首页 > 最新文献

Cognition最新文献

英文 中文
Social perception of animacy: Preferential attentional orienting to animals links with autistic traits 对动物的社会认知:对动物的优先注意定向与自闭症特征有关。
IF 2.8 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-07-23 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2024.105900
Geqing Yang , Ying Wang , Yi Jiang

Animate cues enjoy priority in attentional processes as they carry survival-relevant information and herald social interaction. Whether and in what way such an attention effect is associated with more general aspects of social cognition remains largely unexplored. Here we investigated whether the attentional preference for animals varies with observers' autistic traits — an indicator of autism-like characteristics in general populations related to one's social cognitive abilities. Using the dot-probe paradigm, we found that animal cues can rapidly and persistently recruit preferential attention over inanimate ones in observers with relatively low, but not high, autistic traits, as measured by Autism-Spectrum Quotient (AQ). Moreover, individual AQ scores were negatively correlated with the attentional bias toward animals, especially at the early orienting stage. These results were not simply due to low-level visual factors, as inverted or phase-scrambled pictures did not yield a similar pattern. Our findings demonstrate an automatic and enduring attentional bias beneficial to both rapid detection and continuous monitoring of animals and reveal its link with autistic traits, highlighting the critical role of animacy perception in the architecture of social cognition.

生物线索在注意过程中享有优先权,因为它们承载着与生存相关的信息,并预示着社会互动。这种注意效应是否以及以何种方式与社会认知的更多方面相关联,在很大程度上仍未得到探讨。在这里,我们研究了对动物的注意偏好是否会随着观察者的自闭症特征而变化--自闭症特征是与一个人的社会认知能力相关的普通人群中的一个指标。通过使用点探测范式,我们发现在自闭症谱系商数(AQ)衡量的自闭症特质相对较低而非较高的观察者中,动物线索能迅速而持久地吸引他们对无生命线索的优先注意。此外,个体的自闭症谱系商数得分与对动物的注意偏向呈负相关,尤其是在早期定向阶段。这些结果并不仅仅是低级视觉因素造成的,因为倒置或相位乱码图片并没有产生类似的模式。我们的研究结果表明,一种自动且持久的注意偏向有利于快速检测和持续监控动物,并揭示了它与自闭症特征之间的联系,突出了动物感知在社会认知结构中的关键作用。
{"title":"Social perception of animacy: Preferential attentional orienting to animals links with autistic traits","authors":"Geqing Yang ,&nbsp;Ying Wang ,&nbsp;Yi Jiang","doi":"10.1016/j.cognition.2024.105900","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cognition.2024.105900","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Animate cues enjoy priority in attentional processes as they carry survival-relevant information and herald social interaction. Whether and in what way such an attention effect is associated with more general aspects of social cognition remains largely unexplored. Here we investigated whether the attentional preference for animals varies with observers' autistic traits — an indicator of autism-like characteristics in general populations related to one's social cognitive abilities. Using the dot-probe paradigm, we found that animal cues can rapidly and persistently recruit preferential attention over inanimate ones in observers with relatively low, but not high, autistic traits, as measured by Autism-Spectrum Quotient (AQ). Moreover, individual AQ scores were negatively correlated with the attentional bias toward animals, especially at the early orienting stage. These results were not simply due to low-level visual factors, as inverted or phase-scrambled pictures did not yield a similar pattern. Our findings demonstrate an automatic and enduring attentional bias beneficial to both rapid detection and continuous monitoring of animals and reveal its link with autistic traits, highlighting the critical role of animacy perception in the architecture of social cognition.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48455,"journal":{"name":"Cognition","volume":"251 ","pages":"Article 105900"},"PeriodicalIF":2.8,"publicationDate":"2024-07-23","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141761793","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Using network science to provide insights into the structure of event knowledge 利用网络科学深入了解事件知识的结构。
IF 2.8 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-07-23 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2024.105845
Kevin S. Brown , Kara E. Hannah , Nickolas Christidis , Mikayla Hall-Bruce , Ryan A. Stevenson , Jeffrey L. Elman , Ken McRae

The structure of event knowledge plays a critical role in prediction, reconstruction of memory for personal events, construction of possible future events, action, language usage, and social interactions. Despite numerous theoretical proposals such as scripts, schemas, and stories, the highly variable and rich nature of events and event knowledge have been formidable barriers to characterizing the structure of event knowledge in memory. We used network science to provide insights into the temporal structure of common events. Based on participants' production and ordering of the activities that make up events, we established empirical profiles for 80 common events to characterize the temporal structure of activities. We used the event networks to investigate multiple issues regarding the variability in the richness and complexity of people's knowledge of common events, including: the temporal structure of events; event prototypes that might emerge from learning across many experiential instances and be expressed by people; the degree to which scenes (communities) are present in various events; the degree to which people believe certain activities are central to an event; how centrality might be distributed across an event's activities; and similarities among events in terms of their content and their temporal structure. Thus, we provide novel insights into human event knowledge, and describe 18 predictions for future human studies.

事件知识的结构在预测、重建个人事件记忆、构建未来可能发生的事件、行动、语言使用和社会交往中起着至关重要的作用。尽管有脚本、图式和故事等众多理论建议,但事件和事件知识的多变性和丰富性一直是描述记忆中事件知识结构的巨大障碍。我们利用网络科学来深入了解常见事件的时间结构。根据参与者对构成事件的活动的制作和排序,我们为 80 个常见事件建立了经验档案,以描述活动的时间结构。我们利用事件网络研究了有关人们对常见事件的知识的丰富性和复杂性的变化的多个问题,包括:事件的时间结构;可能从许多经验实例的学习中产生并由人们表达的事件原型;场景(社区)在各种事件中的存在程度;人们认为某些活动是事件的中心的程度;中心性如何在事件的活动中分布;以及事件在内容和时间结构方面的相似性。因此,我们提供了对人类事件知识的新见解,并描述了对未来人类研究的 18 项预测。
{"title":"Using network science to provide insights into the structure of event knowledge","authors":"Kevin S. Brown ,&nbsp;Kara E. Hannah ,&nbsp;Nickolas Christidis ,&nbsp;Mikayla Hall-Bruce ,&nbsp;Ryan A. Stevenson ,&nbsp;Jeffrey L. Elman ,&nbsp;Ken McRae","doi":"10.1016/j.cognition.2024.105845","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cognition.2024.105845","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>The structure of event knowledge plays a critical role in prediction, reconstruction of memory for personal events, construction of possible future events, action, language usage, and social interactions. Despite numerous theoretical proposals such as scripts, schemas, and stories, the highly variable and rich nature of events and event knowledge have been formidable barriers to characterizing the structure of event knowledge in memory. We used network science to provide insights into the temporal structure of common events. Based on participants' production and ordering of the activities that make up events, we established empirical profiles for 80 common events to characterize the temporal structure of activities. We used the event networks to investigate multiple issues regarding the variability in the richness and complexity of people's knowledge of common events, including: the temporal structure of events; event prototypes that might emerge from learning across many experiential instances and be expressed by people; the degree to which scenes (communities) are present in various events; the degree to which people believe certain activities are central to an event; how centrality might be distributed across an event's activities; and similarities among events in terms of their content and their temporal structure. Thus, we provide novel insights into human event knowledge, and describe 18 predictions for future human studies.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48455,"journal":{"name":"Cognition","volume":"251 ","pages":"Article 105845"},"PeriodicalIF":2.8,"publicationDate":"2024-07-23","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141761795","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Storytelling changes the content and perceived value of event memories 讲故事能改变事件记忆的内容和感知价值。
IF 2.8 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-07-23 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2024.105884
Devlin Eckardt , Chelsea Helion , Helen Schmidt , Janice Chen , Vishnu P. Murty

Memories are not only stored for personal recall, but also to communicate knowledge to others in service of adaptive decision-making. Prior research shows that goals to share information can change which content is communicated in memory as well as the linguistic style embedded in this communication. Yet, little is known as to how communication-related alterations in memory narration drive differences of value processing in listeners. Here, we test how memory communication alters multi-featural recall for complex events and the downstream consequence on value estimations in naïve listeners. Participants recalled a memory of playing an exploratory videogame at a 24-h delay under instructions to either share (i.e., social condition) or recall (i.e., control condition) their memory. Sharing goals systematically altered the content and linguistic style of recall, such that narrators from the social condition were biased towards recall of non-episodic details and communicated their memories with more clout, less formality, and less authenticity. Across two independent samples of naïve listeners, these features differentially influenced value estimations of the video game. We found that greater clout was associated with greater enjoyment while listening to memories (hedonic value), and that greater inclusion of non-episodic details resulted in greater willingness to purchase the video game (motivational drive). These findings indicate that sharing an experience as a story can change the content and linguistic tone of memory recall, which in turn shape perceived value in naïve listeners.

记忆的存储不仅是为了个人回忆,也是为了向他人传递知识,以服务于适应性决策。先前的研究表明,分享信息的目标会改变记忆中的交流内容以及交流中的语言风格。然而,对于记忆叙述中与交流相关的改变如何驱动听者的价值加工差异,我们却知之甚少。在这里,我们测试了记忆交流如何改变复杂事件的多自然回忆,以及其对天真听者价值估计的下游影响。受试者在分享(即社交条件)或回忆(即控制条件)其记忆的指令下,延迟 24 小时回忆玩探索性电子游戏的记忆。分享目标系统性地改变了回忆的内容和语言风格,因此社交条件下的叙述者偏向于回忆非情节性细节,并以更大的影响力、更低的形式感和更低的真实性来传达他们的回忆。在两个独立的天真听者样本中,这些特征对视频游戏的价值评估产生了不同的影响。我们发现,更大的影响力与听者在聆听回忆时更大的愉悦感(享乐价值)相关,而更多地包含非情节细节则会使听者更愿意购买视频游戏(动机驱动)。这些研究结果表明,以故事的形式分享经历可以改变记忆回忆的内容和语言基调,进而影响天真的听众的感知价值。
{"title":"Storytelling changes the content and perceived value of event memories","authors":"Devlin Eckardt ,&nbsp;Chelsea Helion ,&nbsp;Helen Schmidt ,&nbsp;Janice Chen ,&nbsp;Vishnu P. Murty","doi":"10.1016/j.cognition.2024.105884","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cognition.2024.105884","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Memories are not only stored for personal recall, but also to communicate knowledge to others in service of adaptive decision-making. Prior research shows that goals to share information can change which content is communicated in memory as well as the linguistic style embedded in this communication. Yet, little is known as to how communication-related alterations in memory narration drive differences of value processing in listeners. Here, we test how memory communication alters multi-featural recall for complex events and the downstream consequence on value estimations in naïve listeners. Participants recalled a memory of playing an exploratory videogame at a 24-h delay under instructions to either share (i.e., social condition) or recall (i.e., control condition) their memory. Sharing goals systematically altered the content and linguistic style of recall, such that narrators from the social condition were biased towards recall of non-episodic details and communicated their memories with more clout, less formality, and less authenticity. Across two independent samples of naïve listeners, these features differentially influenced value estimations of the video game. We found that greater clout was associated with greater enjoyment while listening to memories (hedonic value), and that greater inclusion of non-episodic details resulted in greater willingness to purchase the video game (motivational drive). These findings indicate that sharing an experience as a story can change the content and linguistic tone of memory recall, which in turn shape perceived value in naïve listeners.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48455,"journal":{"name":"Cognition","volume":"251 ","pages":"Article 105884"},"PeriodicalIF":2.8,"publicationDate":"2024-07-23","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141761794","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Risky effort 有风险的努力
IF 2.8 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-07-20 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2024.105895
Alice Mason , Yongming Sun , Nick Simonsen , Christopher R. Madan , Marcia L. Spetch , Elliot A. Ludvig

Decision-making involves weighing up the outcome likelihood, potential rewards, and effort needed. Previous research has focused on the trade-offs between risk and reward or between effort and reward. Here we bridge this gap and examine how risk in effort levels influences choice. We focus on how two key properties of choice influence risk preferences for effort: changes in magnitude and probability. Two experiments assessed people's risk attitudes for effort, and an additional experiment provided a control condition using monetary gambles. The extent to which people valued effort was related to their pattern of risk preferences. Unlike with monetary outcomes, however, there was substantial heterogeneity in effort-based risk preferences: People who responded to effort as costly exhibited a “flipped” interaction pattern of risk preferences. The direction of the pattern depended on whether people treated effort as a loss of resources. Most, but not all, people treat effort as a loss and are more willing to take risks to avoid potentially high levels of effort.

决策涉及权衡结果的可能性、潜在回报和所需付出的努力。以往的研究主要集中在风险与回报或努力与回报之间的权衡。在这里,我们弥补了这一空白,并研究了努力水平的风险如何影响选择。我们关注选择的两个关键属性如何影响努力的风险偏好:幅度和概率的变化。有两个实验评估了人们对努力的风险态度,还有一个实验提供了一个使用货币赌博的对照条件。人们对努力的重视程度与他们的风险偏好模式有关。然而,与货币结果不同的是,基于努力的风险偏好存在很大的异质性:认为付出代价高的人表现出一种 "翻转的 "风险偏好互动模式。这种模式的方向取决于人们是否将努力视为一种资源损失。大多数人,但不是所有人,都把努力视为一种损失,并且更愿意承担风险,以避免潜在的高水平努力。
{"title":"Risky effort","authors":"Alice Mason ,&nbsp;Yongming Sun ,&nbsp;Nick Simonsen ,&nbsp;Christopher R. Madan ,&nbsp;Marcia L. Spetch ,&nbsp;Elliot A. Ludvig","doi":"10.1016/j.cognition.2024.105895","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cognition.2024.105895","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Decision-making involves weighing up the outcome likelihood, potential rewards, and effort needed. Previous research has focused on the trade-offs between risk and reward or between effort and reward. Here we bridge this gap and examine how risk in effort levels influences choice. We focus on how two key properties of choice influence risk preferences for effort: changes in magnitude and probability. Two experiments assessed people's risk attitudes for effort, and an additional experiment provided a control condition using monetary gambles. The extent to which people valued effort was related to their pattern of risk preferences. Unlike with monetary outcomes, however, there was substantial heterogeneity in effort-based risk preferences: People who responded to effort as costly exhibited a “flipped” interaction pattern of risk preferences. The direction of the pattern depended on whether people treated effort as a loss of resources. Most, but not all, people treat effort as a loss and are more willing to take risks to avoid potentially high levels of effort.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48455,"journal":{"name":"Cognition","volume":"251 ","pages":"Article 105895"},"PeriodicalIF":2.8,"publicationDate":"2024-07-20","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0010027724001811/pdfft?md5=9522da9b374442c14110aa993beff1a9&pid=1-s2.0-S0010027724001811-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141732219","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Idiosyncratic and shared contributions shape impressions from voices and faces 杂乱无章的共同贡献塑造了声音和面孔给人的印象
IF 2.8 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-07-18 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2024.105881
Nadine Lavan , Clare A.M. Sutherland

Voices elicit rich first impressions of what the person we are hearing might be like. Research stresses that these impressions from voices are shared across different listeners, such that people on average agree which voices sound trustworthy or old and which do not. However, can impressions from voices also be shaped by the ‘ear of the beholder’? We investigated whether - and how - listeners' idiosyncratic, personal preferences contribute to first impressions from voices. In two studies (993 participants, 156 voices), we find evidence for substantial idiosyncratic contributions to voice impressions using a variance portioning approach. Overall, idiosyncratic contributions were as important as shared contributions to impressions from voices for inferred person characteristics (e.g., trustworthiness, friendliness). Shared contributions were only more influential for impressions of more directly apparent person characteristics (e.g., gender, age). Both idiosyncratic and shared contributions were reduced when stimuli were limited in their (perceived) variability, suggesting that natural variation in voices is key to understanding this impression formation. When comparing voice impressions to face impressions, we found that idiosyncratic and shared contributions to impressions similarly across modality when stimulus properties are closely matched - although voice impressions were overall less consistent than face impressions. We thus reconceptualise impressions from voices as being formed not only based on shared but also idiosyncratic contributions. We use this new framing to suggest future directions of research, including understanding idiosyncratic mechanisms, development, and malleability of voice impression formation.

声音会给我们留下丰富的第一印象,让我们知道听到的人可能是什么样的。研究强调,声音给不同听者留下的印象是相同的,例如,人们平均会认为哪些声音听起来可信或古老,哪些不可信。然而,"听者之耳 "是否也能塑造声音给人的印象呢?我们研究了听众的特异性个人偏好是否以及如何影响声音的第一印象。在两项研究(993 名参与者,156 种声音)中,我们发现了使用方差分配法对声音印象产生重大特异性影响的证据。总体而言,在推断人物特征(如可信度、友好度)方面,特异性贡献与共同贡献一样重要。共同贡献只对更直接明显的个人特征(如性别、年龄)的印象更有影响力。当刺激物的(可感知的)可变性受到限制时,特异性和共同贡献都会减少,这表明声音的自然变化是理解这种印象形成的关键。在将声音印象与脸部印象进行比较时,我们发现,当刺激物的属性密切匹配时,特异性和共享性对印象的贡献在不同模态下相似--尽管声音印象总体上不如脸部印象那么一致。因此,我们重新认识了声音印象,认为声音印象的形成不仅基于共同贡献,也基于特异贡献。我们利用这一新框架提出了未来的研究方向,包括了解声音印象形成的特异机制、发展和可塑性。
{"title":"Idiosyncratic and shared contributions shape impressions from voices and faces","authors":"Nadine Lavan ,&nbsp;Clare A.M. Sutherland","doi":"10.1016/j.cognition.2024.105881","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cognition.2024.105881","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Voices elicit rich first impressions of what the person we are hearing might be like. Research stresses that these impressions from voices are shared across different listeners, such that people on average agree which voices sound trustworthy or old and which do not. However, can impressions from voices also be shaped by the ‘ear of the beholder’? We investigated whether - and how - listeners' idiosyncratic, personal preferences contribute to first impressions from voices. In two studies (993 participants, 156 voices), we find evidence for substantial idiosyncratic contributions to voice impressions using a variance portioning approach. Overall, idiosyncratic contributions were as important as shared contributions to impressions from voices for inferred person characteristics (e.g., trustworthiness, friendliness). Shared contributions were only more influential for impressions of more directly apparent person characteristics (e.g., gender, age). Both idiosyncratic and shared contributions were reduced when stimuli were limited in their (perceived) variability, suggesting that natural variation in voices is key to understanding this impression formation. When comparing voice impressions to face impressions, we found that idiosyncratic and shared contributions to impressions similarly across modality when stimulus properties are closely matched - although voice impressions were overall less consistent than face impressions. We thus reconceptualise impressions from voices as being formed not only based on shared but also idiosyncratic contributions. We use this new framing to suggest future directions of research, including understanding idiosyncratic mechanisms, development, and malleability of voice impression formation.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48455,"journal":{"name":"Cognition","volume":"251 ","pages":"Article 105881"},"PeriodicalIF":2.8,"publicationDate":"2024-07-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0010027724001677/pdfft?md5=c27e3662ec3aed988721c7abecb6f69d&pid=1-s2.0-S0010027724001677-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141637460","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The link between non-human primate vocalizations and cognition is not constrained by maturation alone: Evidence from healthy preterm infants 非人灵长类动物的发声与认知之间的联系并不仅仅受限于成熟度:来自健康早产儿的证据
IF 2.8 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-07-18 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2024.105886
Kali Woodruff Carr , Sandra R. Waxman

To acquire language, infants must not only identify the signals of their language(s), but also discover how these signals are connected to meaning. By 3 months of age, infants' native language, non-native languages, and vocalizations of non-human primates support infants' formation of object categories—a building block of cognition. But by 6 months, only the native language exerts this cognitive advantage. Prior work with preterm infants indicates that maturation constrains this developing link between the native language and cognition. Here, we assess whether maturation exerts similar constraints on the influence of non-human primate vocalizations on infant categorization. Cross-sectional growth curve analyses of new data from preterm infants and extant data from fullterm infants indicate that developmental tuning of this signal's influence on categorization is best predicted by infants' chronological age, and not gestational status. This evidence, together with prior work, suggests that as infants tune the initially broad set of signals that support early cognition, they are guided by two independent processes: maturation constrains the expression of a link between their native language and cognition, while the influence of non-linguistic signals are guided by other factors, such as postnatal age and experience.

要掌握语言,婴儿不仅要识别其语言的信号,还要发现这些信号是如何与意义相联系的。3 个月大时,婴儿的母语、非母语和非人灵长类动物的发声都有助于婴儿形成对象类别--认知的基石。但到 6 个月大时,只有母语才能发挥这种认知优势。之前对早产儿的研究表明,成熟会制约母语与认知之间的这种联系。在此,我们评估了成熟是否会对非人灵长类发声对婴儿分类的影响产生类似的限制。对早产儿的新数据和足月儿的现有数据进行的横截面生长曲线分析表明,婴儿的实际年龄而不是胎龄最能预测非人灵长类发声对分类的影响。这一证据以及之前的研究表明,当婴儿调整最初支持早期认知的一系列信号时,他们受到两个独立过程的引导:成熟限制了其母语与认知之间联系的表达,而非语言信号的影响则受到其他因素的引导,如出生后的年龄和经验。
{"title":"The link between non-human primate vocalizations and cognition is not constrained by maturation alone: Evidence from healthy preterm infants","authors":"Kali Woodruff Carr ,&nbsp;Sandra R. Waxman","doi":"10.1016/j.cognition.2024.105886","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cognition.2024.105886","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>To acquire language, infants must not only identify the signals of their language(s), but also discover how these signals are connected to meaning. By 3 months of age, infants' native language, non-native languages, and vocalizations of non-human primates support infants' formation of object categories—a building block of cognition. But by 6 months, only the native language exerts this cognitive advantage. Prior work with preterm infants indicates that maturation constrains this developing link between the native language and cognition. Here, we assess whether maturation exerts similar constraints on the influence of non-human primate vocalizations on infant categorization. Cross-sectional growth curve analyses of new data from preterm infants and extant data from fullterm infants indicate that developmental tuning of this signal's influence on categorization is best predicted by infants' chronological age, and not gestational status. This evidence, together with prior work, suggests that as infants tune the initially broad set of signals that support early cognition, they are guided by two independent processes: maturation constrains the expression of a link between their native language and cognition, while the influence of non-linguistic signals are guided by other factors, such as postnatal age and experience.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48455,"journal":{"name":"Cognition","volume":"251 ","pages":"Article 105886"},"PeriodicalIF":2.8,"publicationDate":"2024-07-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141637462","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
On humans' (explicit) intuitions about the meaning of novel words 关于人类对新词含义的(明确)直觉
IF 2.8 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-07-17 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2024.105882
Daniele Gatti , Francesca Rodio , Luca Rinaldi , Marco Marelli

Pseudowords offer a unique opportunity to investigate how humans deal with new (verbal) information. Within this framework, previous studies have shown that, at the implicit level, humans exploit systematic associations in the form-meaning interface to process new information by relying on (sub-lexical) contents already mapped in semantic memory. However, whether speakers exploit such processes in explicit decisions about the meanings elicited by unfamiliar terms remains an open, important question. Here, we tested this by leveraging computational models that are able to induce semantic representations for out-of-vocabulary stimuli. Across two experiments, we demonstrate that participants' guesses about pseudoword meanings in a 2AFC task consistently align with the model's predictions. This indicates that humans' ability to extract meaningful knowledge from complex statistical patterns can affect explicit decisions.

伪词为研究人类如何处理新的(言语)信息提供了一个独特的机会。在这一框架内,以往的研究表明,在内隐层面上,人类利用形式-意义界面中的系统关联,依靠语义记忆中已经映射的(次词汇)内容来处理新信息。然而,说话者在明确决定陌生词语的含义时是否利用了这种过程,这仍然是一个有待解决的重要问题。在这里,我们利用能够为词汇外刺激诱导语义表征的计算模型,对这一问题进行了测试。在两个实验中,我们证明了参与者在 2AFC 任务中对假词义的猜测与模型的预测一致。这表明,人类从复杂的统计模式中提取有意义知识的能力可以影响明确的决策。
{"title":"On humans' (explicit) intuitions about the meaning of novel words","authors":"Daniele Gatti ,&nbsp;Francesca Rodio ,&nbsp;Luca Rinaldi ,&nbsp;Marco Marelli","doi":"10.1016/j.cognition.2024.105882","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cognition.2024.105882","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Pseudowords offer a unique opportunity to investigate how humans deal with new (verbal) information. Within this framework, previous studies have shown that, at the implicit level, humans exploit systematic associations in the form-meaning interface to process new information by relying on (sub-lexical) contents already mapped in semantic memory. However, whether speakers exploit such processes in explicit decisions about the meanings elicited by unfamiliar terms remains an open, important question. Here, we tested this by leveraging computational models that are able to induce semantic representations for out-of-vocabulary stimuli. Across two experiments, we demonstrate that participants' guesses about pseudoword meanings in a 2AFC task consistently align with the model's predictions. This indicates that humans' ability to extract meaningful knowledge from complex statistical patterns can affect explicit decisions.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48455,"journal":{"name":"Cognition","volume":"251 ","pages":"Article 105882"},"PeriodicalIF":2.8,"publicationDate":"2024-07-17","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141637461","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Mind the instructions: Reward cues are liked first, wanted later 注意指示:奖励提示先喜欢后想要
IF 2.8 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-07-17 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2024.105885
Nicoleta Prutean , Luc Vermeylen , Nanne Kukkonen , S. Tabitha Steendam , Joshua O. Eayrs , Ruth M. Krebs , Jan R. Wiersema , Eliana Vassena , C. Nico Boehler , Wim Notebaert

Current theories propose that mental effort is invested only when the anticipated benefits, such as rewards, outweigh the associated costs, like task difficulty. Yet, it remains unclear whether this motivational and mitigating aspect of reward processing is reflected in the evaluation of reward/difficulty cues as such, and to what extent it depends on task experience. In a pre-registered experiment (N = 84), we used the affect misattribution procedure (AMP) to gauge affective evaluations of nonword cues predicting reward and task difficulty levels. Contrary to previous studies, the AMP was administered at the outset, after cue instructions, and after the cues were used in a random dot motion (RDM) task. Compared to baseline, cues predicting a larger reward were evaluated more positively after RDM task experience, and most importantly, already after cue instructions, with no difference between the two phases. This evaluative effect manifested in increased performance after larger reward cues in the RDM task. Our results suggest that AMP effects may generally capture performance expectations which are independent of task experience. Importantly, these instructed expectations of reward and difficulty play a crucial role in the evaluation and subsequent investment of mental effort.

目前的理论认为,只有当预期收益(如奖励)大于相关成本(如任务难度)时,才会投入脑力。然而,奖励加工中的这种动机和缓解方面是否反映在对奖励/难度线索的评估中,以及在多大程度上取决于任务经验,目前仍不清楚。在一项预先注册的实验中(N = 84),我们使用了情感错误归因程序(AMP)来衡量对预测奖励和任务难度水平的非词线索的情感评价。与以往研究不同的是,AMP 在开始时、提示说明后以及在随机点运动(RDM)任务中使用提示后进行。与基线相比,在经历了 RDM 任务后,预测会有更大奖励的线索会得到更积极的评价,最重要的是,在线索指示之后就会得到更积极的评价,两个阶段之间没有差异。这种评价效应表现为在 RDM 任务中获得较大奖励线索后成绩的提高。我们的研究结果表明,AMP效应通常可以捕捉到与任务经验无关的成绩预期。重要的是,这些对奖励和难度的指示性预期在评估和随后的脑力投入中起着至关重要的作用。
{"title":"Mind the instructions: Reward cues are liked first, wanted later","authors":"Nicoleta Prutean ,&nbsp;Luc Vermeylen ,&nbsp;Nanne Kukkonen ,&nbsp;S. Tabitha Steendam ,&nbsp;Joshua O. Eayrs ,&nbsp;Ruth M. Krebs ,&nbsp;Jan R. Wiersema ,&nbsp;Eliana Vassena ,&nbsp;C. Nico Boehler ,&nbsp;Wim Notebaert","doi":"10.1016/j.cognition.2024.105885","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cognition.2024.105885","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Current theories propose that mental effort is invested only when the anticipated benefits, such as rewards, outweigh the associated costs, like task difficulty. Yet, it remains unclear whether this motivational and mitigating aspect of reward processing is reflected in the evaluation of reward/difficulty cues as such, and to what extent it depends on task experience. In a pre-registered experiment (<em>N</em> = 84), we used the affect misattribution procedure (AMP) to gauge affective evaluations of nonword cues predicting reward and task difficulty levels. Contrary to previous studies, the AMP was administered at the outset, after cue instructions, and after the cues were used in a random dot motion (RDM) task. Compared to baseline, cues predicting a larger reward were evaluated more positively after RDM task experience, and most importantly, already after cue instructions, with no difference between the two phases. This evaluative effect manifested in increased performance after larger reward cues in the RDM task. Our results suggest that AMP effects may generally capture performance expectations which are independent of task experience. Importantly, these instructed expectations of reward and difficulty play a crucial role in the evaluation and subsequent investment of mental effort.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48455,"journal":{"name":"Cognition","volume":"251 ","pages":"Article 105885"},"PeriodicalIF":2.8,"publicationDate":"2024-07-17","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141637500","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The time course of Cantonese and Hong Kong Sign Language phonological activation: An ERP study of deaf bimodal bilingual readers of Chinese 粤语和香港手语语音激活的时间过程:聋人双模双语中文阅读者的ERP研究
IF 2.8 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-07-17 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2024.105878
Philip Thierfelder

This study investigated Cantonese and Hong Kong Sign Language (HKSL) phonological activation patterns in Hong Kong deaf readers using the ERP technique. Two experiments employing the error disruption paradigm were conducted while recording participants’ EEGs. Experiment 1 focused on orthographic and speech-based phonological processing, while Experiment 2 examined sign-phonological processing. ERP analyses focused on the P200 (180–220 ms) and N400 (300–500 ms) components.

The results of Experiment 1 showed that hearing readers exhibited both orthographic and phonological effects in the P200 and N400 windows, consistent with previous studies on Chinese reading. In deaf readers, significant speech-based phonological effects were observed in the P200 window, and orthographic effects spanned both the P200 and N400 windows. Comparative analysis between the two groups revealed distinct spatial distributions for orthographic and speech-based phonological ERP effects, which may indicate the engagement of different neural networks during early processing stages.

Experiment 2 found evidence of sign-phonological activation in both the P200 and N400 windows among deaf readers, which may reflect the involvement of sign-phonological representations in early lexical access and later semantic integration. Furthermore, exploratory analysis revealed that higher reading fluency in deaf readers correlated with stronger orthographic effects in the P200 window and diminished effects in the N400 window, indicating that efficient orthographic processing during early lexical access is a distinguishing feature of proficient deaf readers.

本研究利用ERP技术调查了香港聋哑读者的粤语和香港手语语音激活模式。在记录参与者脑电图的同时,进行了两个采用错误干扰范式的实验。实验 1 侧重于正字法和基于语音的语音处理,而实验 2 则考察了手语-语音处理。实验 1 的结果显示,听力读者在 P200 和 N400 窗口同时表现出正字法和语音效应,这与之前的中文阅读研究一致。在聋人读者中,在 P200 窗口观察到了明显的基于语音的语音效应,而正字法效应则跨越了 P200 和 N400 窗口。实验 2 发现在聋人读者的 P200 和 N400 窗口中都有符号-语音激活的证据,这可能反映了符号-语音表征参与了早期的词汇访问和后期的语义整合。此外,探索性分析表明,聋人读者的阅读流畅度越高,P200 窗口中的正字法效应就越强,而 N400 窗口中的正字法效应就越弱。
{"title":"The time course of Cantonese and Hong Kong Sign Language phonological activation: An ERP study of deaf bimodal bilingual readers of Chinese","authors":"Philip Thierfelder","doi":"10.1016/j.cognition.2024.105878","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cognition.2024.105878","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>This study investigated Cantonese and Hong Kong Sign Language (HKSL) phonological activation patterns in Hong Kong deaf readers using the ERP technique. Two experiments employing the error disruption paradigm were conducted while recording participants’ EEGs. Experiment 1 focused on orthographic and speech-based phonological processing, while Experiment 2 examined sign-phonological processing. ERP analyses focused on the P200 (180–220 ms) and N400 (300–500 ms) components.</p><p>The results of Experiment 1 showed that hearing readers exhibited both orthographic and phonological effects in the P200 and N400 windows, consistent with previous studies on Chinese reading. In deaf readers, significant speech-based phonological effects were observed in the P200 window, and orthographic effects spanned both the P200 and N400 windows. Comparative analysis between the two groups revealed distinct spatial distributions for orthographic and speech-based phonological ERP effects, which may indicate the engagement of different neural networks during early processing stages.</p><p>Experiment 2 found evidence of sign-phonological activation in both the P200 and N400 windows among deaf readers, which may reflect the involvement of sign-phonological representations in early lexical access and later semantic integration. Furthermore, exploratory analysis revealed that higher reading fluency in deaf readers correlated with stronger orthographic effects in the P200 window and diminished effects in the N400 window, indicating that efficient orthographic processing during early lexical access is a distinguishing feature of proficient deaf readers.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48455,"journal":{"name":"Cognition","volume":"251 ","pages":"Article 105878"},"PeriodicalIF":2.8,"publicationDate":"2024-07-17","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141637501","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
High or low expectations: Expected intensity of action outcome is embedded in action kinetics 期望值高或低:行动结果的预期强度包含在行动动力学中
IF 2.8 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-07-16 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2024.105887
Batel Buaron , Daniel Reznik , Roy Mukamel

Goal-directed actions are performed in order to attain certain sensory consequences in the world. However, expected attributes of these consequences can affect the kinetics of the action. In a set of three studies (n = 120), we examined how expected attributes of stimulus outcome (intensity) shape the kinetics of the triggering action (applied force), even when the action kinetic and attribute are independent. We show that during action execution (button presses), the expected intensity of sensory outcome affects the applied force of the stimulus-producing action in an inverse fashion. Thus, participants applied more force when the expected intensity of the outcome was low (vs. high intensity outcome). In the absence of expectations or when actions were performed in response to the sensory event, no intensity-dependent force modulations were found. Thus, expectations of stimulus intensity and causality play an important role in shaping action kinetics. Finally, we examined the relationship between kinetics and perception and found no influence of applied force level on perceptual detection of low intensity (near-threshold) outcome stimuli, suggesting no causal link between the two. Taken together, our results demonstrate that action kinetics are embedded with high-level context such as the expectation of consequence intensity and the causal relationship with environmental cues.

目标导向的行动是为了在世界上获得某些感官结果。然而,这些结果的预期属性会影响行动的动力。在一组三项研究(n = 120)中,我们考察了刺激结果(强度)的预期属性如何影响触发动作(施加的力)的动力学,即使动作动力学和属性是独立的。我们的研究表明,在执行动作(按下按钮)的过程中,感官结果的预期强度会反向影响刺激动作的施力。因此,当预期结果强度较低时(相对于高强度结果),参与者会施加更大的力。在没有预期或对感觉事件做出反应的情况下,没有发现与强度相关的力量调节。因此,对刺激强度的预期和因果关系在形成动作动力学方面起着重要作用。最后,我们研究了动力学与感知之间的关系,发现施力水平对低强度(接近阈值)结果刺激的感知检测没有影响,这表明两者之间没有因果联系。综上所述,我们的研究结果表明,动作动量与高层次的情境(如对结果强度的预期)以及与环境线索之间的因果关系息息相关。
{"title":"High or low expectations: Expected intensity of action outcome is embedded in action kinetics","authors":"Batel Buaron ,&nbsp;Daniel Reznik ,&nbsp;Roy Mukamel","doi":"10.1016/j.cognition.2024.105887","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cognition.2024.105887","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Goal-directed actions are performed in order to attain certain sensory consequences in the world. However, expected attributes of these consequences can affect the kinetics of the action. In a set of three studies (<em>n</em> = 120), we examined how expected attributes of stimulus outcome (intensity) shape the kinetics of the triggering action (applied force), even when the action kinetic and attribute are independent. We show that during action execution (button presses), the expected intensity of sensory outcome affects the applied force of the stimulus-producing action in an inverse fashion. Thus, participants applied more force when the expected intensity of the outcome was low (vs. high intensity outcome). In the absence of expectations or when actions were performed in response to the sensory event, no intensity-dependent force modulations were found. Thus, expectations of stimulus intensity and causality play an important role in shaping action kinetics. Finally, we examined the relationship between kinetics and perception and found no influence of applied force level on perceptual detection of low intensity (near-threshold) outcome stimuli, suggesting no causal link between the two. Taken together, our results demonstrate that action kinetics are embedded with high-level context such as the expectation of consequence intensity and the causal relationship with environmental cues.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48455,"journal":{"name":"Cognition","volume":"251 ","pages":"Article 105887"},"PeriodicalIF":2.8,"publicationDate":"2024-07-16","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0010027724001732/pdfft?md5=654bdad8283afc095ff3b02e6cbe66cb&pid=1-s2.0-S0010027724001732-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141622934","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Cognition
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1