性的显著性来确定替代证据的强度。我们将讨论这项工作对语言和思维中存在问题的一般说法的影响。
{"title":"The role of exceptions in children's and adults' judgments about generic statements","authors":"Ella Simmons,&nbsp;Susan A. Gelman","doi":"10.1016/j.cognition.2024.106016","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cognition.2024.106016","url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>Generic statements (e.g., “Ducks lay eggs”) provide generalizations about kinds that can be judged as true, even in the face of exceptions. Although past research has focused on the positive evidence that justifies a generic, little work has explored the role of evidence that does not match the generic claim (e.g., ducks that do not lay eggs). The current studies aim to understand how different types of exceptions may differentially undermine generic claims. In Studies 1 and 2, adults (<em>n</em> = 560) and children ages 5–11 (<em>n</em> = 141) were asked to judge the truth of generic statements about fictitious animal kinds (e.g., Wugs have blue horns). Accompanying each statement was a set of 6 kind members, some of which displayed the target property (e.g., blue horns), and others of which displayed either an alternative property (e.g., red horns), or an absence of the property (e.g., no horns). Study 1 found that adults were less likely to endorse generic statements when non-matching examples displayed an alternative property than when they displayed an absence of the property. Study 2 indicated that children as well as adults were less likely to endorse generic statements when presented with alternative evidence, regardless of the salience of the alternative. Study 3 replicated these findings with a more sensitive task in which adults (<em>n =</em> 120) and children (<em>n</em> = 97) were asked to choose between sets with either alternative or absence evidence. These studies provide the first evidence that children and adults attend to non-matching evidence when making judgments about generic statements, interpret alternative evidence to be stronger counterevidence than absence evidence, and do not use the salience of alternative properties to determine the strength of alternative evidence. We discuss the implications of this work for problematic generic claims in language and thought.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":48455,"journal":{"name":"Cognition","volume":"255 ","pages":"Article 106016"},"PeriodicalIF":2.8,"publicationDate":"2024-11-23","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142695917","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Cognition
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1